EdPsy2009_Seifert_Sutton1.pdf

Educational Psychology

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Educational PsychologySecond Edition

Kelvin Seifert and Rosemary Sutton

Copyright © 2009 Kelvin Seifert

For any questions about this text, please email: [email protected]

Editor-In-Chief: Kelvin Seifert

Associate Editor: Marisa Drexel

Editorial Assistant: Jackie Sharman

Proofreader: Rachel Pugliese

The Global Text Project is funded by the Jacobs Foundation, Zurich, Switzerland

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Educational Psychology 2 A Global Text

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

About the authors and reviewerAuthor, Kelvin Seifert

Kelvin Seifert is professor of educational psychology at the University of Manitoba, Winnipeg, Canada. He

earned a BA from Swarthmore College in 1967 and a PhD from the University of Michigan in 1973, in a combined

program from the School of Education and the Department of Psychology. His research interests include the

personal identity development of teachers, the impact of peers in 0pre-service teacher education, and the

development of effective strategies of blended learning. He is the author of four university textbooks (with

Houghton Mifflin, in traditional print format) about educational psychology, child and adolescent development,

and lifespan human development. He is also the editor of the online Canadian Journal of Educational

Administration and Policy. Recent publications include “Student cohorts: Support groups or intellectual

communities?” (Teachers College Record) and “Learning about peers: A missed opportunity for educational

psychology” (The Clearinghouse). His professional service includes serving as chair of the Department of

Educational Administration, Foundations, and Psychology at the University of Manitoba, and serving as president

of the American Educational Research Association Special Interest Group on Teaching Educational Psychology.

During his career of 35 years, he has taught introductory educational psychology over 75 times.

Author, Rosemary Sutton

After four years of teaching high school mathematics in New Zealand, Dr Rosemary Sutton attended graduate

school and earned her MS in Educational Psychology from the University of Illinois and her PhD from Pennsylvania

State University in Human Development. She joined the Cleveland State University faculty in Cleveland, Ohio in

1983 and since that time has taught pre-service and in service undergraduates and graduate students educational

psychology and educational technology. She has received several University awards for her teaching and has

conducted numerous workshops for teachers in North East Ohio.

Dr Sutton has published a variety research articles on teacher development as well as equity issues in

mathematics, technology, and assessment. Her recent research interests have focused in two areas: teaching

educational psychology and teachers' emotions. Recent publications can be found in Social Psychology of

Education, Educational Psychology Review, Journal of Teacher Education, and an edited volume, Emotions and

Education.

Since 2004, Dr Sutton has been working as an Administrator, first as the Director of Assessment for the

University. This position involved coordinating the student learning assessment for all graduate, undergraduate,

and student support programs. In August 2007, Dr Sutton was appointed Vice Provost for Undergraduate Studies

and is now responsible for overseeing offices and functions from academic and student service areas in order to

create a campus culture that coordinates student services with the academic mission of the University.

Reviewer, Sandra Deemer

Sandra Deemer is professor of educational foundations at Millersville University, in Millersville, PA. She is also

the editor of the online journal called “Teaching Educational Psychology,” and has contributed to the development

of the Special Interest Group on Teaching Educational Psychology (TEP SIG) sponsored by the American

Educational Research Association. She teaches courses in educational psychology and educational research; her

research interests focus on how motivational theory can be used to create learning-focused classrooms.

Educational Psychology 3 A Global Text

Table of ContentsPreface……………………………………………………………………………………………………………………………………………7

1. The changing teaching profession and you…………………………………………………………………..8The joys of teaching………………………………………………………………………………………………………………………… 8

Are there also challenges to teaching?………………………………………………………………………………………………10Teaching is different from in the past……………………………………………………………………………………………… 10

How educational psychology can help………………………………………………………………………………………………16

2. The learning process ……………………………………………………………………………………………….20Teachers’ perspectives on learning…………………………………………………………………………………………………. 20Major theories and models of learning……………………………………………………………………………………………. 23

3. Student development………………………………………………………………………………………………. 41Why development matters…………………………………………………………………………………………………………….. 42

Physical development during the school years…………………………………………………………………………………. 43Cognitive development: the theory of Jean Piaget……………………………………………………………………………. 46

Social development: relationships,personal motives, and morality …………………………………………………….50Moral development: forming a sense of rights and responsibilities……………………………………………………. 56

Understanding “the typical student” versus understanding students………………………………………………….. 61

4. Student diversity……………………………………………………………………………………………………..66Individual styles of learning and thinking……………………………………………………………………………………….. 67Multiple intelligences……………………………………………………………………………………………………………………. 68

Gifted and talented students………………………………………………………………………………………………………….. 70Gender differences in the classroom……………………………………………………………………………………………….. 72

Differences in cultural expectations and styles…………………………………………………………………………………. 75Accommodating diversity in practice……………………………………………………………………………………………… 80

5. Students with special educational needs…………………………………………………………………… 85Three people on the margins…………………………………………………………………………………………………………..85

Growing support for people with disabilities: legislation and its effects……………………………………………… 86Responsibilities of teachers for students with disabilities…………………………………………………………………..87

Categories of disabilities—and their ambiguities………………………………………………………………………………. 91Learning disabilities……………………………………………………………………………………………………………………….91

Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder…………………………………………………………………………………………… 94Intellectual disabilities………………………………………………………………………………………………………………….. 96

Behavioral disorders………………………………………………………………………………………………………………………99Physical disabilities and sensory impairments…………………………………………………………………………………101

The value of including students with special needs…………………………………………………………………………. 104

6. Student motivation……………………………………………………………………………………………….. 109Motives as behavior………………………………………………………………………………………………………………………110Motives as goals…………………………………………………………………………………………………………………………… 113

Motives as interests……………………………………………………………………………………………………………………… 116Motives related to attributions……………………………………………………………………………………………………….118

Motivation as self-efficacy……………………………………………………………………………………………………………. 120Motivation as self-determination………………………………………………………………………………………………….. 125

Expectancy x value: effects on students’ motivation…………………………………………………………………………130TARGET: a model for integrating ideas about motivation…………………………………………………………………131

7. Classroom management and the learning environment……………………………………………. 138Why classroom management matters……………………………………………………………………………………………. 139

4

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Preventing management problems by focusing students on learning…………………………………………………140Responding to student misbehavior……………………………………………………………………………………………….150

Keeping management issues in perspective……………………………………………………………………………………. 155

8. The nature of classroom communication………………………………………………………………….159Communication in classrooms vs communication elsewhere…………………………………………………………… 159Effective verbal communication……………………………………………………………………………………………………. 162

Effective nonverbal communication……………………………………………………………………………………………….163Structures of participation: effects on communication……………………………………………………………………..166

Communication styles in the classroom………………………………………………………………………………………….169Using classroom talk to stimulate students’ thinking………………………………………………………………………. 172

The bottom line: messages sent, messages reconstructed………………………………………………………………… 176

9. Facilitating complex thinking………………………………………………………………………………….183Forms of thinking associated with classroom learning……………………………………………………………………..184Critical thinking………………………………………………………………………………………………………………………….. 185

Creative thinking …………………………………………………………………………………………………………………………186Problem-solving………………………………………………………………………………………………………………………….. 187

Broad instructional strategies that stimulate complex thinking………………………………………………………… 191Teacher-directed instruction………………………………………………………………………………………………………… 193

Student-centered models of learning……………………………………………………………………………………………. 200Inquiry learning………………………………………………………………………………………………………………………….. 201

Cooperative learning…………………………………………………………………………………………………………………… 202Examples of cooperative and collaborative learning……………………………………………………………………….. 203

Instructional strategies: an abundance of choices……………………………………………………………………………205

10. Planning instruction……………………………………………………………………………………………. 210Selecting general learning goals……………………………………………………………………………………………………. 210Formulating learning objectives……………………………………………………………………………………………………. 215

Students as a source of instructional goals…………………………………………………………………………………….. 223Enhancing student learning through a variety of resources………………………………………………………………226

Creating bridges among curriculum goals and students’ prior experiences……………………………………….. 229Planning for instruction as well as for learning………………………………………………………………………………. 235

11. Teacher-made assessment strategies…………………………………………………………………….. 240Basic concepts…………………………………………………………………………………………………………………………….. 241

Assessment for learning: an overview of the process………………………………………………………………………..241Selecting appropriate assessment techniques I: high quality assessments…………………………………………243

Reliability ………………………………………………………………………………………………………………………………….. 245Absence of bias ………………………………………………………………………………………………………………………….. 246

Selecting appropriate assessment techniques II: types of teacher-made assessments…………………………246Selected response items………………………………………………………………………………………………………………..249

Constructed response items ………………………………………………………………………………………………………… 254Portfolios…………………………………………………………………………………………………………………………………….264

Assessment that enhances motivation and student confidence………………………………………………………… 267Teachers’ purposes and beliefs …………………………………………………………………………………………………….. 267

Choosing assessments ………………………………………………………………………………………………………………… 268Providing feedback …………………………………………………………………………………………………………………….. 268

Self and peer assessment ……………………………………………………………………………………………………………..269Adjusting instruction based on assessment…………………………………………………………………………………….270

Communication with parents and guardians…………………………………………………………………………………..270

Educational Psychology 5 A Global Text

Action research: studying yourself and your students……………………………………………………………………… 271

Grading and reporting ………………………………………………………………………………………………………………… 272

12. Standardized and other formal assessments……………………………………………………………277Basic concepts…………………………………………………………………………………………………………………………….. 277High-stakes testing by states…………………………………………………………………………………………………………283

International testing……………………………………………………………………………………………………………………. 291International comparisons ………………………………………………………………………………………………………….. 292

Understanding test results…………………………………………………………………………………………………………… 292Issues with standardized tests ………………………………………………………………………………………………………298

Appendix A: Preparing for licensure………………………………………………………………………….. 304Appendix B: Deciding for yourself about the research…………………………………………………..319Appendix C: The reflective practitioner………………………………………………………………………. 351

Types of resources for professional development and learning………………………………………………………… 352

Reading and understanding professional articles ……………………………………………………………………………355Action research: hearing from teachers about improving practice……………………………………………………. 366

The challenges of action research………………………………………………………………………………………………….. 371Benefiting from all kinds of research…………………………………………………………………………………………….. 373

6

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

PrefaceDr. Kelvin Seifert: Why I wanted this book to be part of the Global Textbook Project

I have taught educational psychology to future

teachers for nearly 35 years, during which I used one or

another of the major commercial textbooks written for

this subject. In general I found all of the books well-

written and thorough. But I also found problems:

(1) Though they differed in details, the major

textbooks were surprisingly similar in overall coverage.

This fact, coupled with their large overall size, made it

hard to tailor any of the books to the particular interests

or needs of individuals or groups of students. Too often,

buying a textbook was like having to buy a huge Sunday

newspaper when all you really want is to read one of its

sections. In a similar way, commercial educational

psychology textbooks usually told you more than you

ever needed or wanted to know about the subject. As a

format, the textbook did not allow for individualization.

(2) Educational psychology textbooks were always

expensive, and over the years their costs rose faster than inflation, especially in the United States, where most of the

books have been produced. Currently every major text about educational psychology sells for more than USD 100.

At best this cost is a stress on students' budgets. At worst it puts educational psychology textbooks beyond the reach

of many. The problem of the cost is even more obvious when put in worldwide perspective; in some countries the

cost of one textbook is roughly equivalent to the average annual income of its citizens.

(3) In the competition to sell copies of educational psychology textbooks, authors and publishers have gradually

added features that raise the cost of books without evidence of adding educational value. Educational psychology

publishers in particular have increased the number of illustrations and photographs, switched to full-color editions,

increased the complexity and number of study guides and ancillary publications, and created proprietary websites

usable fully only by adopters of their particular books. These features have sometimes been attractive. My teaching

experience suggests, however, that they also distract students from learning key ideas about educational psychology

about as often as they help students to learn.

By publishing this textbook online with the Global Textbook Project, I have taken a step toward resolving these

problems. Instructors and students can access as much or as little of the textbook as they really need and find

useful. The cost of their doing is minimal. Pedagogical features are available, but are kept to a minimum and

rendered in formats that can be accessed freely and easily by anyone connected to the Internet. In the future,

revisions to the book will be relatively easy and prompt to make. These, I believe, are desirable outcomes for

everyone! –Kelvin Seifert

Educational Psychology 7 A Global Text

Dr. Kelvin Seifert

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

1. The changing teaching profession and you

A teacher named Ashley reflects: She looked around the classroom, enjoying a blessed moment of

quiet after the students left at the end of the day. “Ashley, the teacher, that’s me”, she said proudly to

the empty room. “But why am I doing this?” she asked herself quietly—and realized she wasn’t

always sure of the answer. But then she remembered one reason: she was teaching for Nadia, who

sat at the table to the left, always smiled so well and always (well, usually) tried hard. And another

reason: she was teaching for Lincoln, tired old Lincoln, who needed her help more than he realized.

She remembered twenty other reasons—twenty other students. And one last reason: she was also

teaching for herself, challenging herself to see if she really could keep up with twenty-two young

people at once, and really accomplish something worthwhile with them. She was teaching so she

could keep growing as a person, keep connecting with others, keep learning new ideas. That’s why

she was teaching.

The joys of teaching

Why be a teacher? The short answer is easy:

• to witness the diversity of growth in young people, and their joy in learning

• to encourage lifelong learning—both for yourself and for others

• to experience the challenge of devising and doing interesting, exciting activities for the young

There is, of course, more than this to be said about the value of teaching. Consider, for instance, the “young

people” referred to above. In one class they could be six years old; in another they could be sixteen, or even older.

They could be rich, poor, or somewhere in between. They could come from any ethnic background. Their first

language could be English, or something else. There are all sorts of possibilities. But whoever the particular

students are, they will have potential as human beings: talents and personal qualities—possibly not yet realized—

that can contribute to society, whether as leaders, experts, or supporters of others. A teacher's job—in fact a

teacher's privilege—is to help particular “young people” to realize their potential.

Another teacher reflects: Nathan paused for a deep breath before speaking to me. “It’s not like I

expected it to be,” he said. “I’ve got five kids who speak English as a second language. I didn’t expect

that. I’ve got two, maybe three, with reading disabilities, and one of them has a part-time aide. I’ve

had to learn more about using computers than I ever expected—they’re a lot of curriculum materials

online now, and the computers help the kids that need more practice or who finish activities early.

I’m doing more screening and testing of kids than I expected, and it all takes time away from

teaching.

Educational Psychology 8 A Global Text

1. The changing teaching profession and you

“But it’s not all surprises. I expected to be able to ‘light a fire’ under kids about learning to read. And

that has actually happened, at least sometimes with some children!”

As a teacher, you will be able to do this by laying groundwork for lifelong learning. You will not teach any one

student forever, of course, but you will often work with them long enough to convey a crucial message: that there is

much in life to learn—more in fact than any one teacher or school can provide in a lifetime. The knowledge may be

about science, math, or learning to read; the skills may be sports, music, or art—anything. Whatever you teach, its

immensity can be a source of curiosity, wonder and excitement. It can be a reason to be optimistic about life in

general and about your students in particular. Learning, when properly understood, is never-ending, even though it

often focuses on short-term, immediate concerns. As a teacher, you will have an advantage not shared by every

member of society, namely the excuse not only to teach valuable knowledge and skills, but to point students beyond

what they will be able to learn from you. As an old limerick put it (before the days of gender-balanced language),

“The world is full of such a plenty of things, I’m sure we should all be as happy as kings.”

Jennifer Fuller, a third teacher reflects: “OK”, suddenly getting businesslike in her tone. “Here’s my

typical day teaching tenth grade: I get up at 6:30, have a quick breakfast, get to school by 7:45 if the

traffic’s not bad. Then I check my email—usually there’s a little stuff from the principal or some other

administrator, maybe one or two from parents concerned because their child is doing poorly in one

of my classes, maybe one or two from students—“I’m going to be sick today, Ms Fuller!”—that sort of

thing. Now it’s 8:15 and I have two hours before my first class—this term I teach only biology, and I

only teach periods 2, 3, and 5. Maybe I have marking to do before class, or maybe I have to get a lab

demonstration ready. Or maybe we all have to troupe down to the library for a staff meeting

(groan…). Whatever I don’t finish in the morning, I have to finish after school. But that’s also when I

meet with the Ecology Club (I’m the faculty advisor), so I might have to finish stuff in the evening. I

try not to do it then, but a lot of times I have to. But I always quit by 9:00—that’s always when I

watch TV for an hour, or just “vegetate ” with a book.”

Whatever you teach, you will be able to feel the satisfaction of designing and orchestrating complex activities

that communicate new ideas and skills effectively. The challenge is attractive to many teachers, because that is

where they exercise judgment and “artistry” the most freely and frequently. Your students will depend on your skill

at planning and managing, though sometimes without realizing how much they do so. Teachers will need you to

know how to explain ideas clearly, to present new materials in a sensible sequence and at an appropriate pace, to

point out connections between their new learning and their prior experiences. Although these skills really take a

lifetime to master, they can be practiced successfully even by beginning teachers, and they do improve steadily with

continued teaching over time. Right from the start, though, skill at design and communication of curriculum is one

of the major “perks” of the job.

The very complexity of classroom life virtually guarantees that teaching never needs to get boring. Something

new and exciting is bound to occur just when you least expect it. A student shows an insight that you never

expected to see—or fails to show one that you were sure he had. An activity goes better than expected—or worse, or

merely differently. You understand for the first time why a particular student behaves as she does, and begin

thinking of how to respond to the student's behavior more helpfully in the future. After teaching a particular

9

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

learning objective several times, you realize that you understand it differently than the first time you taught it. And

so on. The job never stays the same; it evolves continually. As long as you keep teaching, you will have a job with

novelty.

Are there also challenges to teaching?

Here, too, the simple answer is “yes”. Every joy of teaching has a possible frustration related to it. You may wish

to make a positive difference in students' lives, but you may also have trouble reaching individuals. A student seems

not to learn much, or to be unmotivated, or unfriendly, or whatever. And some teaching problems can be subtle:

when you call attention to the wonderful immensity of an area of knowledge, you might accidentally discourage a

student by implying that the student can never learn “enough”. The complexity of designing and implementing

instruction can sometimes seem overwhelming, instead of satisfying. Unexpected events in your classroom can

become chaos rather than an attractive novelty. To paraphrase a popular self-help book, sometimes “bad things

happen to good teachers” (Kushner, 1983). But as in the rest of life, the “bad things” of teaching do not negate the

value of the good. If anything, the undesired events make the good, desired ones even more satisfying, and render

the work of teaching all the more valuable. As you will see throughout this book, there are resources for maximizing

the good, the valuable, and the satisfying. You can bring these resources to your work, along with your growing

professional knowledge and a healthy dose of common sense. In this sense you will not need to “go it alone” in

learning to teach well. You will, however, be personally responsible for becoming and remaining the best teacher

that you can possibly be; the only person who can make that happen will be you. Many of the resources for making

this happen are described in this book in the chapters ahead.

Teaching is different from in the past

In the past decade or two teaching has changed significantly, so much in fact that schools may not be what some

of us remember from our own childhood. Changes have affected both the opportunities and the challenges of

teaching, as well as the attitudes, knowledge, and skills needed to prepare for a teaching career. The changes have

influenced much of the content of this book.

To see what we mean, look briefly at four new trends in education, at how they have changed what teachers do,

and at how you will therefore need to prepare to teach:

• increased diversity: there are more differences among students than there used to be. Diversity has

made teaching more fulfilling as a career, but also made more challenging in certain respects.

• * increased instructional technology: classrooms, schools, and students use computers more often

today than in the past for research, writing, communicating, and keeping records. Technology has created

new ways for students to learn (for example, this textbook would not be possible without Internet

technology!). It has also altered how teachers can teach most effectively, and even raised issues about what

constitutes “true” teaching and learning.

• greater accountability in education: both the public and educators themselves pay more attention

than in the past to how to assess (or provide evidence for) learning and good quality teaching. The attention

has increased the importance of education to the public (a good thing) and improved education for some

students. But it has also created new constraints on what teachers teach and what students learn.

Educational Psychology 10 A Global Text

1. The changing teaching profession and you

• increased professionalism of teachers: Now more than ever, teachers are able to assess the quality of

their own work as well as that of colleagues, and to take steps to improve it when necessary.

Professionalism improves teaching, but by creating higher standards of practice it also creates greater

worries about whether particular teachers and schools are “good enough”.

How do these changes show up in the daily life of classrooms? The answer depends partly on where you teach;

circumstances differ among schools, cities, and even whole societies. Some clues about the effects of the trends on

classroom life can be found, however, by considering one particular case—the changes happening in North America.

New trend #1: diversity in students

Students have, of course, always been diverse. Whether in the past or in the present day, students learn at

unique paces, show unique personalities, and learn in their own ways. In recent decades, though, the forms and

extent of diversity have increased. Now more than ever, teachers are likely to serve students from diverse language

backgrounds, to serve more individuals with special educational needs, and to teach students either younger and

older than in the past.

Language diversity

Take the case of language diversity. In the United States, about 40 million people, or 14 per cent of the

population are Hispanic. About 20 per cent of these speak primarily Spanish, and approximately another 50 per

cent speak only limited English (United States Census Bureau, 2005). The educators responsible for the children in

this group need to accommodate instruction to these students somehow. Part of the solution, of course, is to

arrange specialized second-language teachers and classes. But adjustment must also happen in “regular”

classrooms of various grade levels and subjects. Classroom teachers must learn to communicate with students

whose English language background is limited, at the same time that the students themselves are learning to use

English more fluently (Pitt, 2005). Since relatively few teachers are Hispanic or speak fluent Spanish, the

adjustments can sometimes be a challenge. Teachers must plan lessons and tasks that students actually understand.

At the same time teachers must also keep track of the major learning goals of the curriculum. In Chapter 4

(“Student Diversity”) and Chapter 10 (“Planning Instruction”), some strategies for doing so are described. As you

gain experience teaching, you will no doubt find additional strategies and resources (Gebhard, 2006), especially if

second-language learners become an important part of your classes.

Diversity of special educational needs

Another factor making classroom increasingly diverse has been the inclusion of students with disabilities into

classrooms with non-disabled peers. In the United States the trend began in the 1970s, but accelerated with the

passage of the Individuals with Disabilities Education Act in 1975, and again when the Act was amended in 2004

(United States Government Printing Office, 2005). In Canada similar legislation was passed in individual provinces

during the same general time period. The laws guarantee free, appropriate education for children with disabilities

of any kind—whether the impairment is physical, cognitive, emotional, or behavioral. The laws also recognize that

such students need special supports in order to learn or function effectively in a classroom with non-disabled peers,

so they provide for special services (for example, teaching assistants) and procedures for making individualized

educational plans for students with disabilities.

11

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

As a result of these changes, most American and Canadian teachers are likely to have at least a few students with

special educational needs, even if they are not trained as special education teachers or have had no prior personal

experience with people with disabilities. Classroom teachers are also likely to work as part of a professional team

focused on helping these students to learn as well as possible and to participate in the life of the school. The trend

toward inclusion is definitely new compared to circumstances just a generation or two ago. It raises new challenges

about planning instruction (such as how is a teacher to find time to plan for individuals?), and philosophical

questions about the very nature of education (such as what in the curriculum is truly important to learn?). These

questions will come up again in Chapter 5, where we discuss teaching students with special educational needs.

Lifelong learning

The diversity of modern classrooms is not limited to language or disabilities. Another recent change has been

the broadening simply of the age range of individuals who count as “students”. In many nations of the world, half or

most of all three- and four-year-olds attend some form of educational program, either part-time preschool or full-

time child care (National Institute for Early Education Research, 2006). In North America some public school

divisions have moved toward including nursery or preschool programs as a newer “grade level” preceding

kindergarten. Others have expanded the hours of kindergarten (itself considered a “new” program early in the 20th

century) to span a full-day program.

The obvious differences in maturity between preschoolers and older children lead most teachers of the very

young to use flexible, open-ended plans and teaching strategies, and to develop more personal or family-like

relationships with their young “students” than typical with older students (Bredekamp & Copple, 1997). Just as

important, though, are the educational and philosophical issues that early childhood education has brought to

public attention. Some educational critics ask whether preschool and day care programs risk becoming

inappropriate substitutes for families. Other educators suggest, in contrast, that teachers of older students can learn

from the flexibility and open-ended approach common in early childhood education. For teachers of any grade

level, it is a debate that cannot be avoided completely or permanently. In this book, it reappears in Chapter 3, where

I discuss students’ development—their major long-term, changes in skills, knowledge, and attitudes.

The other end of the age spectrum has also expanded. Many individuals take courses well into adulthood even if

they do not attend formal university or college. Adult education, as it is sometimes called, often takes place in

workplaces, but it often also happens in public high schools or at local community colleges or universities. Some

adult students may be completing high school credentials that they missed earlier in their lives, but often the

students have other purposes that are even more focused, such as learning a trade-related skill. The teachers of

adult students have to adjust their instructional strategies and relationships with students so as to challenge and

respect their special strengths and constraints as adults (Bash, 2005). The students’ maturity often means that they

have had life experiences that enhance and motivate their learning. But it may also mean that they have significant

personal responsibilities—such as parenting or a full-time job—which compete for study time, and that make them

impatient with teaching that is irrelevant to their personal goals or needs. These advantages and constraints also

occur to a lesser extent among “regular” high school students. Even secondary school teachers must ask, how they

can make sure that instruction does not waste students’ time, and how they can make it truly efficient, effective, and

valuable. Elsewhere in this book (especially in Chapters 9 through 11, about assessment and instruction), we discuss

these questions from a number of perspectives.

Educational Psychology 12 A Global Text

1. The changing teaching profession and you

New trend #2: using technology to support learning

For most teachers, “technology” means using computers and the Internet as resources for teaching and learning.

These tools have greatly increased the amount and range of information available to students, even if their benefits

have sometimes been exaggerated in media reports (Cuban, 2001). With the Internet, it is now relatively easy to

access up-to-date information on practically any subject imaginable, often with pictures, video clips, and audio to

accompany them. It would seem not only that the Internet and its associated technologies have the potential to

transform traditional school-based learning, but also that they have in fact begun to do so.

For a variety of reasons, however, technology has not always been integrated into teachers’ practices very

thoroughly (Haertel & Means, 2003). One reason is practical: in many societies and regions, classrooms contain

only one or two computers at most, and many schools have at best only limited access to the Internet. Waiting for a

turn on the computer or arranging to visit a computer lab or school library limits how much students use the

Internet, no matter how valuable the Internet may be. In such cases, furthermore, computers tend to function in

relatively traditional ways that do not take full advantage of the Internet: as a word processor (a “fancy typewriter”),

for example, or as a reference book similar to an encyclopedia.

Even so, single-computer classrooms create new possibilities and challenges for teachers. A single computer can

be used, for example, to present upcoming assignments or supplementary material to students, either one at a time

or small groups. In functioning in this way, the computer gives students more flexibility about when to finish old

tasks or to begin new ones. A single computer can also enrich the learning of individual students with special

interests or motivation. And it can provide additional review to students who need extra help. These changes are

not dramatic, but they lead to important revisions in teachers’ roles: they move teachers away from simply

delivering information to students, and toward facilitating students’ own constructions of knowledge.

A shift from “full-frontal teaching” to “guide on the side” becomes easier as the amount and use of computer and

Internet technologies increases. If a school (or better yet, a classroom) has numerous computers with full Internet

access, then students’ can in principle direct their own learning more independently than if computers are scarce

commodities. With ample technology available, teachers can focus much more on helping individuals in developing

and carrying out learning plans, as well as on assisting individuals with special learning problems. In these ways a

strong shift to computers and the Internet can change a teacher’s role significantly, and make the teacher more

effective.

But technology also brings some challenges, or even creates problems. It costs money to equip classrooms and

schools fully: often that money is scarce, and may therefore mean depriving students of other valuable resources,

like additional staff or additional books and supplies. Other challenges are less tangible. In using the Internet, for

example, students need help in sorting out trustworthy information or websites from the “fluff”, websites that are

unreliable or even damaging (Seiter, 2005). Providing this help can sometimes be challenging even for experienced

teachers. And some educational activities simply do not lend themselves to computerized learning—sports, for

example, driver education, or choral practice. As a new teacher, therefore, you will need not only to assess what

technologies are possible in your particular classroom, but also what will actually be assisted by new technologies.

Then be prepared for your decisions to affect how you teach—the ways you work with students.

13

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

New trend #3: accountability in education

In recent years, the public and its leaders have increasingly expected teachers and students to be accountable for

their work, meaning that schools and teachers are held responsible for implementing particular curricula and goals,

and that students are held responsible for learning particular knowledge. The trend toward accountability has

increased the legal requirements for becoming and (sometimes) remaining certified as a teacher. In the United

States in particular, preservice teachers need more subject-area and education-related courses than in the past.

They must also spend more time practice teaching than in the past, and they must pass one or more examinations

of knowledge of subject matter and teaching strategies. The specifics of these requirements vary among regions, but

the general trend—toward more numerous and “higher” levels of requirements—has occurred broadly throughout

the English-speaking world. The changes obviously affect individuals’ experiences of becoming a teacher—

especially the speed and cost of doing so.

Public accountability has led to increased use of high-stakes testing, which are tests taken by all students in a

district or region that have important consequences for students' further education (Fuhrman & Elmore, 2004).

High-stakes tests may influence grades that students receive in courses or determine whether students graduate or

continue to the next level of schooling. The tests are often a mixture of essay and structured-response questions

(such as multiple-choice items), and raise important issues about what teachers should teach, as well as how (and

whether) teachers should help students to pass the examinations. It also raises issues about whether high-stakes

testing is fair to all students and consistent with other ideals of public education, such as giving students the best

possible start in life instead of disqualifying them from educational opportunities. Furthermore, since the results of

high-stakes tests are sometimes also used to evaluate the performance of teachers, schools, or school districts,

insuring students’ success on them becomes an obvious concern for teachers—one that affects instructional

decisions on a daily basis. For this reason we discuss the purpose, nature, and effects of high-stakes tests in detail in

Chapter 12.

New trend #4: increased professionalism of teachers

Whatever your reactions to the first three trends, it is important to realize that they have contributed to a fourth

trend, an increase in professionalism of teachers. By most definitions, an occupation (like medicine or law—or in

this case teaching) is a profession if its members take personal responsibility for the quality of their work, hold each

other accountable for its quality, and recognize and require special training in order to practice it.

By this definition, teaching has definitely become more professional than in the past (Cochran-Smith & Fries,

2005). Increased expectations of achievement by students mean that teachers have increased responsibility not

only for their students’ academic success, but also for their own development as teachers. Becoming a new teacher

now requires more specialized work than in the past, as reflected in the increased requirements for certification and

licensing in many societies and regions. The increased requirements are partly a response to the complexities

created by the increasing diversity of students and increasing use of technology in classrooms.

Greater professionalism has also been encouraged by initiatives from educators themselves to study and

improve their own practice. One way to do so, for example, is through action research (sometimes also called

teacher research), a form of investigation carried out by teachers about their own students or their own teaching.

Educational Psychology 14 A Global Text

1. The changing teaching profession and you

Action research studies lead to concrete decisions that improve teaching and learning in particular educational

contexts (Mertler, 2006; Stringer, 2004). The studies can take many forms, but here are a few brief examples:

• How precisely do individual children learn to read? In an action research study, the teacher might observe

and track one child’s reading progress carefully for an extended time. From the observations she can get

clues about how to help not only that particular child to read better, but also other children in her class or

even in colleagues’ classes.

• Does it really matter if a high school social studies teacher uses more, rather than fewer, open-ended

questions? As an action of research study, the teacher might videotape his own lessons, and systematically

compare students’ responses to his open-ended questions compared to their responses to more closed

questions (the ones with more fixed answers). The analysis might suggest when and how much it is indeed

desirable to use open-ended questions.

• Can an art teacher actually entice students to take more creative risks with their drawings? As an action

research study, the teacher might examine the students’ drawings carefully for signs of visual novelty and

innovation, and then see if the signs increase if she encourages novelty and innovation explicitly.

Table 1: Examples of action research project

Steps in action research

Project

Example 1: students’ use of

the Internet

Example 2: a teacher’s

helpfulness to ESL students

Purpose of the research (as

expressed by the teacher doing the

research)

“In doing assignments, how

successful are my students at finding

high-quality, relevant information?”

“Am I responding to my ESL

students as fully and helpfully as to

my English-speaking students, and

why or why not?”

Who is doing the study? Classroom teacher (elementary

level) and school computer specialist

teacher

Classroom teacher (senior high

level)—studying self;

Possibly collaborating with other

teachers or with ESL specialist.

How information is gathered and

recorded

Assessing students’ assignments;

Observing students while they

search the Internet.

Interviewing students about their

search experiences

Videotaping of self interacting

during class discussions;

Journal diary by teacher of

experiences with ESL vs other

students;

Interviews with teacher’s ESL

students

How information is analyzed Look for obstacles and “search

tips” expressed by several students;

Look for differences in type and

amount of interactions with ESL vs.

15

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Look for common strengths and

problems with research cited on

assignments.

other students;

Look for patterns in the

differences;

Try altering the patterns of

interaction and observe the result.

How information is reported and

communicated

Write a brief report of results for

fellow staff;

Give a brief oral report to fellow

staff about results

Write a summary of the results in

teacher’s journal diary;

Share results with fellow staff;

Share results with teacher’s

students.

Two other, more complete examples of action research are summarized in Table 1. Although these examples, like

many action research studies, resemble “especially good teaching practice”, they are planned more thoughtfully

than usual, carried out and recorded more systematically, and shared with fellow teachers more thoroughly and

openly. As such, they yield special benefits to teachers as professionals, though they also take special time and

effort. For now, the important point is that use of action research simultaneously reflects the increasing

professionalism of teachers, but at the same time creates higher standards for teachers when they teach.

How educational psychology can help

All things considered, then, times have changed for teachers. But teaching remains an attractive, satisfying, and

worthwhile profession. The recent trends mean simply that you need to prepare for teaching differently than you

might have in the past, and perhaps differently than your own school teachers did a generation ago. Fortunately,

there are ways to do this. Many current programs in teacher education provide a balance of experiences in tune

with current and emerging needs of teachers. They offer more time for practice teaching in schools, for example,

and teacher education instructors often make deliberate efforts to connect the concepts and ideas of education and

psychology to current best practices of education. These and other features of contemporary teacher education will

make it easier for you to become the kind of teacher that you not only want to be, but also will need to be.

This book—about educational psychology and its relation to teaching and learning—can be one of your supports

as you get started. To make it as useful as possible, we have written about educational psychology while keeping in

mind the current state of teaching, as well as your needs as a unique future teacher. The text draws heavily on

concepts, research and fundamental theories from educational psychology. But these are selected and framed

around the problems, challenges, and satisfactions faced by teachers daily, and especially as faced by teachers new

to the profession. We have selected and emphasized topics in proportion to two factors: (1) their importance as

reported by teachers and other educational experts, and (2) the ability of educational psychology to comment on

particular problems, challenges, and satisfactions helpfully.

Educational Psychology 16 A Global Text

1. The changing teaching profession and you

There is a lot to learn about teaching, and much of it comes from educational psychology. As a career, teaching

has distinctive features now that it did not have a generation ago. The new features make it more exciting in some

ways, as well as more challenging than in the past. The changes require learning teaching skills that were less

important in earlier times. But the new skills are quite learnable. Educational psychology, and this text, will get you

started at that task.

Chapter summary

Teaching in the twenty-first century offers a number of satisfactions—witnessing and assisting the growth of

young people, lifelong learning, the challenge and excitement of designing effective instruction. Four trends have

affected the way that these satisfactions are experienced by classroom teachers: (1) increased diversity of students,

(2) the spread of instructional technology in schools and classrooms, (3) increased expectations for accountability

in education, and (4) the development of increased professionalism among teachers. Each trend presents new

opportunities to students and teachers, but also raises new issues for teachers. Educational psychology, and this

textbook, can help teachers to make constructive use of the new trends as well as deal with the dilemmas that

accompany them. It offers information, advice, and useful perspectives specifically in three areas of teaching: (1)

students as learners, (2) instruction and assessment, and (3) the psychological and social awareness of teachers.

On the Internet

Try this website of the Educational Testing Service if you are curious to learn more

about licensing examinations for teachers, including the PRAXIS II test that is prominent in the United States (see

pp. xxx). As you will see, specific requirements vary somewhat by state and region.

This is the website for the education branch of UNESCO, which is the

abbreviation for the “United Nations Educational, Scientific, and Cultural Organization.” It has extensive

information and news about all forms of diversity in education, viewed from an international perspective. The

challenges of teaching diverse classrooms, it seems, are not restricted to the United States, though as the new items

on the website show, the challenges take different forms in different countries.

These two websites have numerous resources about diversity

for teachers from a North American (USA and Canada) perspective. They are both useful for planning instruction.

The first one—maintained by a group of educators and calling itself EdChange—focuses on culturally related forms

of diversity, and the second one—by the Council for Exceptional Children—focuses on children with special

educational needs.

Key termsAccountability in education

Action researchAssessment

DiversityHigh-stakes testing

Instructional technology

Lifelong learningProfessionalism

Teacher research

References

Bash, L. (Ed.). (2005). Best practices in adult learning. Boston: Anker Publications.

Bredekamp, S. & Copple, C. (1997). Developmentally appropriate practice, Revised edition. Washington,

D.C.: National Association for the Education of Young Children.

17

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Cochran-Smith, M. (2003). Assessing assessment in teacher education. Journal of Teacher Education, 54(3),

187-191.

Cochran-Smith, M. & Fries, K. (2005). Research teacher education in changing times: Politics and

paradigms. In M. Cochran-Smith & K. Zeichner (Eds.), Studying teacher education: The report of the

AERA Panel on Research and Teacher Education, 69-110.

Cuban, L. (2001). Oversold and underused: Computers in the classroom. Cambridge, MA: Harvard

University Press.

Educational Testing Service. (2004). Study guide for Principles of Learning and Teaching, 2nd edition.

Princeton, NJ: Author.

Fuhrman, S. & Elmore, R. (2004). Redesigning accountability systems for education. New York: Teachers

College Press.

Gebhard, L. (2006). Teaching English as a second or foreign language: A teacher self-development and

methodology guide, 2nd edition. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press.

Glassford, L. (2005). Triumph of politics over pedagogy? The case of the Ontario Teacher Qualifying Test.

Canadian Journal of Educational Administration and Policy, Issue #45. Online at

.

Haertel, G. & Means, B. (2003). Evaluating educational technology: Effective research designs for

improving learning. New York: Teachers College Press.

Harris, D. & Herrington, C. (2006). Accountability, standards, and the growing achievement gap: Lessons

from the past half-century. American Journal of Education, 112(2), 163-208.

Harvard Educational Review. (2005). Interview: United States Secretary of Education Margaret Spellings.

Harvard Educational Review, 75(4), 364-382.

Kushner, H. (1983). When bad things happen to good people. New York: Schocken Books.

Lubienski, C. (2005). Public schools in marketized environments: Shifting incentives and unintended

consequences of competition-based educational reforms. American Journal of Education, 111(4), 464-

486.

Mertler, C. (2006). Action research: Teachers as researchers in the classroom. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

National Institute for Early Education Research. (2006). Percent of population age 3 and 4 who are enrolled

in school: Census 2000. Retrieved on March 21, 2006 from .

Neil, M. (2003). The dangers of testing. Educational Leadership, 60(5), 43-46.

Pitt, K. (2005). Debates in ESL teaching and learning: Culture, communities, and classrooms. London, UK:

Routledge.

Rudalevige, A. (2005, August). Reform or séance? Seeking the “spirit” of the No Child Left Behind. Teachers

College Record. Online at , ID# 12112.

Educational Psychology 18 A Global Text

1. The changing teaching profession and you

Seiter, E. (2005). The INTERNET playground: Children’s access, entertainment, and miseducation. New

York: Peter Lang.

Stringer, E. (2004). Action research in education. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Education.

Sutton, R. (2004). Teaching under high-stakes testing: Dilemmas and decisions of a teacher educator.

Journal of Teacher Education, 55(5), 463-475.

United States Census Bureau. (2005). The Hispanic population in the United States: 2004. Retrieved on

March 21, 2006 from .

United States Government Printing Office. (2002). No Child Left Behind Act: A desktop reference.

Washington, D.C.: Author.

Federal Registry. (2005, June 21). Assistance To States for the Education of Children with Disabilities.

United States Government Printing Office: Author.

Volante, L. (2004). Teaching to the test: What every educator and policy-maker should know. Canadian

Journal of Educational Administration and Policy, Issue #35. Online at

.

19

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

2. The learning process When my son Michael was old enough to talk, and being an eager but naïve dad, I decided to bring

Michael to my educational psychology class to demonstrate to my students “how children learn”. In

one task I poured water from a tall drinking glass to a wide glass pie plate, which according to

Michael changed the “amount” of water—there was less now than it was in the pie plate. I told him

that, on the contrary, the amount of water had stayed the same whether it was in the glass or the pie

plate. He looked at me a bit strangely, but complied with my point of view—agreeing at first that,

yes, the amount had stayed the same. But by the end of the class session he had reverted to his

original position: there was less water, he said, when it was poured into the pie plate compared to

being poured into the drinking glass. So much for demonstrating “learning”!

(Kelvin Seifert)

Learning is generally defined as relatively permanent changes in behavior, skills, knowledge, or attitudes

resulting from identifiable psychological or social experiences. A key feature is permanence: changes do not count

as learning if they are temporary. You do not “learn” a phone number if you forget it the minute after you dial the

number; you do not “learn” to eat vegetables if you only do it when forced. The change has to last. Notice, though,

that learning can be physical, social, or emotional as well as cognitive. You do not “learn” to sneeze simply by

catching cold, but you do learn many skills and behaviors that are physically based, such as riding a bicycle or

throwing a ball. You can also learn to like (or dislike) a person, even though this change may not happen

deliberately.

Each year after that first visit to my students, while Michael was still a preschooler, I returned with

him to my ed-psych class to do the same “learning demonstrations”. And each year Michael came

along happily, but would again fail the task about the drinking glass and the pie plate. He would

comply briefly if I “suggested” that the amount of water stayed the same no matter which way it was

poured, but in the end he would still assert that the amount had changed. He was not learning this

bit of conventional knowledge, in spite of my repeated efforts.

But the year he turned six, things changed. When I told him it was time to visit my ed-psych class

again, he readily agreed and asked: “Are you going to ask me about the water in the drinking glass

and pie plate again?” I said yes, I was indeed planning to do that task again. “That’s good”, he

responded, “because I know that the amount stays the same even after you pour it. But do you want

me to fake it this time? For your students’ sake?”

Teachers’ perspectives on learning

For teachers, learning usually refers to things that happen in schools or classrooms, even though every teacher

can of course describe examples of learning that happen outside of these places. Even Michael, at age 6, had begun

realizing that what counted as “learning” in his dad’s educator-type mind was something that happened in a

Educational Psychology 20 A Global Text

2. The learning process

classroom, under the supervision of a teacher (me). For me, as for many educators, the term has a more specific

meaning than for many people less involved in schools. In particular, teachers’ perspectives on learning often

emphasize three ideas, and sometimes even take them for granted: (1) curriculum content and academic

achievement, (2) sequencing and readiness, and (3) the importance of transferring learning to new or future

situations.

Viewing learning as dependent on curriculum

When teachers speak of learning, they tend to emphasize whatever is taught in schools deliberately, including

both the official curriculum and the various behaviors and routines that make classrooms run smoothly. In practice,

defining learning in this way often means that teachers equate learning with the major forms of academic

achievement—especially language and mathematics—and to a lesser extent musical skill, physical coordination, or

social sensitivity (Gardner, 1999, 2006). The imbalance occurs not because the goals of public education make

teachers responsible for certain content and activities (like books and reading) and the skills which these activities

require (like answering teachers’ questions and writing essays). It does happen not (thankfully!) because teachers

are biased, insensitive, or unaware that students often learn a lot outside of school.

A side effect of thinking of learning as related only to curriculum or academics is that classroom social

interactions and behaviors become issues for teachers—become things that they need to manage. In particular,

having dozens of students in one room makes it more likely that I, as a teacher, think of “learning” as something

that either takes concentration (to avoid being distracted by others) or that benefits from collaboration (to take

advantage of their presence). In the small space of a classroom, no other viewpoint about social interaction makes

sense. Yet in the wider world outside of school, learning often does happen incidentally, “accidentally” and without

conscious interference or input from others: I “learn” what a friend’s personality is like, for example, without either

of us deliberately trying to make this happen. As teachers, we sometimes see incidental learning in classrooms as

well, and often welcome it; but our responsibility for curriculum goals more often focuses our efforts on what

students can learn through conscious, deliberate effort. In a classroom, unlike in many other human settings, it is

always necessary to ask whether classmates are helping or hindering individual students’ learning.

Focusing learning on changes in classrooms has several other effects. One, for example, is that it can tempt

teachers to think that what is taught is equivalent to what is learned—even though most teachers know that doing

so is a mistake, and that teaching and learning can be quite different. If I assign a reading to my students about the

Russian Revolution, it would be nice to assume not only that they have read the same words, but also learned the

same content. But that assumption is not usually the reality. Some students may have read and learned all of what I

assigned; others may have read everything but misunderstood the material or remembered only some of it; and still

others, unfortunately, may have neither read nor learned much of anything. Chances are that my students would

confirm this picture, if asked confidentially. There are ways, of course, to deal helpfully with such diversity of

outcomes; for suggestions, see especially Chapter 10 “Planning instruction” and Chapter 11 “Teacher-made

assessment strategies”. But whatever instructional strategies I adopt, they cannot include assuming that what I

teach is the same as what students understand or retain of what I teach.

21

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Viewing learning as dependent on sequencing and readiness

The distinction between teaching and learning creates a secondary issue for teachers, that of educational

readiness. Traditionally the concept referred to students’ preparedness to cope with or profit from the activities

and expectations of school. A kindergarten child was “ready” to start school, for example, if he or she was in good

health, showed moderately good social skills, could take care of personal physical needs (like eating lunch or going

to the bathroom unsupervised), could use a pencil to make simple drawings, and so on. Table 3 shows a similar set

of criteria for determining whether a child is “ready” to learn to read (Copple & Bredekamp, 2006). At older ages

(such as in high school or university), the term readiness is often replaced by a more specific term, prerequisites. To

take a course in physics, for example, a student must first have certain prerequisite experiences, such as studying

advanced algebra or calculus. To begin work as a public school teacher, a person must first engage in practice

teaching for a period of time (not to mention also studying educational psychology!).

Table 2: Reading readiness in students vs in teachers

Signs of readiness in the child or student Signs of readiness to teach reading

• productive (speaking) vocabulary of 5,000-

8,000 words

• child understands and uses complete sentences

• child’s questions tend to be relevant to the task

at hand

• child’s correctly using most common

grammatical constructions

• child can match some letters to some sounds

• child can string a few letters together to make a

few simple words

• child can tell and retell stories, poems, and

songs

• teacher answers children’s questions when

possible

• teacher encourages child to find out more

through other means in addition to asking teacher

• teacher asks questions designed to elaborate or

expand child’s thinking

• teacher highlights letters and sounds in the

classroom

• teacher provides lots of paper and marking

tools

• teacher assists child with initial writing of

letters

• teacher encourages children to enact stories,

poems, and songs

Source: Copple & Bredekamp, 2006.

Note that this traditional meaning, of readiness as preparedness, focuses attention on students’ adjustment to

school and away from the reverse: the possibility that schools and teachers also have a responsibility for adjusting

to students. But the latter idea is in fact a legitimate, second meaning for readiness: If 5-year-old children

normally need to play a lot and keep active, then it is fair to say that their kindergarten teacher needs to be “ready”

for this behavior by planning for a program that allows a lot of play and physical activity. If she cannot or will not

Educational Psychology 22 A Global Text

2. The learning process

do so (whatever the reason may be), then in a very real sense this failure is not the children’s responsibility. Among

older students, the second, teacher-oriented meaning of readiness makes sense as well. If a teacher has a student

with a disability (for example, the student is visually impaired), then the teacher has to adjust her approach in

appropriate ways—not simply expect a visually impaired child to “sink or swim”. As you might expect, this sense of

readiness is very important for special education, so I discuss it further in Chapter 5 “Students with special

educational needs”. But the issue of readiness also figures importantly whenever students are diverse (which is

most of the time), so it also comes up in Chapter 4 “Student diversity”.

Viewing transfer as a crucial outcome of learning

Still another result of focusing the concept of learning on classrooms is that it raises issues of usefulness or

transfer, which is the ability to use knowledge or skill in situations beyond the ones in which they are acquired.

Learning to read and learning to solve arithmetic problems, for example, are major goals of the elementary school

curriculum because those skills are meant to be used not only inside the classroom, but outside as well. We teachers

intend, that is, for reading and arithmetic skills to “transfer”, even though we also do our best to make the skills

enjoyable while they are still being learned. In the world inhabited by teachers, even more than in other worlds,

making learning fun is certainly a good thing to do, but making learning useful as well as fun is even better.

Combining enjoyment and usefulness, in fact, is a “gold standard” of teaching: we generally seek it for students,

even though we may not succeed at providing it all of the time.

Major theories and models of learning

Several ideas and priorities, then, affect how we teachers think about learning, including the curriculum, the

difference between teaching and learning, sequencing, readiness, and transfer. The ideas form a “screen” through

which to understand and evaluate whatever psychology has to offer education. As it turns out, many theories,

concepts, and ideas from educational psychology do make it through the “screen” of education, meaning that they

are consistent with the professional priorities of teachers and helpful in solving important problems of classroom

teaching. In the case of issues about classroom learning, for example, educational psychologists have developed a

number of theories and concepts that are relevant to classrooms, in that they describe at least some of what usually

happens there and offer guidance for assisting learning. It is helpful to group the theories according to whether they

focus on changes in behavior or in thinking. The distinction is rough and inexact, but a good place to begin. For

starters, therefore, consider two perspectives about learning, called behaviorism (learning as changes in overt

behavior) and constructivism, (learning as changes in thinking). The second category can be further divided into

psychological constructivism (changes in thinking resulting from individual experiences), and social

constructivism, (changes in thinking due to assistance from others). The rest of this chapter describes key ideas

from each of these viewpoints. As I hope you will see, each describes some aspects of learning not just in general,

but as it happens in classrooms in particular. So each perspective suggests things that you might do in your

classroom to make students’ learning more productive.

Behaviorism: changes in what students do

Behaviorism is a perspective on learning that focuses on changes in individuals’ observable behaviors—

changes in what people say or do. At some point we all use this perspective, whether we call it “behaviorism” or

something else. The first time that I drove a car, for example, I was concerned primarily with whether I could

actually do the driving, not with whether I could describe or explain how to drive. For another example: when I

23

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

reached the point in life where I began cooking meals for myself, I was more focused on whether I could actually

produce edible food in a kitchen than with whether I could explain my recipes and cooking procedures to others.

And still another example—one often relevant to new teachers: when I began my first year of teaching, I was more

focused on doing the job of teaching—on day-to-day survival—than on pausing to reflect on what I was doing.

Note that in all of these examples, focusing attention on behavior instead of on “thoughts” may have been

desirable at that moment, but not necessarily desirable indefinitely or all of the time. Even as a beginner, there are

times when it is more important to be able to describe how to drive or to cook than to actually do these things. And

there definitely are many times when reflecting on and thinking about teaching can improve teaching itself. (As a

teacher-friend once said to me: “Don’t just do something; stand there!”) But neither is focusing on behavior which

is not necessarily less desirable than focusing on students’ “inner” changes, such as gains in their knowledge or

their personal attitudes. If you are teaching, you will need to attend to all forms of learning in students, whether

inner or outward.

In classrooms, behaviorism is most useful for identifying relationships between specific actions by a student and

the immediate precursors and consequences of the actions. It is less useful for understanding changes in students’

thinking; for this purpose we need a more cognitive (or thinking-oriented) theory, like the ones described later in

this chapter. This fact is not really a criticism of behaviorism as a perspective, but just a clarification of its particular

strength or source of usefulness, which is to highlight observable relationships among actions, precursors and

consequences. Behaviorists use particular terms (or “lingo”, some might say) for these relationships. They also rely

primarily on two basic images or models of behavioral learning, called respondent (or “classical”) conditioning and

operant conditioning. The names are derived partly from the major learning mechanisms highlighted by each type,

which I describe next.

Respondent conditioning: learning new associations with prior behaviors

As originally conceived, respondent conditioning (sometimes also called classical conditioning) begins with

the involuntary responses to particular sights, sounds, or other sensations (Lavond, 2003). When I receive an

injection from a nurse or doctor, for example, I cringe, tighten my muscles, and even perspire a bit. Whenever a

contented, happy baby looks at me, on the other hand, I invariably smile in response. I cannot help myself in either

case; both of the responses are automatic. In humans as well as other animals, there is a repertoire or variety of

such specific, involuntary behaviors. At the sound of a sudden loud noise, for example, most of us show a “startle”

response—we drop what we are doing (sometimes literally!), our heart rate shoots up temporarily, and we look for

the source of the sound. Cats, dogs and many other animals (even fish in an aquarium) show similar or equivalent

responses.

Involuntary stimuli and responses were first studied systematically early in the twentieth-century by the Russian

scientist Ivan Pavlov (1927). Pavlov’s most well-known work did not involve humans, but dogs, and specifically

their involuntary tendency to salivate when eating. He attached a small tube to the side of dogs’ mouths that

allowed him to measure how much the dogs salivated when fed (Exhibit 1 shows a photograph of one of Pavlov's

dogs). But he soon noticed a “problem” with the procedure: as the dogs gained experience with the experiment, they

often salivated before they began eating. In fact the most experienced dogs sometimes began salivating before they

even saw any food, simply when Pavlov himself entered the room! The sight of the experimenter, which had

Educational Psychology 24 A Global Text

2. The learning process

originally been a neutral experience for the dogs, became associated with the dogs’ original salivation response.

Eventually, in fact, the dogs would salivate at the sight of Pavlov even if he did not feed them.

This change in the dogs’ involuntary response, and especially its growing independence from the food as

stimulus, eventually became the focus of Pavlov’s research. Psychologists named the process respondent

conditioning because it describes changes in responses to stimuli (though some have also called it “classical

conditioning” because it was historically the first form of behavioral learning to be studied systematically).

Respondent conditioning has several elements, each with a special name. To understand these, look at and imagine

a dog (perhaps even mine, named Ginger) prior to any conditioning. At the beginning Ginger salivates (an

unconditioned response (UR)) only when she actually tastes her dinner (an unconditioned stimulus

(US)). As time goes by, however, a neutral stimulus—such as the sound of opening a bag containing fresh dog food

—is continually paired with the eating/tasting experience. Eventually the neutral stimulus becomes able to elicit

salivation even before any dog food is offered to Ginger, or even if the bag of food is empty! At this point the neutral

stimulus is called a conditioned stimulus (UCS) and the original response is renamed as a conditioned

response (CR). Now, after conditioning, Ginger salivates merely at the sound of opening any large bag,

regardless of its contents. (I might add that Ginger also engages in other conditioned responses, such as looking

hopeful and following me around the house at dinner time.)

Before Conditioning:

(UCS) Food→ Salivation (UR)

(UCS) Bell→ No response (UR)

During Conditioning:

Bell + Food→ Salivation

After Conditioning:

(CS) Bell only→ Salivation (CR)

Exhibit 1: Classical conditioning of Ginger, the dog. Before conditioning, Ginger salivates only to the taste of

food and the bell has no effect. After conditioning, she salivates even when the bell is presented by itself.

Respondent Conditioning and Students

“OK,” you may be thinking, “Respondent conditioning may happen to animals. But does anything like it happen

in classrooms?” It might seem like not much would, since teaching is usually about influencing students’ conscious

words and thoughts, and not their involuntary behaviors. But remember that schooling is not just about

encouraging thinking and talking. Teachers, like parents and the public, also seek positive changes in students’

attitudes and feelings—attitudes like a love for learning, for example, and feelings like self-confidence. It turns out

that respondent conditioning describes these kinds of changes relatively well.

25

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Consider, for example, a child who responds happily whenever meeting a new person who is warm and friendly,

but who also responds cautiously or at least neutrally in any new situation. Suppose further that the “new, friendly

person” in question is you, his teacher. Initially the child’s response to you is like an unconditioned stimulus: you

smile (the unconditioned stimulus) and in response he perks up, breathes easier, and smiles (the unconditioned

response). This exchange is not the whole story, however, but merely the setting for an important bit of behavior

change: suppose you smile at him while standing in your classroom, a “new situation” and therefore one to which

he normally responds cautiously. Now respondent learning can occur. The initially neutral stimulus (your

classroom) becomes associated repeatedly with the original unconditioned stimulus (your smile) and the child’s

unconditioned response (his smile). Eventually, if all goes well, the classroom becomes a conditioned stimulus in its

own right: it can elicit the child’s smiles and other “happy behaviors” even without your immediate presence or

stimulus. Exhibit 2 diagrams the situation graphically. When the change in behavior happens, you might say that

the child has “learned” to like being in your classroom. Truly a pleasing outcome for both of you!

Before Conditioning:

(UCS) Seeing Teacher Smile → Student Smiles (UR)

(UCS) Seeing Classroom → No response (UR)

During Conditioning:

Seeing Teaching Smile + Seeing Classroom → Student Smiles

After Conditioning:

(CS) Seeing Classroom → Student Smiles (CR)

Exhibit 2: Respondent conditioning of student to classroom. Before conditioning, the student smiles only when

he sees the teacher smile, and the sight of the classroom has no effect. After conditioning, the student smiles at the

sight of the classroom even without the teacher present.

But less positive or desirable examples of respondent conditioning also can happen. Consider a modification of

the example that I just gave. Suppose the child that I just mentioned did not have the good fortune of being placed

in your classroom. Instead he found himself with a less likeable teacher, whom we could simply call Mr Horrible.

Instead of smiling a lot and eliciting the child’s unconditioned “happy response”, Mr Horrible often frowns and

scowls at the child. In this case, therefore, the child’s initial unconditioned response is negative: whenever Mr

Horrible directs a frown or scowl at the child, the child automatically cringes a little, his eyes widen in fear, and his

heart beat races. If the child sees Mr Horrible doing most of his frowning and scowling in the classroom, eventually

the classroom itself will acquire power as a negative conditioned stimulus. Eventually, that is, the child will not

need Mr Horrible to be present in order to feel apprehensive; simply being in the classroom will be enough. Exhibit

3 diagrams this unfortunate situation. Obviously it is an outcome to be avoided, and in fact does not usually happen

in such an extreme way. But hopefully it makes the point: any stimulus that is initially neutral, but that gets

Educational Psychology 26 A Global Text

2. The learning process

associated with an unconditioned stimulus and response, can eventually acquire the ability to elicit the response by

itself. Anything—whether it is desirable or not.

Before Conditioning:

( UCS) Mr Horrible Frowns → Student Cringes (UCR)

Mr Horrible’s Classroom → No response

During Conditioning:

Mr Horrible Frowns + Sight of Classroom → Student Cringes

After Conditioning:

( CS) Seeing Classroom → Student Cringes ( CR)

Exhibit 3: Respondent conditioning of student to classroom. Before conditioning, the student cringes only

when he sees Mr Horrible smile, and the sight of the classroom has no effect. After conditioning, the student

cringes at the sight of the classroom even without Mr Horrible present.

The changes described in these two examples are important because they can affect students’ attitude about

school, and therefore also their motivation to learn. In the positive case, the child becomes more inclined to please

the teacher and to attend to what he or she has to offer; in the negative case, the opposite occurs. Since the changes

in attitude happen “inside” the child, they are best thought of as one way that a child can acquire i intrinsic

motivation, meaning a desire or tendency to direct attention and energy in a particular way that originates from

the child himself or herself. Intrinsic motivation is sometimes contrasted to extrinsic motivation, a tendency to

direct attention and energy that originates from outside of the child. As we will see, classical conditioning can

influence students’ intrinsic motivation in directions that are either positive or negative. As you might suspect,

there are other ways to influence motivation as well. Many of these are described in Chapter 6 (“Student

motivation”). First, though, let us look at three other features of classical conditioning that complicate the picture a

bit, but also render conditioning a bit more accurate, an appropriate description of students’ learning.

Three key ideas about respondent conditioning

Extinction: This term does not refer to the fate of dinosaurs, but to the disappearance of a link between the

conditioned stimulus and the conditioned response. Imagine a third variation on the conditioning “story” described

above. Suppose, as I suggested above, that the child begins by associating your happy behaviors—your smiles—to

his being present in the classroom, so that the classroom itself becomes enough to elicit his own smiles. But now

suppose there is a sad turn of events: you become sick and must therefore leave the classroom in the middle of the

school year. A substitute is called in who is not Mr Horrible, but simply someone who is not very expressive,

someone we can call Ms Neutral. At first the child continues to feel good (that is, to smile) whenever present in the

classroom. But because the link between the classroom and your particular smile is no longer repeated or

associated, the child’s response gradually extinguishes, or fades until it has disappeared entirely. In a sense the

child’s initial learning is “unlearned”.

27

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Extinction can also happen with negative examples of classical conditioning. If Mr Horrible leaves mid-year

(perhaps because no one could stand working with him any longer!), then the child’s negative responses (cringing,

eyes widening, heart beat racing, and so on) will also extinguish eventually. Note, though, that whether the

conditioned stimulus is positive or negative, extinction does not happen suddenly or immediately, but unfolds over

time. This fact can sometimes obscure the process if you are a busy teacher attending to many students.

Generalization: When Pavlov studied conditioning in dogs, he noticed that the original conditioned stimulus

was not the only neutral stimulus that elicited the conditioned response. If he paired a particular bell with the sight

of food, for example, so that the bell became a conditioned stimulus for salivation, then it turned out that other

bells, perhaps with a different pitch or type or sound, also acquired some ability to trigger salivation—though not as

much as the original bell. Psychologists call this process generalization, or the tendency for similar stimuli to elicit a

conditioned response. The child being conditioned to your smile, for example, might learn to associate your smile

not only with being present in your classroom, but also to being present in other, similar classrooms. His

conditioned smiles may be strongest where he learned them initially (that is, in your own room), but nonetheless

visible to a significant extent in other teachers’ classrooms. To the extent that this happens, he has generalized his

learning. It is of course good news; it means that we can say that the child is beginning to “learn to like school” in

general, and not just your particular room. Unfortunately, the opposite can also happen: if a child learns negative

associations from Mr Horrible, the child’s fear, caution, and stress might generalize to other classrooms as well. The

lesson for teachers is therefore clear: we have a responsibility, wherever possible, to make classrooms pleasant

places to be.

Discrimination: Generalization among similar stimuli can be reduced if only one of the similar stimuli is

associated consistently with the unconditioned response, while the others are not. When this happens,

psychologists say that discrimination learning has occurred, meaning that the individual has learned to

distinguish or respond differently to one stimulus than to another. From an educational point of view,

discrimination learning can be either desirable or not, depending on the particulars of the situation. Imagine again

(for the fourth time!) the child who learns to associate your classroom with your smiles, so that he eventually

produces smiles of his own whenever present in your room. But now imagine yet another variation on his story: the

child is old enough to attend middle school, and therefore has several teachers across the day. You—with your

smiles—are one, but so are Mr Horrible and Ms Neutral. At first the child may generalize his classically conditioned

smiles to the other teachers’ classrooms. But the other teachers do not smile like you do, and this fact causes the

child’s smiling to extinguish somewhat in their rooms. Meanwhile, you keep smiling in your room. Eventually the

child is smiling only in your room and not in the other rooms. When this happens, we say that discrimination has

occurred, meaning that the conditioned associations happen only to a single version of the unconditioned stimuli—

in this case, only to your smiles, and not to the (rather rare) occurrences of smiles in the other classrooms. Judging

by his behavior, the child is making a distinction between your room and others.

In one sense the discrimination in this story is unfortunate in that it prevents the child from acquiring a liking

for school that is generalized. But notice that an opposing, more desirable process is happening at the same time:

the child is also prevented from acquiring a generalized dislike of school. The fear-producing stimuli from Mr

Horrible, in particular, become discriminated from the happiness-producing smiles from you, so the child’s learns

to confine his fearful responses to that particular classroom, and does not generalize them to other “innocent”

Educational Psychology 28 A Global Text

2. The learning process

classrooms, including your own. This is still not an ideal situation for the student, but maybe it is more desirable

than disliking school altogether.

Operant conditioning: new behaviors because of new consequences

Instead of focusing on associations between stimuli and responses, operant conditioning focuses on how the

effects of consequences on behaviors. The operant model of learning begins with the idea that certain consequences

tend to make certain behaviors happen more frequently. If I compliment a student for a good comment during a

discussion, there is more of a chance that I will hear comments from the student more often in the future (and

hopefully they will also be good ones!). If a student tells a joke to several classmates and they laugh at it, then the

student is more likely to tell additional jokes in the future and so on.

As with respondent conditioning, the original research about this model of learning was not done with people,

but with animals. One of the pioneers in the field was a Harvard professor named B. F. Skinner, who published

numerous books and articles about the details of the process and who pointed out many parallels between operant

conditioning in animals and operant conditioning in humans (1938, 1948, 1988). Skinner observed the behavior of

rather tame laboratory rats (not the unpleasant kind that sometimes live in garbage dumps). He or his assistants

would put them in a cage that contained little except a lever and a small tray just big enough to hold a small amount

of food. (Exhibit 4 shows the basic set-up, which is sometimes nicknamed a “Skinner box”.) At first the rat would

sniff and “putter around” the cage at random, but sooner or later it would happen upon the lever and eventually

happen to press it. Presto! The lever released a small pellet of food, which the rat would promptly eat. Gradually the

rat would spend more time near the lever and press the lever more frequently, getting food more frequently.

Eventually it would spend most of its time at the lever and eating its fill of food. The rat had “discovered” that the

consequence of pressing the level was to receive food. Skinner called the changes in the rat’s behavior an example of

operant conditioning, and gave special names to the different parts of the process. He called the food pellets the

reinforcement and the lever-pressing the operant (because it “operated” on the rat’s environment). See below.

Operant → Reinforcement

Press lever → Food pellet

Exhibit 4: Operant conditioning with a laboratory rat

Skinner and other behavioral psychologists experimented with using various reinforcers and operants. They also

experimented with various patterns of reinforcement (or schedules of reinforcement), as well as with various

29

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

cues or signals to the animal about when reinforcement was available. It turned out that all of these factors—the

operant, the reinforcement, the schedule, and the cues—affected how easily and thoroughly operant conditioning

occurred. For example, reinforcement was more effective if it came immediately after the crucial operant behavior,

rather than being delayed, and reinforcements that happened intermittently (only part of the time) caused learning

to take longer, but also caused it to last longer.

Operant conditioning and students’ learning: As with respondent conditioning, it is important to ask

whether operant conditioning also describes learning in human beings, and especially in students in classrooms. On

this point the answer seems to be clearly “yes”. There are countless classroom examples of consequences affecting

students’ behavior in ways that resemble operant conditioning, although the process certainly does not account for

all forms of student learning (Alberto & Troutman, 2005). Consider the following examples. In most of them the

operant behavior tends to become more frequent on repeated occasions:

• A seventh-grade boy makes a silly face (the operant) at the girl sitting next to him. Classmates sitting around

them giggle in response (the reinforcement).

• A kindergarten child raises her hand in response to the teacher’s question about a story (the operant). The

teacher calls on her and she makes her comment (the reinforcement).

• Another kindergarten child blurts out her comment without being called on (the operant). The teacher frowns,

ignores this behavior, but before the teacher calls on a different student, classmates are listening attentively

(the reinforcement) to the student even though he did not raise his hand as he should have.

• A twelfth-grade student—a member of the track team—runs one mile during practice (the operant). He notes

the time it takes him as well as his increase in speed since joining the team (the reinforcement).

• A child who is usually very restless sits for five minutes doing an assignment (the operant). The teaching

assistant compliments him for working hard (the reinforcement).

• A sixth-grader takes home a book from the classroom library to read overnight (the operant). When she

returns the book the next morning, her teacher puts a gold star by her name on a chart posted in the room (the

reinforcement).

Hopefully these examples are enough to make four points about operant conditioning. First, the process is

widespread in classrooms—probably more widespread than respondent conditioning. This fact makes sense, given

the nature of public education: to a large extent, teaching is about making certain consequences for students (like

praise or marks) depend on students’ engaging in certain activities (like reading certain material or doing

assignments). Second, learning by operant conditioning is not confined to any particular grade, subject area, or

style of teaching, but by nature happens in nearly every imaginable classroom. Third, teachers are not the only

persons controlling reinforcements. Sometimes they are controlled by the activity itself (as in the track team

example), or by classmates (as in the “giggling” example). A result of all of the above points is the fourth: that

multiple examples of operant conditioning often happen at the same time. The skill builder for this chapter (The

decline and fall of Jane Gladstone) suggests how this happened to someone completing student teaching.

Educational Psychology 30 A Global Text

2. The learning process

Because operant conditioning happens so widely, its effects on motivation are a bit more complex than the

effects of respondent conditioning. As in respondent conditioning, operant conditioning can encourage intrinsic

motivation to the extent that the reinforcement for an activity can sometimes be the activity itself. When a student

reads a book for the sheer enjoyment of reading, for example, he is reinforced by the reading itself; then we often

say that his reading is “intrinsically motivated”. More often, however, operant conditioning stimulates both

intrinsic and extrinsic motivation at the same time. The combining of both is noticeable in the examples that I

listed above. In each example, it is reasonable to assume that the student felt intrinsically motivated to some partial

extent, even when reward came from outside the student as well. This was because part of what reinforced their

behavior was the behavior itself—whether it was making faces, running a mile, or contributing to a discussion. At

the same time, though, note that each student probably was also extrinsically motivated, meaning that another

part of the reinforcement came from consequences or experiences not inherently part of the activity or behavior

itself. The boy who made a face was reinforced not only by the pleasure of making a face, for example, but also by

the giggles of classmates. The track student was reinforced not only by the pleasure of running itself, but also by

knowledge of his improved times and speeds. Even the usually restless child sitting still for five minutes may have

been reinforced partly by this brief experience of unusually focused activity, even if he was also reinforced by the

teacher aide’s compliment. Note that the extrinsic part of the reinforcement may sometimes be more easily

observed or noticed than the intrinsic part, which by definition may sometimes only be experienced within the

individual and not also displayed outwardly. This latter fact may contribute to an impression that sometimes

occurs, that operant conditioning is really just “bribery in disguise”, that only the external reinforcements operate

on students’ behavior. It is true that external reinforcement may sometimes alter the nature or strength of internal

(or intrinsic) reinforcement, but this is not the same as saying that it destroys or replaces intrinsic reinforcement.

But more about this issue later! (See especially Chapter 6, “Student motivation”.)

Comparing operant conditioning and respondent conditioning: Operant conditioning is made more

complicated, but also more realistic, by many of the same concepts as used in respondent conditioning. In most

cases, however, the additional concepts have slightly different meanings in each model of learning. Since this

circumstance can make the terms confusing, let me explain the differences for three major concepts used in both

models—extinction, generalization, and discrimination. Then I will comment on two additional concepts—

schedules of reinforcement and cues—that are sometimes also used in talking about both forms of conditioning, but

that are important primarily for understanding operant conditioning. The explanations and comments are also

summarized in Table 2.

Table 3: Comparison of terms common to operant and respondent conditioning

Term As defined in respondent conditioning As defined in operant

conditioning

Extinction Disappearance of an association between a

conditioned stimulus and a conditioned

response

Disappearance of the operant

behavior due to lack of

reinforcement

Generalization Ability of stimulus similar to the conditioned

stimulus to elicit the conditioned response

Tendency of behaviors similar to

operant to be conditioned along with

31

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

the original operant

Discrimination Learning not to respond to stimuli that are

similar to the originally conditioned stimulus

Learning not to emit behaviors that

are similar to the originally

conditioned operant

Schedule of Reinforcement The pattern or frequency by which a CS is

paired with the UCS during learning

The pattern or frequency by which a

reinforcement is a consequence of

an operant during learning

Cue Not applicable Stimulus prior to the operant that

signals the availability or not of

reinforcement

In both respondent and operant conditioning, extinction refers to the disappearance of “something”. In

operant conditioning, what disappears is the operant behavior because of a lack of reinforcement. A student who

stops receiving gold stars or compliments for prolific reading of library books, for example, may extinguish (i.e.

decrease or stop) book-reading behavior. In respondent conditioning, on the other hand, what disappears is

association between the conditioned stimulus (the CS) and the conditioned response (CR). If you stop smiling at a

student, then the student may extinguish her association between you and her pleasurable response to your smile,

or between your classroom and the student’s pleasurable response to your smile.

In both forms of conditioning, generalization means that something “extra” gets conditioned if it is somehow

similar to “something”. In operant conditioning, the extra conditioning is to behaviors similar to the original

operant. If getting gold stars results in my reading more library books, then I may generalize this behavior to other

similar activities, such as reading the newspaper, even if the activity is not reinforced directly. In respondent

conditioning, however, the extra conditioning refers to stimuli similar to the original conditioned stimulus. If I am a

student and I respond happily to my teacher’s smiles, then I may find myself responding happily to other people

(like my other teachers) to some extent, even if they do not smile at me. Generalization is a lot like the concept of

transfer that I discussed early in this chapter, in that it is about extending prior learning to new situations or

contexts. From the perspective of operant conditioning, though, what is being extended (or “transferred” or

generalized) is a behavior, not knowledge or skill.

In both forms of conditioning, discrimination means learning not to generalize. In operant conditioning,

though, what is not being overgeneralized is the operant behavior. If I am a student who is being complimented

(reinforced) for contributing to discussions, I must also learn to discriminate when to make verbal contributions

from when not to make verbal contributions—such as when classmates or the teacher are busy with other tasks. In

respondent conditioning, what are not being overgeneralized are the conditioned stimuli that elicit the conditioned

response. If I, as a student, learn to associate the mere sight of a smiling teacher with my own happy, contented

behavior, then I also have to learn not to associate this same happy response with similar, but slightly different

sights, such as a teacher looking annoyed.

Educational Psychology 32 A Global Text

2. The learning process

In both forms of conditioning, the schedule of reinforcement refers to the pattern or frequency by which

“something” is paired with “something else”. In operant conditioning, what is being paired is the pattern by which

reinforcement is linked with the operant. If a teacher praises me for my work, does she do it every time, or only

sometimes? Frequently or only once in awhile? In respondent conditioning, however, the schedule in question is

the pattern by which the conditioned stimulus is paired with the unconditioned stimulus. If I am student with Mr

Horrible as my teacher, does he scowl every time he is in the classroom, or only sometimes? Frequently or rarely?

Behavioral psychologists have studied schedules of reinforcement extensively (for example, Ferster, et al., 1997;

Mazur, 2005), and found a number of interesting effects of different schedules. For teachers, however, the most

important finding may be this: partial or intermittent schedules of reinforcement generally cause learning to take

longer, but also cause extinction of learning to take longer. This dual principle is important for teachers because so

much of the reinforcement we give is partial or intermittent. Typically, if I am teaching, I can compliment a student

a lot of the time, for example, but there will inevitably be occasions when I cannot do so because I am busy

elsewhere in the classroom. For teachers concerned both about motivating students and about minimizing

inappropriate behaviors, this is both good news and bad. The good news is that the benefits of my praising students’

constructive behavior will be more lasting, because they will not extinguish their constructive behaviors

immediately if I fail to support them every single time they happen. The bad news is that students’ negative

behaviors may take longer to extinguish as well, because those too may have developed through partial

reinforcement. A student who clowns around inappropriately in class, for example, may not be “supported” by

classmates’ laughter every time it happens, but only some of the time. Once the inappropriate behavior is learned,

though, it will take somewhat longer to disappear even if everyone—both teacher and classmates—make a concerted

effort to ignore (or extinguish) it.

Finally, behavioral psychologists have studied the effects of cues. In operant conditioning, a cue is a stimulus

that happens just prior to the operant behavior and that signals that performing the behavior may lead to

reinforcement. Its effect is much like discrimination learning in respondent conditioning, except that what is

“discriminated” in this case is not a conditioned behavior that is reflex-like, but a voluntary action, the operant. In

the original conditioning experiments, Skinner’s rats were sometimes cued by the presence or absence of a small

electric light in their cage. Reinforcement was associated with pressing a lever when, and only when, the light was

on. In classrooms, cues are sometimes provided by the teacher or simply by the established routines of the class.

Calling on a student to speak, for example, can be a cue that if the student does say something at that moment, then

he or she may be reinforced with praise or acknowledgment. But if that cue does not occur—if the student is not

called on—speaking may not be rewarded. In more everyday, non-behaviorist terms, the cue allows the student to

learn when it is acceptable to speak, and when it is not.

Constructivism: changes in how students think

Behaviorist models of learning may be helpful in understanding and influencing what students do, but teachers

usually also want to know what students are thinking, and how to enrich what students are thinking. For this goal

of teaching, some of the best help comes from constructivism, which is a perspective on learning focused on how

students actively create (or “construct”) knowledge out of experiences. Constructivist models of learning differ

about how much a learner constructs knowledge independently, compared to how much he or she takes cues from

people who may be more of an expert and who help the learner's efforts (Fosnot, 2005; Rockmore, 2005). For

33

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

convenience these are called psychological constructivism and social constructivism, even though both

versions are in a sense explanations about thinking within individuals.

Psychological constructivism: the independent investigator

The main idea of psychological constructivism is that a person learns by mentally organizing and reorganizing

new information or experiences. The organization happens partly by relating new experiences to prior knowledge

that is already meaningful and well understood. Stated in this general form, individual constructivism is sometimes

associated with a well-known educational philosopher of the early twentieth century, John Dewey (1938-1998).

Although Dewey himself did not use the term constructivism in most of his writing, his point of view amounted to a

type of constructivism, and he discussed in detail its implications for educators. He argued, for example, that if

students indeed learn primarily by building their own knowledge, then teachers should adjust the curriculum to fit

students’ prior knowledge and interests as fully as possible. He also argued that a curriculum could only be justified

if it related as fully as possible to the activities and responsibilities that students will probably have later, after

leaving school. To many educators these days, his ideas may seem merely like good common sense, but they were

indeed innovative and progressive at the beginning of the twentieth century.

A more recent example of psychological constructivism is the cognitive theory of Jean Piaget (Piaget, 2001;

Gruber & Voneche, 1995). Piaget described learning as interplay between two mental activities that he called

assimilation and accommodation. Assimilation is the interpretation of new information in terms of pre-existing

concepts, information or ideas. A preschool child who already understands the concept of bird, for example, might

initially label any flying object with this term—even butterflies or mosquitoes. Assimilation is therefore a bit like the

idea of generalization in operant conditioning, or the idea of transfer described at the beginning of this chapter. In

Piaget’s viewpoint, though, what is being transferred to a new setting is not simply a behavior (Skinner's “operant”

in operant conditioning), but a mental representation for an object or experience.

Assimilation operates jointly with accommodation, which is the revision or modification of pre-existing

concepts in terms of new information or experience. The preschooler who initially generalizes the concept of bird to

include any flying object, for example, eventually revises the concept to include only particular kinds of flying

objects, such as robins and sparrows, and not others, like mosquitoes or airplanes. For Piaget, assimilation and

accommodation work together to enrich a child’s thinking and to create what Piaget called cognitive

equilibrium, which is a balance between reliance on prior information and openness to new information. At any

given time, cognitive equilibrium consists of an ever-growing repertoire of mental representations for objects and

experiences. Piaget called each mental representation a schema (all of them together—the plural—was called

schemata). A schema was not merely a concept, but an elaborated mixture of vocabulary, actions, and experience

related to the concept. A child’s schema for bird, for example, includes not only the relevant verbal knowledge (like

knowing how to define the word “bird”), but also the child’s experiences with birds, pictures of birds, and

conversations about birds. As assimilation and accommodation about birds and other flying objects operate

together over time, the child does not just revise and add to his vocabulary (such as acquiring a new word,

“butterfly”), but also adds and remembers relevant new experiences and actions. From these collective revisions

and additions the child gradually constructs whole new schemata about birds, butterflies, and other flying objects.

In more everyday (but also less precise) terms, Piaget might then say that “the child has learned more about birds”.

Educational Psychology 34 A Global Text

2. The learning process

The upper part of Exhibit 5 diagrams the relationships among the Piagetian version of psychological

constructivist learning. Note that the model of learning in the Exhibit is rather “individualistic”, in the sense that it

does not say much about how other people involved with the learner might assist in assimilating or accommodating

information. Parents and teachers, it would seem, are left lingering on the sidelines, with few significant

responsibilities for helping learners to construct knowledge. But the Piagetian picture does nonetheless imply a role

for helpful others: someone, after all, has to tell or model the vocabulary needed to talk about and compare birds

from airplanes and butterflies! Piaget did recognize the importance of helpful others in his writings and theorizing,

calling the process of support or assistance social transmission. But he did not emphasize this aspect of

constructivism. Piaget was more interested in what children and youth could figure out on their own, so to speak,

than in how teachers or parents might be able to help the young to figure out (Salkind, 2004). Partly for this reason,

his theory is often considered less about learning and more about development, which is long-term change in a

person resulting from multiple experiences. For the same reason, educators have often found Piaget’s ideas

especially helpful for thinking about students’ readiness to learn, another one of the lasting educational issues that

I discussed at the beginning of this chapter. I will therefore return to Piaget later to discuss development and its

importance for teaching in more detail.

Learning According to Piaget:

Assimilation + Accommodation → Equilibrium → Schemata

Learning According to Vygotsky:

Novice →Zone of Proximal Development ← Expert

(ZPD)

Exhibit 5: Constructivist models of learning

Social Constructivism: assisted performance

Unlike Piaget’s rather individually oriented version of constructivism, some psychologists and educators have

explicitly focused on the relationships and interactions between a learner and more knowledgeable and experienced

individuals. One early expression of this viewpoint came from the American psychologist Jerome Bruner (1960,

1966, 1996), who became convinced that students could usually learn more than had been traditionally expected as

long as they were given appropriate guidance and resources. He called such support instructional scaffolding—

literally meaning a temporary framework, like one used in constructing a building, that allows a much stronger

structure to be built within it. In a comment that has been quoted widely (and sometimes disputed), he wrote: “We

[constructivist educators] begin with the hypothesis that any subject can be taught effectively in some intellectually

honest form to any child at any stage of development.” (1960, p. 33). The reason for such a bold assertion was

Bruner’s belief in scaffolding—his belief in the importance of providing guidance in the right way and at the right

time. When scaffolding is provided, students seem more competent and “intelligent,” and they learn more.

Similar ideas were proposed independently by the Russian psychologist Lev Vygotsky (1978), whose writing

focused on how a child’s or novice’s thinking is influenced by relationships with others who are more capable,

35

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

knowledgeable, or expert than the learner. Vygotsky proposed that when a child (or any novice) is learning a new

skill or solving a new problem, he or she can perform better if accompanied and helped by an expert than if

performing alone—though still not as well as the expert. Someone who has played very little chess, for example, will

probably compete against an opponent better if helped by an expert chess player than if competing alone against an

opponent. Vygotsky called the difference between solo performance and assisted performance the zone of

proximal development (or ZPD for short)—meaning the place or area (figuratively speaking) of immediate

change. From this perspective learning is like assisted performance (Tharp & Gallimore, 1991). Initially during

learning, knowledge or skill is found mostly “in” the expert helper. If the expert is skilled and motivated to help,

then the expert arranges experiences that allow the novice to practice crucial skills or to construct new knowledge.

In this regard the expert is a bit like the coach of an athlete—offering help and suggesting ways of practicing, but

never doing the actual athletic work himself or herself. Gradually, by providing continued experiences matched to

the novice learner’s emerging competencies, the expert-coach makes it possible for the novice or apprentice to

appropriate (or make his or her own) the skills or knowledge that originally resided only with the expert. These

relationships are diagrammed in the lower part of Exhibit 5.

In both the psychological and social versions of constructivist learning, the novice is not really “taught” so much

as just allowed to learn. The social version of constructivism, however, highlights the responsibility of the expert for

making learning possible. He or she must not only have knowledge and skill, but also know how to arrange

experiences that make it easy and safe for learners to gain knowledge and skill themselves. These requirements

sound, of course, a lot like the requirements for classroom teaching. In addition to knowing what is to be learned,

the expert (i.e. the teacher) also has to break the content into manageable parts, offer the parts in a sensible

sequence, provide for suitable and successful practice, bring the parts back together again at the end, and somehow

relate the entire experience to knowledge and skills already meaningful to the learner. But of course, no one said

that teaching is easy!

Implications of constructivism for teaching

Fortunately there are strategies that teachers can use for giving students this kind of help—in fact they constitute

a major portion of this book, and are a major theme throughout the entire preservice teacher education programs.

For now, let me just point briefly to two of them, saving a complete discussion for later. One strategy that teachers

often find helpful is to organize the content to be learned as systematically as possible, because doing this allows the

teacher to select and devise learning activities that are more effective. One of the most widely used frameworks for

organizing content, for example, is a classification scheme proposed by the educator Benjamin Bloom, published

with the somewhat imposing title of Taxonomy of Educational Objectives: Handbook #1: Cognitive Domain

(Bloom, et al., 1956; Anderson & Krathwohl, 2001). Bloom’s taxonomy, as it is usually called, describes six kinds

of learning goals that teachers can in principle expect from students, ranging from simple recall of knowledge to

complex evaluation of knowledge. (The levels are defined briefly in Table 2.3 with examples from Goldilocks and

the Three Bears.)

Bloom’s taxonomy makes useful distinctions among possible kinds of knowledge needed by students, and

therefore potentially helps in selecting activities that truly target students’ “zones of proximal development” in the

sense meant by Vygotsky. A student who knows few terms for the species studied in biology unit (a problem at

Bloom’s knowledge and comprehension levels), for example, may initially need support at remembering and

Educational Psychology 36 A Global Text

2. The learning process

defining the terms before he or she can make useful comparisons among species (Bloom’s analysis level).

Pinpointing the most appropriate learning activities to accomplish this objective remains the job of the teacher-

expert (that’s you), but the learning itself has to be accomplished by the student. Put in more social constructivist

terms, the teacher arranges a zone of proximal development that allows the student to compare species

successfully, but the student still has to construct or appropriate the comparisons for him or herself.

Table 4: Bloom’s taxonomy of educational objectives: cognitive domain

Category or type of

thinking

Definition Example

(with apologies to Goldilocks

and her bear friends!)

Knowledge Remembering or recalling facts,

information, or procedures

List three things Goldilocks did in

the three bears’ house.

Comprehension Understanding facts, interpreting

information

Explain why Goldilocks liked the

little bear’s chair the best.

Application Using concepts in new situations,

solving particular problems

Predict some of the things that

Goldilocks might have used if she

had entered your house.

Analysis Distinguish parts of information, a

concept, or a procedure

Select the part of the story where

Goldilocks seemed most

comfortable.

Synthesis Combining elements or parts into a new

object, idea, or procedure

Tell how the story would have been

different if it had been about three

fishes.

Evaluation Assessing and judging the value or

ideas, objects, or materials in a

particular situation

Decide whether Goldilocks was a

bad girl, and justify your position.

A second strategy may be coupled with the first. As students gain experience as students, they become able to

think about how they themselves learn best, and you (as the teacher) can encourage such self-reflection as one of

your goals for their learning. These changes allow you to transfer some of your responsibilities for arranging

learning to the students themselves. For the biology student mentioned above, for example, you may be able not

only to plan activities that support comparing species, but also to devise ways for the student to think about how he

or she might learn the same information independently. The resulting self-assessment and self-direction of learning

often goes by the name of metacognition—an ability to think about and regulate one’s own thinking (Israel,

2005). Metacognition can sometimes be difficult for students to achieve, but it is an important goal for social

constructivist learning because it gradually frees learners from dependence on expert teachers to guide their

learning. Reflective learners, you might say, become their own expert guides. Like with using Bloom’s taxonomy,

37

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

though, promoting metacognition and self-directed learning is important enough that I will come back to it later in

more detail (especially in Chapter 9, “Facilitating complex thinking”).

By assigning a more visible role to expert helpers—and by implication also to teachers—than does the

psychological constructivism, social constructivism is seemingly more complete as a description of what teachers

usually do in classrooms, and of what they usually hope students will experience there. As we will see in the next

chapter, however, there are more uses to a theory than whether it describes the moment-to-moment interactions

between teacher and students. As I explain there, some theories can be helpful for planning instruction rather than

for doing it. It turns out that this is the case for psychological constructivism, which offers important ideas about

the appropriate sequencing of learning and development. This fact makes the psychological constructivism valuable

in its own way, even though it (and a few other learning theories as well) seem to “omit” mentioning teachers,

parents, or experts in detail. So do not make up your mind about the relative merits of different learning theories

yet!

Chapter summary

Although the term learning has many possible meanings, the term as used by teachers emphasizes its

relationship to curriculum, to teaching, and to the issues of sequencing, readiness, and transfer. Viewed in this

light, the two major psychological perspectives of learning—behaviorist and constructivist—have important ideas to

offer educators. Within the behaviorist perspective are two major theories or models of learning, called respondent

conditioning and operant conditioning. Respondent conditioning describes how previously neutral associations can

acquire the power to elicit significant responses in students. Operant conditioning describes how the consequences

and cues for a behavior can cause the behavior to become more frequent. In either case, from a teacher’s point of

view, the learned behaviors or responses can be either desirable or unwanted.

The other major psychological perspective—constructivism—describes how individuals build or “construct”

knowledge by engaging actively with their experiences. The psychological version of constructivism emphasizes the

learners’ individual responses to experience—their tendency both to assimilate it and to accommodate to it. The

social version of constructivism emphasizes how other, more expert individuals can create opportunities for the

learner to construct new knowledge. Social constructivism suggests that a teacher’s role must include deliberate

instructional planning, such as facilitated by Bloom’s taxonomy of learning objectives, but also that teachers need to

encourage metacognition, which is students’ ability to monitor their own learning.

On the Internet

This is the website for the Journal of Applied Behavior

Analysis, and as such it is an excellent source of examples of how behaviorist learning principles can be applied to a

wide variety of behavior-related difficulties. Any article older than one year is available in full-text, free of charge

from the website. (If it is from the most recent three issues, however, you have to subscribe to the journal.)

This is the website for the Jean Piaget Society, which in spite of its name is not just about

Piaget, but about all forms of constructivist research about learning and development, including social

constructivist versions. They have excellent brief publications about this perspective, available free of charge at the

website, as well as information about how to find additional information.

Educational Psychology 38 A Global Text

2. The learning process

Key termsAppropriate (verb)Behaviorism

Bloom’s taxonomyClassical conditioning

ConstructivismPsychological constructivism

John DeweyJean Piaget

AssimilationAccommodation

EquilibriumSchema

Social constructivismJerome Bruner

Instructional scaffoldingLev Vygotsky

Zone of proximal developmentDiscrimination

Extinction

Extrinsic motivationGeneralization

LearningIntrinsic motivation

MetacognitionOperant conditioning

CueOperant

ReinforcementSchedule of reinforcement

Ivan PavlovReadiness

Respondent conditioningConditioned response

Conditioned stimulusUnconditioned response

Unconditioned stimulusB. F. Skinner

Transfer

References

Alberto, P. & Troutman, A. (2005). Applied behavior analysis for teachers, 7th edition. Upper Saddle River,

NJ: Prentice Hall.

Anderson, L. & Krathwohl, D. (Eds.). (2001). A taxonomy for learning, teaching, and assessing: A revision

of Bloom’s taxonomy of educational objectives. New York: Longman.

Bruner, J. (1960). The process of education. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Bruner, J. (1966). Toward a theory of instruction. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Bruner, J. (1996). The culture of education. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Copple, C. & Bredekamp, S. (2006). Basics of developmentally appropriate practice. Washington, D.C.:

National Association for the Education of Young Children.

Dewey, J. (1938/1998). How we think. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

Ferster, C., Skinner, B. F., Cheney, C., Morse, W., & Dews, D. Schedules of reinforcement. New York: Copley

Publishing Group.

Fosnot, C. (Ed.). (2005). Constructivism: Theory, perspectives, and practice, 2nd edition. New York:

Teachers College Press.

Gardner, H. (1999). Intelligence reframed: Multiple intelligences for the 21st century. New York: Basic

Books.

Gardner, H. (2006). The development and education of the mind. New York: Routledge.

39

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Goldman, J. (2006). Web-based designed activities for young people in health education: A constructivist

approach. Health Education Journal 65(1), 14-27.

Gruber, H. & Voneche, J. (Eds.). (1995). The essential Piaget. New York: Basic Books.

Israel, S. (Ed.). (2005). Metacognition in literacy learning. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Lavond, D. & Steinmetz, J. (2003). Handbook of classical conditioning. Boston: Kluwer Academic

Publishing.

Mazur, J. (2005). Learning and behavior, 6th edition. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Onslow, M., Menzies, R., & Packman, A. (2001). An operant intervention for early stuttering. Behavior

modification 25(1), 116-139.

Pavlov, I. (1927). Conditioned reflexes. London, UK: Oxford University Press.

Piaget, J. (2001). The psychology of intelligence. London, UK: Routledge.

Rockmore, T. (2005). On constructivist epistemology. Lanham, MD: Rowman & Littlefield Publishers.

Salkind, N. (2004). An introduction to theories of human development. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage

Publications.

Skinner, B. F. (1938). The behavior of organisms. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

Skinner, B. F. (1948). Walden Two. New York: Macmillan.

Skinner, B. F. (1988). The selection of behavior: The operant behaviorism of B. F. Skinner. New York:

Cambridge University Press.

Tharp, R. & Gallimore, R. (1991). Rousing minds to life: Teaching, learning, and schooling in social context.

Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Vygotsky, L. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA:

Harvard University Press.

Educational Psychology 40 A Global Text

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

3. Student developmentWhen one of our authors (Kelvin Seifert) was growing up, he was provided with piano lessons. Daily

practice was a staple of childhood–365 days a year, and in a home that was deliberately kept quiet

to facilitate practice. Music—especially the piano—defined a major part of his emerging self-identity.

Altogether he studied piano for 13 years, from age 4 to the end of high school, with only occasional

interruptions.

At any one time, Kelvin witnessed small changes in his skills. He performed a simple piece a bit

better than he had the previous week, or he played more of it from memory. There were direct,

obvious connections between his skills at one moment and at the moment just before or after. Back

then, if you had asked him what accounted for the changes, he would have stated without hesitation

that they were because he was “learning” specific piano pieces.

Across broader spans of time, however, he noticed changes that were more dramatic. Kelvin learned

much more complex pieces than he had several years earlier, for example. He also played with

significantly more “finesse”, sensitivity and polish than as a young child. He was even listening to

classical music on the radio some of the time! Kelvin's musical talent became transformed over the

long term, and in some sense he did not have the “same” talent that he had had as a beginner.

If you had asked what accounted for these longer-term changes, he would have had a harder time

answering than when asked about the short-term changes. He might have said simply and a bit

vaguely: “I have been getting better at piano.” If you ask the same question now, however, he would

say that his music skills had developed, that their development had been slow and gradual, and

that the changes resulted not just from simple practice, but also from becoming more widely skilled

about music in general.

Development refers to long-term personal changes that have multiple sources and multiple effects. It is like

the difference between Kelvin's music at age fifteen compared to his music at age five, rather than the difference

between his music one week and his music the next. Some human developments are especially broad and take years

to unfold fully; a person's ever-evolving ability to “read” other's moods, for example, may take a lifetime to develop

fully. Other developments are faster and more focused, like a person's increasing skill at solving crossword puzzles.

The faster and simpler is the change, the more likely we are to call the change “learning” instead of development.

The difference between learning and development is a matter of degree. When a child learns to name the planets of

the solar system, for example, the child may not need a lot of time, nor does the learning involve a multitude of

experiences. So it is probably better to think of that particular experience—learning to name the planets—as an

example of learning rather than of development (Salkind, 2004; Lewis, 1997).

Educational Psychology 41 A Global Text

3. Student development

Why development matters

Students’ development matters for teachers, but the way it matters depends partly on how schooling is

organized. In teaching a single, “self-contained” grade-level, the benefits of knowing about development will be less

explicit, but just as real, as if you teach many grade levels. Working exclusively with a single grade (like, say, a third-

grade classroom) highlights differences among students that happen in spite of their similar ages, and obscures

similarities that happen because of having similar ages. Under these conditions it is still easy to notice students’

diversity, but harder to know how much of it comes from differences in long-term development, compared to

differences in short-term experiences. Knowledge about long term changes is still useful, however, in planning

appropriate activities and in holding appropriate expectations about students. What changes in students can you

expect relatively soon simply from your current program of activities, and which ones may take a year or more to

show up? This is a question that developmental psychology can help to answer.

If you teach multiple grade levels, as often is true of specialists or teachers in middle school or high school, then

your need for developmental knowledge will be more obvious because you will confront wide age differences on a

daily basis. As a physical education teacher, for example, you may teach kindergarten children at one time during

the day, but sixth-graders at another time, or teach seventh-graders at one time but twelfth-graders at another.

Students will differ more obviously because of age, in addition to differing because of other factors like their skills

or knowledge learned recently. Nonetheless, the instructional challenge will be the same as the one faced by

teachers of single-grade classes: you will want to know what activities and expectations are appropriate for your

students. To answer this question, you will need to know something not only about how your students are unique,

but also about general trends of development during childhood and adolescence.

Note that developmental trends vary in two important ways. The first, as indicated already, is in their generality.

Some theories or models of development boldly assert that certain changes happen to virtually every person on the

planet, and often at relatively predictable points in life. For example, a theory might assert that virtually every

toddler acquires a spoken language, or that every teenager forms a sense of personal identity. Individuals who do

not experience these developments would be rare, though not necessarily disabled as a result. Other theories

propose developmental changes that are more limited, claiming only that the changes happen to some people or

only under certain conditions. Developing a female gender role, for example, does not happen to everyone, but only

to the females in a population, and the details vary according to the family, community, or society in which a child

lives.

The second way that developmental trends vary is in how strictly they are sequenced and hierarchical. In some

views of development, changes are thought to happen in a specific order and to build on each other—sort of a

“staircase” model of development (Case, 1991, 1996). For example, a developmental psychologist (and many of the

rest of us) might argue that young people must have tangible, hands-on experience with new materials before they

can reason about the materials in the abstract. The order cannot be reversed. In other views of development, change

happens, but not with a sequence or end point that is uniform. This sort of change is more like a “kaleidoscope”

than a staircase (Levinson, 1990; Lewis, 1997; Harris, 2006). A person who becomes permanently disabled, for

example, may experience complex long-term changes in personal values and priorities that are different both in

timing and content from most people's developmental pathway.

42

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

In general, educational psychologists have tended to emphasize explanations of development that are relatively

general, universal and sequential, rather than specific to particular cultures or that are unsequenced and

kaleidoscopic (see, for example, Woolfolk, 2006, Chapter 3; or Slavin, 2005, Chapters 8 and 9). Such models

(sometimes called “grand theories”) have the advantage of concisely integrating many features of development,

while also describing the kind of people children or adolescents usually end up to be. The preference for integrative

perspectives makes sense given educators’ need to work with and teach large numbers of diverse students both

efficiently and effectively. But the approach also risks overgeneralizing or oversimplifying the experiences of

particular children and youth. It can also confuse what does happen as certain children (like the middle-class ones)

develop with what should happen to children. To understand this point, imagine two children of about the same age

who have dramatically very different childhood experiences—for example, one who grows up in poverty and

another who grows up financially well-off. In what sense can we say that these two children experience the same

underlying developmental changes as they grow up? And how much should they even be expected to do so?

Developmental psychology, and especially the broad theories of developmental psychology, highlight the

“sameness” or common ground between these two children. As such, it serves as counterpoint to knowledge of their

obvious uniqueness, and places their uniqueness in broader perspective.

Physical development during the school years

Although it may be tempting to think that physical development is the concern of physical education teachers

only, it is actually a foundation for many academic tasks. In first grade, for example, it is important to know

whether children can successfully manipulate a pencil. In later grades, it is important to know how long students

can be expected to sit still without discomfort—a real physical challenge. In all grades, it is important to have a

sense of students’ health needs related to their age or maturity, if only to know who may become ill, and with what

illness, and to know what physical activities are reasonable and needed.

Trends in height and weight

Typical height and weight for well-nourished, healthy students are shown in Table 9. The figure shows averages

for several ages from preschool through the end of high school. But the table does not show the diversity among

children. At age 6, for example, when children begin school, the average boy or girl is about 115 centimeters tall, but

some are 109 and others are 125 centimeters. Average weight at age 6 is about 20 kilograms, but ranges between

about 16 and 24 kilograms—about 20% variation in either direction.

Table 5: Average height and weight of well-nourished children

Age Height (cm) Weight (kg)

2 85 7.0

6 115 20.0

10 135 31.0

14 162 52.0

18 169 60.5

Educational Psychology 43 A Global Text

3. Student development

There are other points to keep in mind about average height and weight that are not evident from Table 9. The

first is that boys and girls, on average, are quite similar in height and weight during childhood, but diverge in the

early teenage years, when they reach puberty. For a time (approximately age 10-14), the average girl is taller, but

not much heavier, than the average boy. After that the average boy becomes both taller and heavier than the

average girl—though there remain individual exceptions (Malina, et al., 2004). The pre-teen difference can

therefore be awkward for some children and youth, at least among those who aspire to looking like older teenagers

or young adults. For young teens less concerned with “image”, though, the fact that girls are taller may not be

especially important, or even noticed (Friedman, 2000).

A second point is that as children get older, individual differences in weight diverge more radically than

differences in height. Among 18-year-olds, the heaviest youngsters weigh almost twice as much as the lightest, but

the tallest ones are only about 10 per cent taller than the shortest. Nonetheless, both height and weight can be

sensitive issues for some teenagers. Most modern societies (and the teenagers in them) tend to favor relatively short

women and tall men, as well as a somewhat thin body build, especially for girls and women. Yet neither “socially

correct” height nor thinness is the destiny for many individuals. Being overweight, in particular, has become a

common, serious problem in modern society (Tartamella, et al., 2004) due to the prevalence of diets high in fat and

lifestyles low in activity. The educational system has unfortunately contributed to the problem as well, by gradually

restricting the number of physical education courses and classes in the past two decades.

The third point to keep in mind is that average height and weight is related somewhat to racial and ethnic

background. In general, children of Asian background tend to be slightly shorter than children of European and

North American background. The latter in turn tend to be shorter than children from African societies (Eveleth &

Tanner, 1990). Body shape differs slightly as well, though the differences are not always visible until after puberty.

Asian youth tend to have arms and legs that are a bit short relative to their torsos, and African youth tend to have

relatively long arms and legs. The differences are only averages; there are large individual differences as well, and

these tend to be more relevant for teachers to know about than broad group differences.

Puberty and its effects on students

A universal physical development in students is puberty, which is the set of changes in early adolescence that

bring about sexual maturity. Along with internal changes in reproductive organs are outward changes such as

growth of breasts in girls and the penis in boys, as well as relatively sudden increases in height and weight. By about

age 10 or 11, most children experience increased sexual attraction to others (usually heterosexual, though not

always) that affects social life both in school and out (McClintock & Herdt, 1996). By the end of high school, more

than half of boys and girls report having experienced sexual intercourse at least once—though it is hard to be

certain of the proportion because of the sensitivity and privacy of the information. (Center for Disease Control,

2004b; Rosenbaum, 2006).

At about the same time that puberty accentuates gender, role differences also accentuate for at least some

teenagers. Some girls who excelled at math or science in elementary school may curb their enthusiasm and displays

of success at these subjects for fear of limiting their popularity or attractiveness as girls (Taylor & Gilligan, 1995;

Sadker, 2004). Some boys who were not especially interested in sports previously may begin dedicating themselves

to athletics to affirm their masculinity in the eyes of others. Some boys and girls who once worked together

44

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

successfully on class projects may no longer feel comfortable doing so—or alternatively may now seek to be working

partners, but for social rather than academic reasons. Such changes do not affect all youngsters equally, nor affect

any one youngster equally on all occasions. An individual student may act like a young adult on one day, but more

like a child the next. When teaching children who are experiencing puberty, , teachers need to respond flexibly and

supportively.

Development of motor skills

Students’ fundamental motor skills are already developing when they begin kindergarten, but are not yet

perfectly coordinated. Five-year-olds generally can walk satisfactorily for most school-related purposes (if they

could not, schools would have to be organized very differently!). For some fives, running still looks a bit like a

hurried walk, but usually it becomes more coordinated within a year or two. Similarly with jumping, throwing, and

catching: most children can do these things, though often clumsily, by the time they start school, but improve their

skills noticeably during the early elementary years (Payne & Isaacs, 2005). Assisting such developments is usually

the job either of physical education teachers, where they exist, or else of classroom teachers during designated

physical education activities. Whoever is responsible, it is important to notice if a child does not keep more-or-less

to the usual developmental timetable, and to arrange for special assessment or supports if appropriate. Common

procedures for arranging for help are described in Chapter 5 (“Special education”).

Even if physical skills are not a special focus of a classroom teacher,, they can be quite important to students

themselves. Whatever their grade level, students who are clumsy are aware of that fact and how it could potentially

negatively effect respect from their peers. In the long term, self-consciousness and poor self-esteem can develop for

a child who is clumsy, especially if peers (or teachers and parents) place high value on success in athletics. One

research study found, for example, what teachers and coaches sometimes suspect: that losers in athletic

competitions tend to become less sociable and are more apt to miss subsequent athletic practices than winners

(Petlichkoff, 1996).

Health and illness

By world standards, children and youth in economically developed societies tend, on average, to be remarkably

healthy. Even so, much depends on precisely how well-off families are and on how much health care is available to

them. Children from higher-income families experience far fewer serious or life-threatening illnesses than children

fromlower-income families. Whatever their income level, parents and teachers often rightly note that children—

especially the youngest ones—get far more illnesses than do adults. In 2004, for example, a government survey

estimated that children get an average of 6-10 colds per year, but adults get only about 2-4 per year (National

Institute of Allergies and Infectious Diseases, 2004). The difference probably exists because children’s immune

systems are not as fully formed as adults’, and because children at school are continually exposed to other children,

many of whom may be contagious themselves. An indirect result of children’s frequent illnesses is that teachers

(along with airline flight attendants, incidentally!) also report more frequent minor illnesses than do adults in

general—about five colds per year, for example, instead of just 2-4 (Whelen, et al., 2005). The “simple” illnesses are

not life threatening, but they are responsible for many lost days of school, both for students and for teachers, as well

as days when a student may be present physically, but functions below par while simultaneously infecting

classmates. In these ways, learning and teaching often suffer because health is suffering.

Educational Psychology 45 A Global Text

3. Student development

The problem is not only the prevalence of illness as such (in winter, even in the United States, approximately

one person gets infected with a minor illness every few seconds), but the fact that illnesses are not distributed

uniformly among students, schools, or communities. Whether it is a simple cold or something more serious, illness

is particularly common where living conditions are crowded, where health care is scarce or unaffordable, and where

individuals live with frequent stresses of any kind. Often, but not always, these are the circumstances of poverty.

Table 6 summarizes these effects for a variety of health problems, not just for colds or flu.

Table 6: Health effects of children's economic level

Health program Comparison: poor vs non-poor

Delayed immunizations 3 times higher

Asthma Somewhat higher

Lead poisoning 3 times higher

Deaths in childhood from accidents 2-3 times higher

Deaths in childhood from disease 3-4 times higher

Having a condition that limits school activity 2-3 times higher

Days sick in bed 40 per cent higher

Seriously impaired vision 2-3 times higher

Severe iron-deficiency (anemia) 2 times higher

Source: Richardson, J> (2005). The Cost of Being Poor. New York: Praeger. Spencer, N. (2000). Poverty and

Child Health, 2nd edition. Abington, UK: Radcliffe Medical Press. Allender, J. (2005). Community Health

Nursing. Philadelphia: Lippinsott, Williams & Wilkins.

As students get older, illnesses become less frequent, but other health risks emerge. The most widespread is the

consumption of alcohol and the smoking of cigarettes. As of 2004, about 75 per cent of teenagers reported drinking

an alcoholic beverage at least occasionally, and 22 per cent reported smoking cigarettes (Center for Disease Control,

2004a). The good news is that these proportions show a small, but steady decline in the frequencies over the past 10

years or so. The bad news is that teenagers also show increases in the abuse of some prescription drugs, such as

inhalants, that act as stimulants (Johnston, et al., 2006). As with the prevalence of illnesses, the prevalence of drug

use is not uniform, with a relatively small fraction of individuals accounting for a disproportionate proportion of

usage. One survey, for example, found that a teenager was 3-5 times more likely to smoke or to use alcohol, smoke

marijuana, or use drugs if he or she has a sibling who has also indulged these habits (Fagan & Najman, 2005).

Siblings, it seems, are more influential in this case than parents.

Cognitive development: the theory of Jean Piaget

Cognition refers to thinking and memory processes, and cognitive development refers to long-term

changes in these processes. One of the most widely known perspectives about cognitive development is the

46

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

cognitive stage theory of a Swiss psychologist named Jean Piaget. Piaget created and studied an account of how

children and youth gradually become able to think logically and scientifically. Because his theory is especially

popular among educators, we focus on it in this chapter. We will look at other cognitive perspectives—ones that are

not as fully “developmental”, in later chapters, especially Chapter 9 (“Facilitating complex thinking”).

In brief comments in Chapter 2 (see “Psychological constructivism”) about how Piaget explained learning, we

described Piaget as a psychological constructivist: in his view, learning proceeded by the interplay of assimilation

(adjusting new experiences to fit prior concepts) and accommodation (adjusting concepts to fit new experiences).

The to-and-fro of these two processes leads not only to short-term learning, as pointed out in Chapter 1, but also to

long-term developmental change. The long-term developments are really the main focus of Piaget’s cognitive

theory.

After observing children closely, Piaget proposed that cognition developed through distinct stages from birth

through the end of adolescence. By stages he meant a sequence of thinking patterns with four key features:

1. They always happen in the same order.

2. No stage is ever skipped.

3. Each stage is a significant transformation of the stage before it.

4. Each later stage incorporated the earlier stages into itself. Basically this is the “staircase” model of

development mentioned at the beginning of this chapter. Piaget proposed four major stages of cognitive

development, and called them (1) sensorimotor intelligence, (2) preoperational thinking, (3) concrete

operational thinking, and (4) formal operational thinking. Each stage is correlated with an age period of

childhood, but only approximately.

The sensorimotor stage: birth to age 2

In Piaget’s theory, the sensorimotor stage is first, and is defined as the period when infants “think” by means

of their senses and motor actions. As every new parent will attest, infants continually touch, manipulate, look, listen

to, and even bite and chew objects. According to Piaget, these actions allow them to learn about the world and are

crucial to their early cognitive development.

The infant’s actions allow the child to represent (or construct simple concepts of) objects and events. A toy

animal may be just a confusing array of sensations at first, but by looking, feeling, and manipulating it repeatedly,

the child gradually organizes her sensations and actions into a stable concept, toy animal. The representation

acquires a permanence lacking in the individual experiences of the object, which are constantly changing. Because

the representation is stable, the child “knows”, or at least believes, that toy animal exists even if the actual toy

animal is temporarily out of sight. Piaget called this sense of stability object permanence, a belief that objects

exist whether or not they are actually present. It is a major achievement of sensorimotor development, and marks a

qualitative transformation in how older infants (24 months) think about experience compared to younger infants (6

months).

During much of infancy, of course, a child can only barely talk, so sensorimotor development initially happens

without the support of language. It might therefore seem hard to know what infants are thinking, but Piaget devised

several simple, but clever experiments to get around their lack of language, and that suggest that infants do indeed

Educational Psychology 47 A Global Text

3. Student development

represent objects even without being able to talk (Piaget, 1952). In one, for example, he simply hid an object (like a

toy animal) under a blanket. He found that doing so consistently prompts older infants (18-24 months) to search

for the object, but fails to prompt younger infants (less than six months) to do so. (You can try this experiment

yourself if you happen to have access to young infant.) “Something” motivates the search by the older infant even

without the benefit of much language, and the “something” is presumed to be a permanent concept or

representation of the object.

The preoperational stage: age 2 to 7

In the preoperational stage, children use their new ability to represent objects in a wide variety of activities,

but they do not yet do it in ways that are organized or fully logical. One of the most obvious examples of this kind of

cognition is dramatic play, the improvised make-believe of preschool children. If you have ever had responsibility

for children of this age, you have likely witnessed such play. Ashley holds a plastic banana to her ear and says:

“Hello, Mom? Can you be sure to bring me my baby doll? OK!” Then she hangs up the banana and pours tea for

Jeremy into an invisible cup. Jeremy giggles at the sight of all of this and exclaims: “Rinnng! Oh Ashley, the phone

is ringing again! You better answer it.” And on it goes.

In a way, children immersed in make-believe seem “mentally insane”, in that they do not think realistically. But

they are not truly insane because they have not really taken leave of their senses. At some level, Ashley and Jeremy

always know that the banana is still a banana and not really a telephone; they are merely representing it as a

telephone. They are thinking on two levels at once—one imaginative and the other realistic. This dual processing of

experience makes dramatic play an early example of metacognition, or reflecting on and monitoring of thinking

itself. As we explained in Chapter 2, metacognition is a highly desirable skill for success in school, one that teachers

often encourage (Bredekamp & Copple, 1997; Paley, 2005). Partly for this reason, teachers of young children

(preschool, kindergarten, and even first or second grade) often make time and space in their classrooms for

dramatic play, and sometimes even participate in it themselves to help develop the play further.

The concrete operational stage: age 7 to 11

As children continue into elementary school, they become able to represent ideas and events more flexibly and

logically. Their rules of thinking still seem very basic by adult standards and usually operate unconsciously, but they

allow children to solve problems more systematically than before, and therefore to be successful with many

academic tasks. In the concrete operational stage, for example, a child may unconsciously follow the rule: “If

nothing is added or taken away, then the amount of something stays the same.” This simple principle helps children

to understand certain arithmetic tasks, such as in adding or subtracting zero from a number, as well as to do certain

classroom science experiments, such as ones involving judgments of the amounts of liquids when mixed. Piaget

called this period the concrete operational stage because children mentally “operate” on concrete objects and

events. They are not yet able, however, to operate (or think) systematically about representations of objects or

events. Manipulating representations is a more abstract skill that develops later, during adolescence.

Concrete operational thinking differs from preoperational thinking in two ways, each of which renders children

more skilled as students. One difference is reversibility, or the ability to think about the steps of a process in any

order. Imagine a simple science experiment, for example, such as one that explores why objects sink or float by

having a child place an assortment of objects in a basin of water. Both the preoperational and concrete operational

48

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

child can recall and describe the steps in this experiment, but only the concrete operational child can recall them in

any order. This skill is very helpful on any task involving multiple steps—a common feature of tasks in the

classroom. In teaching new vocabulary from a story, for another example, a teacher might tell students: “First

make a list of words in the story that you do not know, then find and write down their definitions, and finally get a

friend to test you on your list”. These directions involve repeatedly remembering to move back and forth between a

second step and a first—a task that concrete operational students—and most adults—find easy, but that

preoperational children often forget to do or find confusing. If the younger children are to do this task reliably, they

may need external prompts, such as having the teacher remind them periodically to go back to the story to look for

more unknown words.

The other new feature of thinking during the concrete operational stage is the child’s ability to decenter, or

focus on more than one feature of a problem at a time. There are hints of decentration in preschool children’s

dramatic play, which requires being aware on two levels at once—knowing that a banana can be both a banana and

a “telephone”. But the decentration of the concrete operational stage is more deliberate and conscious than

preschoolers’ make-believe. Now the child can attend to two things at once quite purposely. Suppose you give

students a sheet with an assortment of subtraction problems on it, and ask them to do this: “Find all of the

problems that involve two-digit subtraction and that involve borrowing’ from the next column. Circle and solve

only those problems.” Following these instructions is quite possible for a concrete operational student (as long as

they have been listening!) because the student can attend to the two subtasks simultaneously—finding the two-digit

problems and identifying which actually involve borrowing. (Whether the student actually knows how to “borrow”

however, is a separate question.)

In real classroom tasks, reversibility and decentration often happen together. A well-known example of joint

presence is Piaget’s experiments with conservation, the belief that an amount or quantity stays the same even if it

changes apparent size or shape (Piaget, 2001; Matthews, 1998). Imagine two identical balls made of clay. Any child,

whether preoperational or concrete operational, will agree that the two indeed have the same amount of clay in

them simply because they look the same. But if you now squish one ball into a long, thin “hot dog”, the

preoperational child is likely to say that the amount of that ball has changed—either because it is longer or because

it is thinner, but at any rate because it now looks different. The concrete operational child will not make this

mistake, thanks to new cognitive skills of reversibility and decentration: for him or her, the amount is the same

because “you could squish it back into a ball again” (reversibility) and because “it may be longer, but it is also

thinner” (decentration). Piaget would say the concrete operational child “has conservation of quantity”.

The classroom examples described above also involve reversibility and decentration. As already mentioned, the

vocabulary activity described earlier requires reversibility (going back and forth between identifying words and

looking up their meanings); but it can also be construed as an example of decentration (keeping in mind two tasks

at once—word identification and dictionary search). And as mentioned, the arithmetic activity requires

decentration (looking for problems that meet two criteria and also solving them), but it can also be construed as an

example of reversibility (going back and forth between subtasks, as with the vocabulary activity). Either way, the

development of concrete operational skills support students in doing many basic academic tasks; in a sense they

make ordinary schoolwork possible.

Educational Psychology 49 A Global Text

3. Student development

The formal operational stage: age 11 and beyond

In the last of the Piagetian stages, the child becomes able to reason not only about tangible objects and events,

but also about hypothetical or abstract ones. Hence it has the name formal operational stage—the period when

the individual can “operate” on “forms” or representations. With students at this level, the teacher can pose

hypothetical (or contrary-to-fact) problems: “What if the world had never discovered oil?” or “What if the first

European explorers had settled first in California instead of on the East Coast of the United States?” To answer such

questions, students must use hypothetical reasoning, meaning that they must manipulate ideas that vary in

several ways at once, and do so entirely in their minds.

The hypothetical reasoning that concerned Piaget primarily involved scientific problems. His studies of formal

operational thinking therefore often look like problems that middle or high school teachers pose in science classes.

In one problem, for example, a young person is presented with a simple pendulum, to which different amounts of

weight can be hung (Inhelder & Piaget, 1958). The experimenter asks: “What determines how fast the pendulum

swings: the length of the string holding it, the weight attached to it, or the distance that it is pulled to the side?” The

young person is not allowed to solve this problem by trial-and-error with the materials themselves, but must reason

a way to the solution mentally. To do so systematically, he or she must imagine varying each factor separately, while

also imagining the other factors that are held constant. This kind of thinking requires facility at manipulating

mental representations of the relevant objects and actions—precisely the skill that defines formal operations.

As you might suspect, students with an ability to think hypothetically have an advantage in many kinds of school

work: by definition, they require relatively few “props” to solve problems. In this sense they can in principle be

more self-directed than students who rely only on concrete operations—certainly a desirable quality in the opinion

of most teachers. Note, though, that formal operational thinking is desirable but not sufficient for school success,

and that it is far from being the only way that students achieve educational success. Formal thinking skills do not

insure that a student is motivated or well-behaved, for example, nor does it guarantee other desirable skills, such as

ability at sports, music, or art. The fourth stage in Piaget’s theory is really about a particular kind of formal

thinking, the kind needed to solve scientific problems and devise scientific experiments. Since many people do not

normally deal with such problems in the normal course of their lives, it should be no surprise that research finds

that many people never achieve or use formal thinking fully or consistently, or that they use it only in selected areas

with which they are very familiar (Case & Okomato, 1996). For teachers, the limitations of Piaget's ideas suggest a

need for additional theories about development—ones that focus more directly on the social and interpersonal

issues of childhood and adolescence. The next sections describe some of these.

Social development: relationships,personal motives, and morality

Social development refers to the long-term changes in relationships and interactions involving self, peers,

and family. It includes both positive changes, such as how friendships develop, and negative changes, such as

aggression or bullying. The social developments that are the most obviously relevant to classroom life fall into three

main areas: (1) changes in self-concept and in relationships among students and teachers, (2) changes in basic

needs or personal motives, and (3) changes in sense of rights and responsibilities. As with cognitive development,

each of these areas has a broad, well-known theory (and theorist) that provides a framework for thinking about how

the area relates to teaching. For development of self-concept and relationships, it is the theory of Erik Erikson;

for development of personal motives, it is the theory of Abraham Maslow; and for development of ethical

50

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

knowledge and beliefs, it is the work of Lawrence Kohlberg and his critic, Carol Gilligan. Their theories are

definitely not the only ones related to social development of students, and their ideas are often debated by other

researchers. But their accounts do explain much about social development that is relevant to teaching and

education.

Erik Erikson: eight psychosocial crises of development

Like Piaget, Erik Erikson developed a theory of social development that relies on stages, except that Erikson

thought of stages as a series of psychological or social (or psychosocial) crises—turning points in a person’s

relationships and feelings about himself or herself (Erikson, 1963, 1980). Each crisis consists of a dilemma or

choice that carries both advantages and risks, but in which one choice or alternative is normally considered more

desirable or “healthy”. How one crisis is resolved affects how later crises are resolved. The resolution also helps to

create an individual’s developing personality. Erikson proposed eight crises that extend from birth through old age;

they are summarized in Table 7. Four of the stages occur during the school years, so we give these special attention

here, but it is helpful also to know what crises are thought to come both before and after those in the school years.

Table 7: Eight psychosocial crises according to Erikson

Psychosocial crisis Approximate age Description

Trust and mistrust Birth to one year Development of trust between caregiver and child

Autonomy and shame Age 1-3 Development of control over bodily functions and activities

Initiative and guilt Age 3-6 Testing limits of self-assertion and purposefulness

Industry and inferiority Age 6-12 Development of sense of mastery and competence

Identity and role confusion Age 12-19 Development of identity and acknowledge of identity by others

Intimacy and isolation Age 19-25+ Formation of intimate relationships and commitments

Generativity and

stagnation

Age 25-50+ Development of creative or productive activities that

contribute to future generations

Integrity and despair Age 50+ Acceptance of personal life history and forgiveness of self and

others

Crises of infants and preschoolers: trust, autonomy, and initiative

Almost from the day they are born, infants face a crisis (in Erikson’s sense) about trust and mistrust. They

are happiest if they can eat, sleep, and excrete according to their own physiological schedules, regardless of whether

their schedules are convenient for the caregiver (often the mother). Unfortunately, though, a young infant is in no

position to control or influence a mother’s care giving or scheduling needs; so the baby faces a dilemma about how

much to trust or mistrust the mother’s helpfulness. It is as if the baby asks, “If I demand food (or sleep or a clean

diaper) now, will my mother actually be able to help me meet this need?” Hopefully, between the two of them,

mother and child resolve this choice in favor of the baby's trust: the mother proves herself at least “good enough” in

her attentiveness, and the baby risks trusting mother's motivation and skill at care giving.

Educational Psychology 51 A Global Text

3. Student development

Almost as soon as this crisis is resolved, however, a new one develops over the issue of autonomy and shame.

The child (who is now a toddler) may now trust his or her caregiver (mother), but the very trust contributes to a

desire to assert autonomy by taking care of basic personal needs, such as feeding, toileting, or dressing. Given the

child’s lack of experience in these activities, however, self-care is risky at first—the toddler may feed (or toilet or

dress) clumsily and ineffectively. The child’s caregiver, for her part, risks overprotecting the child and criticizing his

early efforts unnecessarily and thus causing the child to feel shame for even trying. Hopefully, as with the earlier

crisis of trust, the new crisis gets resolved in favor of autonomy through the combined efforts of the child to exercise

autonomy and of the caregiver to support the child’s efforts.

Eventually, about the time a child is of preschool age, the autonomy exercised during the previous period

becomes more elaborate, extended, and focused on objects and people other than the child and basic physical

needs. The child at a day care center may now undertake, for example, to build the “biggest city in the world” out of

all available unit blocks—even if other children want some of the blocks for themselves. The child’s projects and

desires create a new crisis of initiative and guilt, because the child soon realizes that acting on impulses or

desires can sometimes have negative effects on others—more blocks for the child may mean fewer for someone else.

As with the crisis over autonomy, caregivers have to support the child’s initiatives where possible, but also not make

the child feel guilty just for desiring to have or to do something that affects others' welfare. By limiting behavior

where necessary but not limiting internal feelings, the child can develop a lasting ability to take initiative. Expressed

in Erikson’s terms, the crisis is then resolved in favor of initiative.

Even though only the last of these three crises overlaps with the school years, all three relate to issues faced by

students of any age, and even by their teachers. A child or youth who is fundamentally mistrustful, for example, has

a serious problem in coping with school life. If you are a student, it is essential for your long-term survival to believe

that teachers and school officials have your best interests at heart, and that they are not imposing assignments or

making rules, for example, “just for the heck of it.” Even though students are not infants any more, teachers

function like Erikson’s caregiving parents in that they need to prove worthy of students’ trust through their initial

flexibility and attentiveness.

Parallels from the classroom also exist for the crises of autonomy and of initiative. To learn effectively, students

need to make choices and undertake academic initiatives at least some of the time, even though not every choice or

initiative may be practical or desirable. Teachers, for their part, need to make true choices and initiatives possible,

and refrain from criticizing, even accidentally, a choice or intention behind an initiative even if the teacher privately

believes that it is “bound to fail”. Support for choices and initiative should be focused on providing resources and on

guiding the student’s efforts toward more likely success. In these ways teachers function like parents of toddlers

and preschoolers in Erikson’s theory of development, regardless of the age of their students.

The crisis of childhood: industry and inferiority

Once into elementary school, the child is faced for the first time with becoming competent and worthy in the

eyes of the world at large, or more precisely in the eyes of classmates and teachers. To achieve their esteem, he or

she must develop skills that require effort that is sustained and somewhat focused. The challenge creates the crisis

of industry and inferiority. To be respected by teachers, for example, the child must learn to read and to behave

like a “true student”. To be respected by peers, he or she must learn to cooperate and to be friendly, among other

52

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

things. There are risks involved in working on these skills and qualities, because there can be no guarantee of

success with them in advance. If the child does succeed, therefore, he or she experiences the satisfaction of a job

well done and of skills well learned—a feeling that Eriks0n called industry. If not, however, the child risks feeling

lasting inferiority compared to others. Teachers therefore have a direct, explicit role in helping students to resolve

this crisis in favor of industry or success. They can set realistic academic goals for students—ones that tend to lead

to success—and then provide materials and assistance for students to reach their goals. Teachers can also express

their confidence that students can in fact meet their goals if and when the students get discouraged, and avoid

hinting (even accidentally) that a student is simply a “loser”. Paradoxically, these strategies will work best if the

teacher is also tolerant of less-than-perfect performance by students. Too much emphasis on perfection can

undermine some students’ confidence—foster Erikson’s inferiority—by making academic goals seem beyond reach.

The crisis of adolescence: identity and role confusion

As the child develops lasting talents and attitudes as a result of the crisis of industry, he begins to face a new

question: what do all the talents and attitudes add up to be? Who is the “me” embedded in this profile of qualities?

These questions are the crisis of identity and role confusion. Defining identity is riskier than it may appear for

a person simply because some talents and attitudes may be poorly developed, and some even may be undesirable in

the eyes of others. (If you are poor at math, how do you live with family and friends if they think you should be good

at this skill?) Still others may be valuable but fail to be noticed by other people. The result is that who a person

wants to be may not be the same as who he or she is in actual fact, nor the same as who other people want the

person to be. In Erikson's terms, role confusion is the result.

Teachers can minimize role confusion in a number of ways. One is to offer students lots of diverse role models—

by identifying models in students’ reading materials, for example, or by inviting diverse guests to school. The point

of these strategies would be to express a key idea: that there are many ways to be respected, successful, and satisfied

with life. Another way to support students’ identity development is to be alert to students’ confusions about their

futures, and refer them to counselors or other services outside school that can help sort these out. Still another

strategy is to tolerate changes in students’ goals and priorities—sudden changes in extra-curricular activities or in

personal plans after graduation. Since students are still trying roles out, discouraging experimentation may not be

in students’ best interests.

The crises of adulthood: intimacy, generativity, and integrity

Beyond the school years, according to Erikson, individuals continue psychosocial development by facing

additional crises. Young adults, for example, face a crisis of intimacy and isolation. This crisis is about the risk

of establishing close relationships with a select number of others. Whether the relationships are heterosexual,

homosexual, or not sexual at all, their defining qualities are depth and sustainability. Without them, an individual

risks feeling isolated. Assuming that a person resolves this crisis in favor of intimacy, however, he or she then faces

a crisis about generativity and stagnation. This crisis is characteristic of most of adulthood, and not

surprisingly therefore is about caring for or making a contribution to society, and especially to its younger

generation. Generativity is about making life productive and creative so that it matters to others. One obvious way

for some to achieve this feeling is by raising children, but there are also many other ways to contribute to the

welfare of others. The final crisis is about integrity and despair, and is characteristically felt during the final

years of life. At the end of life, a person is likely to review the past and to ask whether it has been lived as well as

Educational Psychology 53 A Global Text

3. Student development

possible, even if it was clearly not lived perfectly. Since personal history can no longer be altered at the end of life, it

is important to make peace with what actually happened and to forgive oneself and others for mistakes that may

have been made. The alternative is despair, or depression from believing not only that one’s life was lived badly, but

also that there is no longer any hope of correcting past mistakes.

Even though Erikson conceives of these crises as primarily concerns of adulthood, there are precursors of them

during the school years. Intimacy, for example, is a concern of many children and youth in that they often desire,

but do not always find, lasting relationships with others (Beidel, 2005; Zimbardo & Radl, 1999). Personal isolation

is a particular risk for students with disabilities, as well as for students whose cultural or racial backgrounds differ

from classmates’ or the teacher’s. Generativity—feeling helpful to others and to the young—is needed not only by

many adults, but also by many children and youth; when given the opportunity as part of their school program, they

frequently welcome a chance to be of authentic service to others as part of their school programs (Eyler & Giles,

1999; Kay, 2003). Integrity—taking responsibility for your personal past, “warts and all”, is often a felt need for

anyone, young or old, who has lived long enough to have a past on which to look. Even children and youth have a

past in this sense, though their pasts are of course shorter than persons who are older.

Abraham Maslow: a hierarchy of motives and needs

Abraham Maslow's theory frames personal needs or motives as a hierarchy, meaning that basic or “lower-level”

needs have to be satisfied before higher-level needs become important or motivating (1976, 1987). Compared to the

stage models of Piaget and Erikson, Maslow’s hierarchy is only loosely “developmental”, in that Maslow was not

concerned with tracking universal, irreversible changes across the lifespan. Maslow's stages are universal, but they

are not irreversible; earlier stages sometimes reappear later in life, in which case they must be satisfied again before

later stages can redevelop. Like the theories of Piaget and Erikson, Maslow’s is a rather broad “story”, one that has

less to say about the effects of a person’s culture, language, or economic level, than about what we all have in

common.

In its original version, Maslow’s theory distinguishes two types of needs, called deficit needs and being

needs (or sometimes deficiency needs and growth needs). Table 8 summarizes the two levels and their sublevels.

Deficit needs are prior to being needs, not in the sense of happening earlier in life, but in that deficit needs must be

satisfied before being needs can be addressed. As pointed out, deficit needs can reappear at any age, depending on

circumstances. If that happens, they must be satisfied again before a person’s attention can shift back to “higher”

needs. Among students, in fact, deficit needs are likely to return chronically to those whose families lack economic

or social resources or who live with the stresses associated with poverty (Payne, 2005).

Table 8: Maslow's hierarchy of motives and needs

Deficit Needs

Physiological needs

Safety and security needs

Love and belonging needs

Cognitive needs

54

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Being Needs Aesthetic needs

Self-actualization needs

Deficit needs: getting the basic necessities of life

Deficit needs are the basic requirements of physical and emotional well-being. First are physiological needs—

food, sleep, clothing, and the like. Without these, nothing else matters, and especially nothing very “elevated” or

self-fulfilling. A student who is not getting enough to eat is not going to feel much interest in learning! Once

physiological needs are met, however, safety and security needs become important. The person looks for stability

and protection, and welcomes a bit of structure and limits if they provide these conditions. A child from an abusive

family, for example, may be getting enough to eat, but may worry chronically about personal safety. In school, the

student may appreciate a well-organized classroom with rules that insures personal safety and predictability,

whether or not the classroom provides much in the way of real learning.

After physiological and safety needs are met, love and belonging needs emerge. The person turns attention to

making friends, being a friend, and cultivating positive personal relationships in general. In the classroom, a

student motivated at this level may make approval from peers or teachers into a top priority. He or she may be

provided for materially and find the classroom and family life safe enough, but still miss a key ingredient in life—

love. If such a student (or anyone else) eventually does find love and belonging, however, then his or her motivation

shifts again, this time to esteem needs. Now the concern is with gaining recognition and respect—and even more

importantly, gaining self-respect. A student at this level may be unusually concerned with achievement, for

example, though only if the achievement is visible or public enough to earn public recognition.

Being needs: becoming the best that you can be

Being needs are desires to become fulfilled as a person, or to be the best person that you can possibly be. They

include cognitive needs (a desire for knowledge and understanding), aesthetic needs (an appreciation of beauty and

order), and most importantly, self-actualization needs (a desire for fulfillment of one’s potential). Being needs

emerge only after all of a person’s deficit needs have been largely met. Unlike deficit needs, being needs beget more

being needs; they do not disappear once they are met, but create a desire for even more satisfaction of the same

type. A thirst for knowledge, for example, leads to further thirst for knowledge, and aesthetic appreciation leads to

more aesthetic appreciation. Partly because being needs are lasting and permanent once they appear, Maslow

sometimes treated them as less hierarchical than deficit needs, and instead grouped cognitive, aesthetic, and self-

actualization needs into the single category self-actualization needs.

People who are motivated by self-actualization have a variety of positive qualities, which Maslow went to some

lengths to identify and describe (Maslow, 1976). Self-actualizing individuals, he argued, value deep personal

relationships with others, but also value solitude; they have a sense of humor, but do not use it against others; they

accept themselves as well as others; they are spontaneous, humble, creative, and ethical. In short, the self-

actualizing person has just about every good quality imaginable! Not surprisingly, therefore, Maslow felt that true

self-actualization is rare. It is especially unusual among young people, who have not yet lived long enough to satisfy

earlier, deficit-based needs.

Educational Psychology 55 A Global Text

3. Student development

In a way this last point is discouraging news for teachers, who apparently must spend their lives providing as

best they can for individuals—students—still immersed in deficit needs. Teachers, it seems, have little hope of ever

meeting a student with fully fledged being needs. Taken less literally, though, Maslow’s hierarchy is still useful for

thinking about students’ motives. Most teachers would argue that students—young though they are—can display

positive qualities similar to the ones described in Maslow’s self-actualizing person. However annoying students may

sometimes be, there are also moments when they show care and respect for others, for example, and moments

when they show spontaneity, humility, or a sound ethical sense. Self-actualization is an appropriate way to think

about these moments—the times when students are at their best. At the same time, of course, students sometimes

also have deficit needs. Keeping in mind the entire hierarchy outlined by Maslow can therefore deepen teachers'

understanding of the full humanity of students.

Moral development: forming a sense of rights and responsibilities

Morality is a system of beliefs about what is right and good compared to what is wrong or bad. Moral

development refers to changes in moral beliefs as a person grows older and gains maturity. Moral beliefs are

related to, but not identical with, moral behavior: it is possible to know the right thing to do, but not actually do it.

It is also not the same as knowledge of social conventions, which are arbitrary customs needed for the smooth

operation of society. Social conventions may have a moral element, but they have a primarily practical purpose.

Conventionally, for example, motor vehicles all keep to the same side of the street (to the right in the United States,

to the left in Great Britain). The convention allows for smooth, accident-free flow of traffic. But following the

convention also has a moral element, because an individual who chooses to drive on the wrong side of the street can

cause injuries or even death. In this sense, choosing the wrong side of the street is wrong morally, though the choice

is also unconventional.

When it comes to schooling and teaching, moral choices are not restricted to occasional dramatic incidents, but

are woven into almost every aspect of classroom life. Imagine this simple example. Suppose that you are teaching,

reading to a small group of second-graders, and the students are taking turns reading a story out loud. Should you

give every student the same amount of time to read, even though some might benefit from having additional time?

Or should you give more time to the students who need extra help, even if doing so bores classmates and deprives

others of equal shares of “floor time”? Which option is more fair, and which is more considerate? Simple dilemmas

like this happen every day at all grade levels simply because students are diverse, and because class time and a

teacher’s energy are finite.

Embedded in this rather ordinary example are moral themes about fairness or justice, on the one hand, and

about consideration or care on the other. It is important to keep both themes in mind when thinking about how

students develop beliefs about right or wrong. A morality of justice is about human rights—or more specifically,

about respect for fairness, impartiality, equality, and individuals’ independence. A morality of care, on the other

hand, is about human responsibilities—more specifically, about caring for others, showing consideration for

individuals’ needs, and interdependence among individuals. Students and teachers need both forms of morality. In

the next sections therefore we explain a major example of each type of developmental theory, beginning with the

morality of justice.

56

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Kohlberg’s morality of justice

One of the best-known explanations of how morality of justice develops was developed by Lawrence Kohlberg

and his associates (Kohlberg, Levine, & Hewer, 1983; Power, Higgins, & Kohlberg, 1991). Using a stage model

similar to Piaget’s, Kohlberg proposed six stages of moral development, grouped into three levels. Individuals

experience the stages universally and in sequence as they form beliefs about justice. He named the levels simply

preconventional, conventional, and (you guessed it) postconventional. The levels and stages are summarized in

Table 5.

Table 9: Moral stages according to Kohlberg

Moral stage Definition of what is “good”

Preconventional Level:

Stage 1: Obedience and punishment

Stage 2: Market exchange

Action that is rewarded and not punished

Action that is agreeable to the child and child's partner

Conventional Level:

Stage 3: Peer opinion

Stage 4: Law and order

Action that wins approval from friends or peers

Action that conforms to community customs or laws

Postconventional Level:

Stage 5: Social contract

Stage 6: Universal principles

Action that follows social accepted ways of making decisions

Action that is consistent with self-chosen, general principles

Preconventional justice: obedience and mutual advantage

The preconventional level of moral development coincides approximately with the preschool period of life and

with Piaget’s preoperational period of thinking. At this age the child is still relatively self-centered and insensitive to

the moral effects of actions on others. The result is a somewhat short-sighted orientation to morality. Initially

(Kohlberg’s Stage 1), the child adopts an ethics of obedience and punishment—a sort of “morality of keeping

out of trouble”. The rightness and wrongness of actions is determined by whether actions are rewarded or punished

by authorities such as parents or teachers. If helping yourself to a cookie brings affectionate smiles from adults,

then taking the cookie is considered morally “good”. If it brings scolding instead, then it is morally “bad”. The child

does not think about why an action might be praised or scolded; in fact, says Kohlberg, he would be incapable at

Stage 1 of considering the reasons even if adults offered them.

Eventually the child learns not only to respond to positive consequences, but also learns how to produce them by

exchanging favors with others. The new ability creates Stage 2, an ethics of market exchange. At this stage the

morally “good” action is one that favors not only the child, but another person directly involved. A “bad” action is

one that lacks this reciprocity. If trading the sandwich from your lunch for the cookies in your friend’s lunch is

Educational Psychology 57 A Global Text

3. Student development

mutually agreeable, then the trade is morally good; otherwise it is not. This perspective introduces a type of fairness

into the child’s thinking for the first time. But it still ignores the larger context of actions—the effects on people not

present or directly involved. In Stage 2, for example, it would also be considered morally “good” to pay a classmate

to do another student's homework—or even to avoid bullying or to provide sexual favors—provided that both

parties regard the arrangement as being fair.

Conventional justice: conformity to peers and society

As children move into the school years, their lives expand to include a larger number and range of peers and

(eventually) of the community as a whole. The change leads to conventional morality, which are beliefs based on

what this larger array of people agree on—hence Kohlberg’s use of the term “conventional”. At first, in Stage 3, the

child’s reference group are immediate peers, so Stage 3 is sometimes called the ethics of peer opinion. If peers

believe, for example, that it is morally good to behave politely with as many people as possible, then the child is

likely to agree with the group and to regard politeness as not merely an arbitrary social convention, but a moral

“good”. This approach to moral belief is a bit more stable than the approach in Stage 2, because the child is taking

into account the reactions not just of one other person, but of many. But it can still lead astray if the group settles

on beliefs that adults consider morally wrong, like “Shop lifting for candy bars is fun and desirable.”

Eventually, as the child becomes a youth and the social world expands even more, he or she acquires even larger

numbers of peers and friends. He or she is therefore more likely to encounter disagreements about ethical issues

and beliefs. Resolving the complexities lead to Stage 4, the ethics of law and order, in which the young person

increasingly frames moral beliefs in terms of what the majority of society believes. Now, an action is morally good if

it is legal or at least customarily approved by most people, including people whom the youth does not know

personally. This attitude leads to an even more stable set of principles than in the previous stage, though it is still

not immune from ethical mistakes. A community or society may agree, for example, that people of a certain race

should be treated with deliberate disrespect, or that a factory owner is entitled to dump waste water into a

commonly shared lake or river. To develop ethical principles that reliably avoid mistakes like these require further

stages of moral development.

Postconventional justice: social contract and universal principles

As a person becomes able to think abstractly (or “formally”, in Piaget’s sense), ethical beliefs shift from

acceptance of what the community does believe to the process by which community beliefs are formed. The new

focus constitutes Stage 5, the ethics of social contract. Now an action, belief, or practice is morally good if it has

been created through fair, democratic processes that respect the rights of the people affected. Consider, for

example, the laws in some areas that require motorcyclists to wear helmets. In what sense are the laws about this

behavior ethical? Was it created by consulting with and gaining the consent of the relevant people? Were cyclists

consulted and did they give consent? Or how about doctors or the cyclists' families? Reasonable, thoughtful

individuals disagree about how thoroughly and fairly these consultation processes should be. In focusing on the

processes by which the law was created, however, individuals are thinking according to Stage 5, the ethics of social

contract, regardless of the position they take about wearing helmets. In this sense, beliefs on both sides of a debate

about an issue can sometimes be morally sound even if they contradict each other.

58

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Paying attention to due process certainly seems like it should help to avoid mindless conformity to conventional

moral beliefs. As an ethical strategy, though, it too can sometimes fail. The problem is that an ethics of social

contract places more faith in democratic process than the process sometimes deserves, and does not pay enough

attention to the content of what gets decided. In principle (and occasionally in practice), a society could decide

democratically to kill off every member of a racial minority, for example, but would deciding this by due process

make it ethical? The realization that ethical means can sometimes serve unethical ends leads some individuals

toward Stage 6, the ethics of self-chosen, universal principles. At this final stage, the morally good action is

based on personally held principles that apply both to the person’s immediate life as well as to the larger

community and society. The universal principles may include a belief in democratic due process (Stage 5 ethics),

but also other principles, such as a belief in the dignity of all human life or the sacredness of the natural

environment. At Stage 6, the universal principles will guide a person’s beliefs even if the principles mean

disagreeing occasionally with what is customary (Stage 4) or even with what is legal (Stage 5).

Gilligan’s morality of care

As logical as they sound, Kohlberg’s stages of moral justice are not sufficient for understanding the development

of moral beliefs. To see why, suppose that you have a student who asks for an extension of the deadline for an

assignment. The justice orientation of Kohlberg’s theory would prompt you to consider issues of whether granting

the request is fair. Would the late student be able to put more effort into the assignment than other students?

Would the extension place a difficult demand on you, since you would have less time to mark the assignments?

These are important considerations related to the rights of students and the teacher. In addition to these, however,

are considerations having to do with the responsibilities that you and the requesting student have for each other

and for others. Does the student have a valid personal reason (illness, death in the family, etc.) for the assignment

being late? Will the assignment lose its educational value if the student has to turn it in prematurely? These latter

questions have less to do with fairness and rights, and more to do with taking care of and responsibility for

students. They require a framework different from Kohlberg’s to be understood fully.

One such framework has been developed by Carol Gilligan, whose ideas center on a morality of care, or

system of beliefs about human responsibilities, care, and consideration for others. Gilligan proposed three moral

positions that represent different extents or breadth of ethical care. Unlike Kohlberg, Piaget, or Erikson, she does

not claim that the positions form a strictly developmental sequence, but only that they can be ranked hierarchically

according to their depth or subtlety. In this respect her theory is “semi-developmental” in a way similar to Maslow’s

theory of motivation (Brown & Gilligan, 1992; Taylor, Gilligan, & Sullivan, 1995). Table 10 summarizes the three

moral positions from Gilligan’s theory

Table 10: Positions of moral development according to Gilligan

Moral position Definition of what is morally good

Position 1: Survival

orientation

Action that considers one's personal needs only

Position 2: Conventional

care

Action that considers others' needs or preferences, but not one's own

Educational Psychology 59 A Global Text

3. Student development

Position 3: Integrated care Action that attempts to coordinate one's own personal needs with those of

others

Position 1: caring as survival

The most basic kind of caring is a survival orientation, in which a person is concerned primarily with his or

her own welfare. If a teenage girl with this ethical position is wondering whether to get an abortion, for example,

she will be concerned entirely with the effects of the abortion on herself. The morally good choice will be whatever

creates the least stress for herself and that disrupts her own life the least. Responsibilities to others (the baby, the

father, or her family) play little or no part in her thinking.

As a moral position, a survival orientation is obviously not satisfactory for classrooms on a widespread scale. If

every student only looked out for himself or herself, classroom life might become rather unpleasant! Nonetheless,

there are situations in which focusing primarily on yourself is both a sign of good mental health and relevant to

teachers. For a child who has been bullied at school or sexually abused at home, for example, it is both healthy and

morally desirable to speak out about how bullying or abuse has affected the victim. Doing so means essentially

looking out for the victim’s own needs at the expense of others’ needs, including the bully’s or abuser’s. Speaking

out, in this case, requires a survival orientation and is healthy because the child is taking caring of herself.

Position 2: conventional caring

A more subtle moral position is caring for others, in which a person is concerned about others’ happiness and

welfare, and about reconciling or integrating others’ needs where they conflict with each other. In considering an

abortion, for example, the teenager at this position would think primarily about what other people prefer. Do the

father, her parents, and/or her doctor want her to keep the child? The morally good choice becomes whatever will

please others the best. This position is more demanding than Position 1, ethically and intellectually, because it

requires coordinating several persons’ needs and values. But it is often morally insufficient because it ignores one

crucial person: the self.

In classrooms, students who operate from Position 2 can be very desirable in some ways; they can be eager to

please, considerate, and good at fitting in and at working cooperatively with others. Because these qualities are

usually welcome in a busy classroom, teachers can be tempted to reward students for developing and using them.

The problem with rewarding Position 2 ethics, however, is that doing so neglects the student’s development—his or

her own academic and personal goals or values. Sooner or later, personal goals, values, and identity need attention

and care, and educators have a responsibility for assisting students to discover and clarify them.

Position 3: integrated caring

The most developed form of moral caring in Gilligan’s model is integrated caring, the coordination of

personal needs and values with those of others. Now the morally good choice takes account of everyone including

yourself, not everyone except yourself. In considering an abortion, a woman at Position 3 would think not only

about the consequences for the father, the unborn child, and her family, but also about the consequences for

herself. How would bearing a child affect her own needs, values, and plans? This perspective leads to moral beliefs

that are more comprehensive, but ironically are also more prone to dilemmas because the widest possible range of

individuals are being considered.

60

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

In classrooms, integrated caring is most likely to surface whenever teachers give students wide, sustained

freedom to make choices. If students have little flexibility about their actions, there is little room for considering

anyone’s needs or values, whether their own or others’. If the teacher says simply: “Do the homework on page 50

and turn it in tomorrow morning”, then the main issue becomes compliance, not moral choice. But suppose instead

that she says something like this: “Over the next two months, figure out an inquiry project about the use of water

resources in our town. Organize it any way you want—talk to people, read widely about it, and share it with the class

in a way that all of us, including yourself, will find meaningful.” An assignment like this poses moral challenges that

are not only educational, but also moral, since it requires students to make value judgments. Why? For one thing,

students must decide what aspect of the topic really matters to them. Such a decision is partly a matter of personal

values. For another thing, students have to consider how to make the topic meaningful or important to others in the

class. Third, because the time line for completion is relatively far in the future, students may have to weigh personal

priorities (like spending time with friends or family) against educational priorities (working on the assignment a bit

more on the weekend). As you might suspect, some students might have trouble making good choices when given

this sort of freedom—and their teachers might therefore be cautious about giving such an assignment. But the

difficulties in making choices are part of Gilligan’s point: integrated caring is indeed more demanding than the

caring based only on survival or on consideration of others. Not all students may be ready for it.

Understanding “the typical student” versus understanding students

In this chapter, in keeping with the general nature of developmental theory, we have often spoken of students in

a generalized way, referring to “the” child, student, or youngster, as if a single typical or average individual exists

and develops through single, predictable pathways. As every teacher knows, however, development is not that

simple. A class of 25 or 30 students will contain 25 or 30 individuals each learning and developing along distinct

pathways. Why then study developmental patterns at all? Because underlying their obvious diversity, students

indeed show important similarities. This chapter has indicated some of the similarities and how they relate to the

job of teaching. Our references to “the” student should not be understood, therefore, as supporting simple-minded

stereotypes; they refer instead to common tendencies of real, live children and youth. Pointing to developmental

changes is like pointing to a flock of birds in flight: the flock has a general location, but individual birds also have

their own locations and take individual flight paths. Development and diversity therefore have to be understood

jointly, not separately. There are indeed similarities woven among the differences in students, but also differences

woven among students’ commonalities. We recommend therefore that you read this chapter on development

together with the next one, which looks explicitly at student diversity.

Chapter summary

Understanding development, or the long-term changes in growth, behavior, and knowledge, helps teachers to

hold appropriate expectations for students as well as to keep students’ individual diversity in perspective. From

kindergarten through the end of high school, students double their height, triple their weight, experience the social

and hormonal effects of puberty, and improve basic motor skills. Their health is generally good, though illnesses are

affected significantly by students’ economic and social circumstances.

Cognitively, students develop major new abilities to think logically and abstractly, based on a foundation of

sensory and motor experiences with the objects and people around them. Jean Piaget has one well-known theory

detailing how these changes unfold.

Educational Psychology 61 A Global Text

3. Student development

Socially, students face and resolve a number of issues—especially the issue of industry (dedicated, sustained

work) during childhood and the issue of identity during adolescence. Erik Erikson has described these crises in

detail, as well as social crises that precede and follow the school years. Students are motivated both by basic human

needs (food, safety, belonging, esteem) and by needs to enhance themselves psychologically (self-actualization).

Abraham Maslow has described these motivations and how they relate to each other.

Morally, students develop both a sense of justice and of care for others, and their thinking in each of these

realms undergoes important changes as they mature. Lawrence Kohlberg has described changes in children and

youth’s beliefs about justice, and Carol Gilligan has described changes in their beliefs about care.

On the Internet

This is part of the website for the Society for Research in Child Development, an

organization that supports research about children and youth, and that advocates for government policies on their

behalf. The specific web page recommended here contains their press releases, which summarize findings from

current research and their implications for children’s welfare. You will need to register to use this page, but

registration is free.

This is the website for the American Psychological Association, the largest professional

association of psychologists in the English-speaking world. From the homepage you can go to a section called

“psychology topics”, which offers a variety of interesting articles and press releases free of charge. Among other

topics, for example, there are articles about obesity and its effects, as well as about factors that support (and/or

detract from) children’s well-being.

Key termsDevelopmentPuberty

CognitionCognitive stages

Jean PiagetSensorimotor stage

Object permanencePreoperational stage

Dramatic playConcrete operational stage

DecenterConservation

Formal operational stageHypothetical reasoning

Social developmentErik Erikson

Abraham MaslowLawrence Kohlberg

Carol GilliganPsychosocial crises

Trust, autonomy, and initiativeIndustry

IdentityIntimacy, generativity, and integrity

Maslow’s hierarchy of needsDeficit needs

Being needsSelf-actualization

Moral developmentLawrence Kohlberg

Carol GilliganMorality of justice

Preconventional justiceEthics of obedience

Ethics of mutual advantageConventional justice

Ethics of peer opinionEthics of law and order

Postconventional justiceEthics of social contract

Ethics of universal principlesMorality of care

Survival OrientationConventional care

62

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Integrated care

References

Allender, J. (2005). Community health nursing, 6th edition. Philadelphia: Lippincott, Williams, & Wilkins.

Bawa, S. (2005). The role of the consumption of beverages in the obesity epidemic. Journal of the Royal

Society for the Promotion of Health, 125(3), 124-128.

Beidel, B. (2005). Childhood anxiety disorders. Oxford, UK: Brunner-Routledge.

Bredekamp, S. & Copple, C. (1997). Developmentally appropriate practice, Revised edition. Washington,

D.C.: National Association for the Education of Young Children.

Brown, L. & Gilligan, C. (1992). Meeting at the crossroads: Women’s psychology and girls’ development.

Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Case, R. (1991). The mind’s staircase: Exploring the conceptual underpinnings of children’s thought and

knowledge. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Case, R. & Okamoto, Y. (1996). The role of central conceptual structures in children’s thought. Chicago:

Society for Research on Child Development.

Center for Disease Control. (2004a). National survey on drug use and health. Bethesda, MD: Department of

Health and Human Services.

Center for Disease Control (2004b). Trends in the prevalence of sexual behaviors, 1991-2003. Bethesda,

MD: Author.

Eveleth, P. & Tanner, J. (1990). Worldwide variation in human growth (2nd edition). New York: Cambridge

University Press.

Eyler, J. & Giles, D. (1999). Where’s the learning in service learning? San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Fagan, A. & Najman, J. (2005). The relative contribution of parental and sibling substance use to adolescent

alcohol, tobacco, and other drug use. Journal of Drug Issues, 35, 869-883.

Friedman, S. (2000). When girls feel fat: Helping girls through adolescence. Toronto: Firefly Books.

Harris, J. (2006). No two alike: Human nature and human individuality. New York: Norton.

Inhelder, B. & Piaget, J. (1958). The growth of logical thinking from childhood to adolescence: An essay on

the growth of formal operational structures. New York: Basic Books.

Johnston, L., O’Malley, P., Bachman, J., & Schulenberg, J. (2006). Monitoring the future: National results

on adolescent drug use: Overview of key findings, 2005. Bethesda, MD: National Institute on Drug

Abuse.

Kay, C. (2003). The complete guide to service learning. New York: Free Spirit Publishing.

Kohlberg, L., Levine, C., & Hewer, A. (1983). Moral stages: A current formulation and a response to critics.

Basel: S. Karger.

Lewis, M. (1997). Altering fate: Why the past does not predict the future. New York: Guilford Press.

Educational Psychology 63 A Global Text

3. Student development

Malina, R., Bouchard, C., & Bar-Or O. (2004). Growth, maturation, and physical activity. Champaign, IL:

Human Kinetics Press.

Maslow, A. (1987). Motivation and personality, 3rd edition. New York: Harper & Row.

Maslow, A. (1976). The Farther Reaches of Human Nature, 2nd edition. New York: Penguin Books.

Matthews, G. (1998). The philosophy of childhood. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

McClintock, M. & Herdt, G. (1996). Rethinking puberty: The development of sexual attraction. Current

Directions in Psychological Science, 5, 178-183.

Narayan, K., Boyle, J., Thompson, T., Sorensen, S., & Williamson, D. (2003). Lifetime risk for diabetes

mellitus in the United States. Journal of the American Medical Association, 290(14), 1884-1890.

National Institute of Allergies and Infectious Diseases. (2005). The common cold. Bethesda, MD: Author.

Also available at .

Ogden, C., Flega, K., Carroll, M. & Johnson, C. (2002). Prevalence and trends in overweight among U.S.

children and adolescents, 1999-2000. Journal of the American Medical Association, 288(14), 1728-1732.

Paley, V. (2005). A child’s work: The importance of fantasy play. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Payne, R. (2005). A framework for understanding poverty. Highlands, TX: aha!Process, Inc.

Payne, V. & Isaacs, L. (2005). Human motor development: A lifespan approach, 6th edition. Boston:

McGraw-Hill.

Piaget, J. (1952). The origins of intelligence in children. New York: International Universities Press.

Piaget, J. (1983). Piaget’s theory. In P. Mussen (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology, volume 1. New York:

Wiley.

Piaget, J. (2001). The psychology of intelligence. Oxford, UK: Routledge.

Petlichkoff, L. (1996). The drop-out dilemma in youth sports. In O. Bar-Or (Ed.), The child and adolescent

athlete (pp. 418-432). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.

Power, F., Higgins, A., & Kohlberg, L. (1991). Lawrence Kohlberg’s approach to moral education. New York:

Columbia University Press.

Richardson, J. (2005). The cost of being poor. Westport, CN: Praeger.

Rosenbaum, J. (2006). Reborn a Virgin: Adolescents' Retracting of Virginity Pledges and Sexual Histories.

American Journal of Public Health, 96(6), xxx-yyy.

Sadker, M. (2004). Gender equity in the classroom: The unfinished agenda. In M. Kimmel (Ed.), The

gendered society reader, 2nd edition. New York: Oxford University Press.

Salkind, N. (2004). An introduction to theories of human development. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage

Publications.

Slavin, R. (2005). Educational psychology, 7th edition. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.

64

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Spencer, N. (2000). Poverty and child health, 2nd edition. Abingdon, UK: Radcliffe Medical Press.

Tartamella, L., Herscher, E., Woolston, C. (2004). Generation extra large: Rescuing our children from the

obesity epidemic. New York: Basic Books.

Taylor, J. & Gilligan, C., & Sullivan, A. (1995). Between voice and silence: Women and girls, race and

relationship. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

United States Government Printing Office. (2002). No Child Left Behind Act: A desktop reference.

Washington, D.C.: Author.

Whelen, E., Lawson, C., Grajewski, B., Petersen, M., Pinkerton, L., Ward, E., & Schnorr, T. (2003).

Prevalence of respiratory symptoms among female flight attendants and teachers. Occupational and

Environmental Medicine, 60, 929-934.

Woolfolk, A. (2006). Educational psychology, 10th edition. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.

Yin, Z., Hanes, J., Moore, J., Humbles, P., Barbeau, & Gutin, B. (2005). An after-school physical activity

program for obesity prevention in children. Evaluation and the Health Professions, 28(1), 67-89.

Zimbardo, P. & Radl, S. (1999). The shy child: Overcoming and preventing shyness from birth to adulthood.

Cambridge, MA: Malor Books.

Educational Psychology 65 A Global Text

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

4. Student diversityI’ll tell you this: There are some people, and then there are others.

(Anna Harris)

Anna Harris was Kelvin Seifert's grandmother as well as a schoolteacher from about 1910 to 1930.

She used to make comments, like the one above, that sounded odd but that also contained a grain of

wisdom. In this case her remark makes a good theme for this chapter—and even for teaching in

general. Students do differ in a multitude of ways, both individually and because of memberships in

families, communities or cultural groups. Sometimes the differences can make classroom-style

teaching more challenging, but other times, as Anna Harris implied, they simply enrich classroom

life. To teach students well, we need to understand the important ways that they differ among

themselves, and when or how the differences really matter for their education. This chapter offers

some of that understanding and suggests how you might use it in order to make learning effective

and enjoyable for everyone, including yourself.

For convenience we will make a major distinction between differences among individuals and

differences among groups of students. As the term implies, individual differences are qualities

that are unique; just one person has them at a time. Variation in hair color, for example, is an

individual difference; even though some people have nearly the same hair color, no two people are

exactly the same. Group differences are qualities shared by members of an identifiable group or

community, but not shared by everyone in society. An example is gender role: for better or for worse,

one portion of society (the males) is perceived differently and expected to behave a bit differently

than another portion of society (the females). Notice that distinguishing between individual and

group differences is convenient, but a bit arbitrary. Individuals with similar, but nonetheless unique

qualities sometimes group themselves together for certain purposes, and groups unusually contain a

lot of individual diversity within them. If you happen to enjoy playing soccer and have some talent

for it (an individual quality), for example, you may end up as a member of a soccer team or club (a

group defined by members’ common desire and ability to play soccer). But though everyone on the

team fits a “soccer player’s profile” at some level, individual members will probably vary in level of

skill and motivation. The group, by its very nature, may obscure these signs of individuality.

To begin, then, we look at several differences normally considered to be individually rather than

group based. This discussion will necessarily be incomplete simply because individual differences are

so numerous and important in teaching that some of them are also discussed in later chapters. Later

sections of this chapter deal with three important forms of group diversity: gender differences,

cultural differences, and language differences.

Educational Psychology 66 A Global Text

4. Student diversity

Individual styles of learning and thinking

All of us, including our students, have preferred ways of learning. Teachers often refer to these differences as

learning styles, though this term may imply that students are more consistent across situations than is really the

case. One student may like to make diagrams to help remember a reading assignment, whereas another student

may prefer to write a sketchy outline instead. Yet in many cases, the students could in principle reverse the

strategies and still learn the material: if coaxed (or perhaps required), the diagram-maker could take notes for a

change and the note-taker could draw diagrams. Both would still learn, though neither might feel as comfortable as

when using the strategies that they prefer. This reality suggests that a balanced, middle-of-the-road approach may

be a teacher’s best response to students’ learning styles. Or put another way, it is good to support students’

preferred learning strategies where possible and appropriate, but neither necessary nor desirable to do so all of the

time (Loo, 2004; Stahl, 2002). Most of all, it is neither necessary nor possible to classify or label students according

to seemingly fixed learning styles and then allow them to learn only according to those styles. A student may prefer

to hear new material rather than see it; he may prefer for you to explain something orally, for example, rather than

to see it demonstrated in a video. But he may nonetheless tolerate or sometimes even prefer to see it demonstrated.

In the long run, in fact, he may learn it best by encountering the material in both ways, regardless of his habitual

preferences.

That said, there is evidence that individuals, including students, do differ in how they habitually think. These

differences are more specific than learning styles or preferences, and psychologists sometimes call them cognitive

styles, meaning typical ways of perceiving and remembering information, and typical ways of solving problems

and making decisions (Zhang & Sternberg, 2006). In a style of thinking called field dependence, for example,

individuals perceive patterns as a whole rather than focus on the parts of the pattern separately. In a

complementary tendency, called field independence, individuals are more inclined to analyze overall patterns

into their parts. Cognitive research from the 1940s to the present has found field dependence/independence

differences to be somewhat stable for any given person across situations, though not completely so (Witkin, Moore,

Goodenough, & Cox, 1977; Zhang & Sternberg, 2005). Someone who is field dependent (perceives globally or

“wholistically”) in one situation, tends to a modest extent to perceive things globally or wholistically in other

situations. Field dependence and independence can be important in understanding students because the styles

affect students’ behaviors and preferences in school and classrooms. Field dependent persons tend to work better in

groups, it seems, and to prefer “open-ended” fields of study like literature and history. Field independent persons,

on the other hand, tend to work better alone and to prefer highly analytic studies like math and science. The

differences are only a tendency, however, and there are a lot of students who contradict the trends. As with the

broader notion of learning styles, the cognitive styles of field dependence and independence are useful for tailoring

instruction to particular students, but their guidance is only approximate. They neither can nor should be used to

“lock” students to particular modes of learning or to replace students’ own expressed preferences and choices about

curriculum.

Another cognitive style is impulsivity as compared to reflectivity. As the names imply, an impulsive cognitive

style is one in which a person reacts quickly, but as a result makes comparatively more errors. A reflective style is

the opposite: the person reacts more slowly and therefore makes fewer errors. As you might expect, the reflective

style would seem better suited to many academic demands of school. Research has found that this is indeed the

67

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

case for academic skills that clearly benefit from reflection, such as mathematical problem solving or certain

reading tasks (Evans, 2004). Some classroom or school-related skills, however, may actually develop better if a

student is relatively impulsive. Being a good partner in a cooperative learning group, for example, may depend

partly on responding spontaneously (i.e. just a bit “impulsively”) to others’ suggestions; and being an effective

member of an athletic team may depend on not taking time to reflect carefully on every move that you or your team

mates make.

There are two major ways to use knowledge of students’ cognitive styles (Pritchard, 2005). The first and the

more obvious is to build on students’ existing style strengths and preferences. A student who is field independent

and reflective, for example, can be encouraged to explore tasks and activities that are relatively analytic and that

require relatively independent work. One who is field dependent and impulsive, on the other hand, can be

encouraged and supported to try tasks and activities that are more social or spontaneous. But a second, less obvious

way to use knowledge of cognitive styles is to encourage more balance in cognitive styles for students who need it. A

student who lacks field independence, for example, may need explicit help in organizing and analyzing key

academic tasks (like organizing a lab report in a science class). One who is already highly reflective may need

encouragement to try ideas spontaneously, as in a creative writing lesson.

Multiple intelligences

For nearly a century, educators and psychologists have debated the nature of intelligence, and more specifically

whether intelligence is just one broad ability or can take more than one form. Many classical definitions of the

concept have tended to define intelligence as a single broad ability that allows a person to solve or complete many

sorts of tasks, or at least many academic tasks like reading, knowledge of vocabulary, and the solving of logical

problems (Garlick, 2002). There is research evidence of such a global ability, and the idea of general intelligence

often fits with society’s everyday beliefs about intelligence. Partly for these reasons, an entire mini-industry has

grown up around publishing tests of intelligence, academic ability, and academic achievement. Since these tests

affect the work of teachers, I return to discussing them later in this book.

But there are also problems with defining intelligence as one general ability. One way of summing up the

problems is to say that conceiving of intelligence as something general tends to put it beyond teachers’ influence.

When viewed as a single, all-purpose ability, students either have a lot of intelligence or they do not, and

strengthening their intelligence becomes a major challenge, or perhaps even an impossible one (Gottfredson, 2004;

Lubinski, 2004). This conclusion is troubling to some educators, especially in recent years as testing school

achievements have become more common and as students have become more diverse.

But alternate views of intelligence also exist that portray intelligence as having multiple forms, whether the

forms are subparts of a single broader ability or are multiple “intelligences” in their own right. For various reasons

such this perspective has gained in popularity among teachers in recent years, probably because it reflects many

teachers’ beliefs that students cannot simply be rated along a single scale of ability, but are fundamentally diverse

(Kohn, 2004).

One of the most prominent of these models is Howard Gardner’s theory of multiple intelligences

(Gardner, 1983, 2003). Gardner proposes that there are eight different forms of intelligence, each of which

functions independently of the others. (The eight intelligences are summarized in Table 11. Each person has a mix

Educational Psychology 68 A Global Text

4. Student diversity

of all eight abilities—more of one and less of another—that helps to constitute that person’s individual cognitive

profile. Since most tasks—including most tasks in classrooms—require several forms of intelligence and can be

completed in more than one way, it is possible for people with various profiles of talents to succeed on a task

equally well. In writing an essay, for example, a student with high interpersonal intelligence but rather average

verbal intelligence might use his or her interpersonal strength to get a lot of help and advice from classmates and

the teacher. A student with the opposite profile might work well alone, but without the benefit of help from others.

Both students might end up with essays that are good, but good for different reasons.

Table 11: Multiple intelligences according to Howard Gardner

Form of intelligence Examples of activities using the intelligence

Linguistic: verbal skill; ability to use language well • verbal persuasion

• writing a term paper skillfully

Musical: ability to create and understand music • singing, playing a musical instrument

• composing a tune

Logical: Mathematical: logical skill; ability to reason,

often using mathematics

• solving mathematical problems easily and

accurately

• developing and testing hypotheses

Spatial: ability to imagine and manipulate the

arrangement of objects in the environment

• completing a difficult jigsaw puzzle

• assembling a complex appliance (e.g. a bicycle)

Bodily: kinesthetic: sense of balance; coordination in

use of one's body

• dancing

• gymnastics

Interpersonal: ability to discern others' nonverbal

feelings and thoughts

• sensing when to be tactful

• sensing a “subtext” or implied message in a

person's statements

Intrapersonal: sensitivity to one's own thoughts and

feelings

• noticing complex of ambivalent feelings in

oneself

• identifying true motives for an action in oneself

Naturalist: sensitivity to subtle differences and

patterns found in the natural environment

• identifying examples of species of plants or

animals

• noticing relationships among species and

natural processes in the environment

69

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Source: Gardner, 1983, 2003

As evidence for the possibility of multiple intelligences, Gardner cites descriptions of individuals with

exceptional talent in one form of intelligence (for example, in playing the piano) but who are neither above nor

below average in other areas. He also cites descriptions of individuals with brain damage, some of whom lose one

particular form of intelligence (like the ability to talk) but retain other forms. In the opinion of many psychologists,

however, the evidence for multiple intelligences is not strong enough to give up the “classical” view of general

intelligence. Part of the problem is that the evidence for multiple intelligences relies primarily on anecdotes—

examples or descriptions of particular individuals who illustrate the model—rather than on more widespread

information or data (Eisner, 2004).

Nonetheless, whatever the status of the research evidence, the model itself can be useful as a way for teachers to

think about their work. Multiple intelligences suggest the importance of diversifying instruction in order to honor

and to respond to diversity in students’ talents and abilities. Viewed like this, whether Gardner’s classification

scheme is actually accurate is probably less important than the fact there is (or may be) more than one way to be

“smart”. In the end, as with cognitive and learning styles, it may not be important to label students’ talents or

intellectual strengths. It may be more important simply to provide important learning and knowledge in several

modes or styles, ways that draw on more than one possible form of intelligence or skill. A good example of this

principle is your own development in learning to teach. It is well and good to read books about teaching (like this

one, perhaps), but it is even better to read books and talk with classmates and educators about teaching and getting

actual experience in classrooms. The combination both invites and requires a wide range of your talents and usually

proves more effective than any single type of activity, whatever your profile of cognitive styles or intellectual

abilities happens to be.

Gifted and talented students

The idea of multiple intelligences leads to new ways of thinking about students who have special gifts and

talents. Traditionally, the term gifted referred only to students with unusually high verbal skills. Their skills were

demonstrated especially well, for example, on standardized tests of general ability or of school achievement, like

those described in Chapter 12 (“Standardized and other formal assessments”). More recently, however, the meaning

of gifted has broadened to include unusual talents in a range of activities, such as music, creative writing, or the arts

(G. Davis & Rimm, 2004). To indicate the change, educators often use the dual term gifted and talented.

Qualities of the gifted and talented

What are students who are gifted and talented like? Generally they show some combination of the following

qualities:

• They learn more quickly and independently than most students their own age.

• They often have well-developed vocabulary, as well as advanced reading and writing skills.

• They are very motivated, especially on tasks that are challenging or difficult.

• They hold themselves to higher than usual standards of achievement.

Educational Psychology 70 A Global Text

4. Student diversity

Contrary to a common impression, students who are gifted or talented are not necessarily awkward socially, less

healthy, or narrow in their interests—in fact, quite the contrary (Steiner & Carr, 2003). They also come from all

economic and cultural groups.

Ironically, in spite of their obvious strengths as learners, such students often languish in school unless teachers

can provide them with more than the challenges of the usual curriculum. A kindergarten child who is precociously

advanced in reading, for example, may make little further progress at reading if her teachers do not recognize and

develop her skill; her talent may effectively disappear from view as her peers gradually catch up to her initial level.

Without accommodation to their unusual level of skill or knowledge, students who are gifted or talented can

become bored by school, and eventually the boredom can even turn into behavior problems.

Partly for these reasons, students who are gifted or talented have sometimes been regarded as the responsibility

of special education, along with students with other sorts of disabilities. Often their needs are discussed, for

example, in textbooks about special education, alongside discussions of students with intellectual disabilities,

physical impairments, or major behavior disorders (Friend, 2008). There is some logic to this way of thinking

about their needs; after all, they are quite exceptional, and they do require modifications of the usual school

programs in order to reach their full potential. But it is also misleading to ignore obvious differences between

exceptional giftedness and exceptional disabilities of other kinds. The key difference is in students' potential. By

definition, students with gifts or talents are capable of creative, committed work at levels that often approach

talented adults. Other students—including students with disabilities—may reach these levels, but not as soon and

not as frequently. Many educators therefore think of the gifted and talented not as examples of students with

disabilities, but as examples of diversity. As such they are not so much the responsibility of special education

specialists, as the responsibility of all teachers to differentiate their instruction.

Supporting students who are gifted and talented

Supporting the gifted and talented usually involves a mixture of acceleration and enrichment of the usual

curriculum (Schiever & Maker, 2003). Acceleration involves either a child's skipping a grade, or else the teacher's

redesigning the curriculum within a particular grade or classroom so that more material is covered faster. Either

strategy works, but only up to a point: children who have skipped a grade usually function well in the higher grade,

both academically and socially. Unfortunately skipping grades cannot happen repeatedly unless teacher, parents,

and the students themselves are prepared to live with large age and maturity differences within single classrooms.

In itself, too, there is no guarantee that instruction in the new, higher-grade classroom will be any more stimulating

than it was in the former, lower-grade classroom. Redesigning the curriculum is also beneficial to the student, but

impractical to do on a widespread basis; even if teachers had the time to redesign their programs, many non-gifted

students would be left behind as a result.

Enrichment involves providing additional or different instruction added on to the usual curriculum goals and

activities. Instead of books at more advanced reading levels, for example, a student might read a wider variety of

types of literature at the student's current reading level, or try writing additional types of literature himself. Instead

of moving ahead to more difficult kinds of math programs, the student might work on unusual logic problems not

assigned to the rest of the class. Like acceleration, enrichment works well up to a point. Enrichment curricula exist

to help classroom teachers working with gifted students (and save teachers the time and work of creating

71

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

enrichment materials themselves). Since enrichment is not part of the normal, officially sanctioned curriculum,

however, there is a risk that it will be perceived as busywork rather than as intellectual stimulation, particularly if

the teacher herself is not familiar with the enrichment material or is otherwise unable to involve herself in the

material fully.

Obviously acceleration and enrichment can sometimes be combined. A student can skip a grade and also be

introduced to interesting “extra” material at the new grade level. A teacher can move a student to the next unit of

study faster than she moves the rest of the class, while at the same time offering additional activities not related to

the unit of study directly. For a teacher with a student who is gifted or talented, however, the real challenge is not

simply to choose between acceleration and enrichment, but to observe the student, get to know him or her as a

unique individual, and offer activities and supports based on that knowledge. This is essentially the challenge of

differentiating instruction, something needed not just by the gifted and talented, but by students of all sorts. As you

might suspect, differentiating instruction poses challenges about managing instruction; we discuss it again in more

detail in Chapter 9 (“Facilitating complex thinking”) and Chapter 10 (“Instructional planning”).

Gender differences in the classroom

Gender roles are the patterns of behaviors, attitudes, and expectations associated with a particular sex—with

being either male or female. For clarity, psychologists sometimes distinguish gender differences, which are related

to social roles, from sex differences, which are related only to physiology and anatomy. Using this terminology,

gender matters in teaching more than sex (in spite of any jokes told about the latter!).

Although there are many exceptions, boys and girls do differ on average in ways that parallel conventional

gender stereotypes and that affect how the sexes behave at school and in class. The differences have to do with

physical behaviors, styles of social interaction, academic motivations, behaviors, and choices. They have a variety of

sources—primarily parents, peers, and the media. Teachers are certainly not the primary cause of gender role

differences, but sometimes teachers influence them by their responses to and choices made on behalf of students.

Physical differences in gender roles

Physically, boys tend to be more active than girls, and by the same token more restless if they have to sit for long

periods. They are also more prone than girls to rely on physical aggression if they are frustrated (Espelage &

Swearer, 2004). Both tendencies are inconsistent with the usual demands of classroom life, of course, and make it a

little more likely that school will be a difficult experience for boys, even for boys who never actually get in trouble

for being restless or aggressive.

During the first two or three years of elementary school, gross motor skills develop at almost the same average

rate for boys and girls. As a group, both sexes can run, jump, throw a ball, and the like with about equal ease,

though there are of course wide significant differences among individuals of both sexes. Toward the end of

elementary school, however, boys pull ahead of girls at these skills even though neither sex has begun yet to

experience puberty. The most likely reason is that boys participate more actively in formal and informal sports

because of expectations and support from parents, peers, and society (Braddock, Sokol-Katz, Greene, & Basinger-

Fleischman, 2005; Messner, Duncan, & Cooky, 2003). Puberty eventually adds to this advantage by making boys

taller and stronger than girls, on average, and therefore more suited at least for sports that rely on height and

strength.

Educational Psychology 72 A Global Text

4. Student diversity

In thinking about these differences, keep in mind that they refer to average trends and that there are numerous

individual exceptions. Every teacher knows of individual boys who are not athletic, for example, or of particular

girls who are especially restless in class. The individual differences mean, among other things, that it is hard to

justify providing different levels of support or resources to boys than to girls for sports, athletics, or physical

education. The differences also suggest, though, that individual students who contradict gender stereotypes about

physical abilities may benefit from emotional support or affirmation from teachers, simply because they may be less

likely than usual to get such affirmation from elsewhere.

Social differences in gender roles

When relaxing socially, boys more often gravitate to large groups. Whether on the playground, in a school

hallway, or on the street, boys’ social groups tend literally to fill up a lot of space, and often include significant

amounts of roughhousing as well as organized and “semi-organized” competitive games or sports (Maccoby, 2002).

Girls, for their part, are more likely to seek and maintain one or two close friends and to share more intimate

information and feelings with these individuals. To the extent that these gender differences occur, they can make

girls less visible or noticeable than boys, at least in leisure play situations where children or youth choose their

companions freely. As with physical differences, however, keep in mind that differences in social interactions do

not occur uniformly for all boys and girls. There are boys with close friends, contradicting the general trend, and

girls who play primarily in large groups.

Differences in social interaction styles happen in the classroom as well. Boys, on average, are more likely to

speak up during a class discussion—sometimes even if not called on, or even if they do not know as much about the

topic as others in the class (Sadker, 2002). When working on a project in a small co-ed group, furthermore they

have a tendency to ignore girls’ comments and contributions to the group. In this respect co-ed student groups

parallel interaction patterns in many parts of society, where men also have a tendency to ignore women’s comments

and contributions (Tannen, 2001).

Academic and cognitive differences in gender

On average, girls are more motivated than boys to perform well in school, at least during elementary school. By

the time girls reach high school, however, some may try to down play their own academic ability in order make

themselves more likeable by both sexes (Davies, 2005). Even if this occurs, though, it does not affect their grades:

from kindergarten through twelfth grade, girls earn slightly higher average grades than boys (Freeman, 2004). This

fact does not lead to similar achievement, however, because as youngsters move into high school, they tend to

choose courses or subjects conventionally associated with their gender—math and science for boys, in particular,

and literature and the arts for girls. By the end of high school, this difference in course selection makes a

measurable difference in boys’ and girls’ academic performance in these subjects.

But again, consider my caution about stereotyping: there are individuals of both sexes whose behaviors and

choices run counter to the group trends. (I have made this point as well in “Preparing for Licensure: Interpreting

Gender-Related Behavior” by deliberately concealing the gender of a student described.) Differences within each

gender group generally are far larger than any differences between the groups. A good example is the “difference” in

cognitive ability of boys and girls. Many studies have found none at all. A few others have found small differences,

with boys slightly better at math and girls slightly better at reading and literature. Still other studies have found the

73

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

differences not only are small, but have been getting smaller in recent years compared to earlier studies.

Collectively the findings about cognitive abilities are virtually “non-findings”, and it is worth asking why gender

differences have therefore been studied and discussed so much for so many years (Hyde, 2005). How teachers

influence gender roles?

Teachers often intend to interact with both sexes equally, and frequently succeed at doing so. Research has

found, though, that they do sometimes respond to boys and girls differently, perhaps without realizing it. Three

kinds of differences have been noticed. The first is the overall amount of attention paid to each sex; the second is

the visibility or “publicity” of conversations; and the third is the type of behavior that prompts teachers to support

or criticize students.

Attention paid

In general, teachers interact with boys more often than with girls by a margin of 10 to 30 percent, depending on

the grade level of the students and the personality of the teacher (Measor & Sykes, 1992). One possible reason for

the difference is related to the greater assertiveness of boys that I already noted; if boys are speaking up more

frequently in discussions or at other times, then a teacher may be “forced” to pay more attention to them. Another

possibility is that some teachers may feel that boys are especially prone to getting into mischief, so they may

interact with them more frequently to keep them focused on the task at hand (Erden & Wolfgang, 2004). Still

another possibility is that boys, compared to girls, may interact in a wider variety of styles and situations, so there

may simply be richer opportunities to interact with them. This last possibility is partially supported by another

gender difference in classroom interaction, the amount of public versus private talk.

Public talk versus private talk

Teachers have a tendency to talk to boys from a greater physical distance than when they talk to girls (Wilkinson

& Marrett, 1985). The difference may be both a cause and an effect of general gender expectations, expressive

nurturing is expected more often of girls and women, and a businesslike task orientation is expected more often of

boys and men, particularly in mixed-sex groups (Basow & Rubenfeld, 2003; Myaskovsky, Unikel, & Dew, 2005).

Whatever the reason, the effect is to give interactions with boys more “publicity”. When two people converse with

each other from across the classroom, many others can overhear them; when they are at each other’s elbows,

though, few others can overhear.

Distributing praise and criticism

In spite of most teachers’ desire to be fair to all students, it turns out that they sometimes distribute praise and

criticism differently to boys and girls. The differences are summarized in Table 4.2. The tendency is to praise boys

more than girls for displaying knowledge correctly, but to criticize girls more than boys for displaying knowledge

incorrectly (Golombok & Fivush, 1994; Delamont, 1996). Another way of stating this difference is by what teachers

tend to overlook: with boys, they tend to overlook wrong answers, but with girls, they tend to overlook right

answers. The result (which is probably unintended) is a tendency to make boys’ knowledge seem more important

and boys themselves more competent. A second result is the other side of this coin: a tendency to make girls’

knowledge less visible and girls themselves less competent.

Table 12: Gender differences in how teachers praise and criticize students

Type of response from Boys Girls

Educational Psychology 74 A Global Text

4. Student diversity

teacher

Praises Correct knowledge “Good” or compliant behavior

Overlooks or ignores “Good” or compliant behavior;

incorrect knowledge

Misbehavior; correct knowledge

Criticizes Misbehavior Incorrect knowledge

Source: Golobuk & Fivush, 1994

Gender differences also occur in the realm of classroom behavior. Teachers tend to praise girls for “good”

behavior, regardless of its relevance to content or to the lesson at hand, and tend to criticize boys for “bad” or

inappropriate behavior (Golombok & Fivush, 1994). This difference can also be stated in terms of what teachers

overlook: with girls, they tend to overlook behavior that is not appropriate, but with boys they tend to overlook

behavior that is appropriate. The net result in this case is to make girls’ seem more good than they may really be,

and also to make their “goodness” seem more important than their academic competence. By the same token, the

teacher’s patterns of response imply that boys are more “bad” than they may really be.

At first glance, the gender differences in interaction can seem discouraging and critical of teachers because they

imply that teachers as a group are biased about gender. But this conclusion is too simplistic for a couple of reasons.

One is that like all differences between groups, interaction patterns are trends, and as such they hide a lot of

variation within them. The other is that the trends suggest what often tends in fact to happen, not what can in fact

happen if a teacher consciously sets about to avoid interaction patterns like the ones I have described. Fortunately

for us all, teaching does not need to be unthinking; we have choices that we can make, even during a busy class!

Differences in cultural expectations and styles

A culture is the system of attitudes, beliefs, and behaviors that constitute the distinctive way of life of a people.

Although sometimes the term is also used to refer specifically to the artistic, intellectual and other “high-brow”

aspects of life, I use it here more broadly to refer to everything that characterizes a way of life—baseball games as

well as symphony concerts, and McDonald’s as well as expensive restaurants. In this broad sense culture is nearly

synonymous with ethnicity, which refers to the common language, history, and future experienced by a group

within society. Culture has elements that are obvious, like unique holidays or customs, but also features that are

subtle or easy for outsiders to overlook, like beliefs about the nature of intelligence or about the proper way to tell a

story. When a classroom draws students from many cultures or ethnic groups, therefore, the students bring to it

considerable diversity. Teachers need to understand that diversity—understand how students’ habitual attitudes,

beliefs, and behaviors differ from each other, and especially how they differ from the teacher’s.

But this kind of understanding can get complicated. To organize the topic, therefore, I will discuss aspects of

cultural diversity according to how directly they relate to language differences compared to differences in other

social and psychological features of culture. The distinction is convenient, but it is also a bit arbitrary because, as

you will see, the features of a culture overlap and influence each other.

75

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Bilingualism: language differences in the classroom

Although monolingual speakers often do not realize it, the majority of children around the world are bilingual,

meaning that they understand and use two languages (Meyers-Scotton, 2005). Even in the United States, which is a

relatively monolingual society, more than 47 million people speak a language other than English at home, and

about 10 million of these people were children or youths in public schools (United States Department of Commerce,

2003). The large majority of bilingual students (75 per cent) are Hispanic, but the rest represent more than a

hundred different language groups from around the world. In larger communities throughout the United States, it

is therefore common for a single classroom to contain students from several language backgrounds at once.

In classrooms as in other social settings, bilingualism exists in different forms and degrees. At one extreme are

students who speak both English and another language fluently; at the other extreme are those who speak only

limited versions of both languages. In between are students who speak their home (or heritage) language much

better than English, as well as others who have partially lost their heritage language in the process of learning

English (Tse, 2001). Commonly, too, a student may speak a language satisfactorily, but be challenged by reading or

writing it—though even this pattern has individual exceptions. Whatever the case, each bilingual student poses

unique challenges to teachers.

Balanced or fluent bilingualism

The student who speaks both languages fluently has a definite cognitive advantage. As you might suspect and as

research has confirmed, a fully fluent bilingual student is in a better position than usual to express concepts or

ideas in more than one way, and to be aware of doing so (Jimenez, et al. 1995; Francis, 2006). The question: “What

if a dog were called a cat?” is less likely to confuse even a very young bilingual child. Nor will the follow-up

question: “Could the ‘cat’ meow?” confuse them. Such skill in reflecting on language is a form of metacognition,

which I discussed in Chapter 2 and defined as using language as an object of thought. Metacognition can be helpful

for a variety of academic purposes, such as writing stories and essays, or interpreting complex text materials.

Unbalanced bilingualism

Unfortunately, the bilingualism of many students is “unbalanced” in the sense that they are either still learning

English, or else they have lost some earlier ability to use their original, heritage language—or occasionally a bit of

both. The first sort of student—sometimes called an English language learner (ELL) or limited English learner

(LEL)—has received the greatest attention and concern from educators, since English is the dominant language of

instruction and skill and obviously helps prepare a student for life in American society. ELL students essentially

present teachers with this dilemma: how to respect the original language and culture of the student while also

helping the student to join more fully in the mainstream—i.e. English-speaking—culture? Programs to address this

question have ranged from total immersion in English from a young age (the “sink or swim” approach) to phasing

in English over a period of several years (sometimes called an additive approach to bilingual education). In general,

evaluations of bilingual programs have favored the more additive approaches (Beykont, 2002). Both languages are

developed and supported, and students ideally become able to use either language permanently, though often for

different situations or purposes. A student may end up using English in the classroom or at work, for example, but

continue using Spanish at home or with friends, even though he or she is perfectly capable of speaking English with

them.

Educational Psychology 76 A Global Text

4. Student diversity

Language loss

What about the other kind of imbalance, in which a student is acquiring English but losing ability with the

student’s home or heritage language? This sort of bilingualism is quite common in the United States and other

nations with immigrant populations (Tse, 2001). Imagine this situation: First-generation immigrants arrive, and

they soon learn just enough English to manage their work and daily needs, but continue using their original

language at home with family and friends from their former country. Their children, however, experience strong

expectations and pressure to learn and use English, and this circumstance dilutes the children’s experience with the

heritage language. By the time the children become adults, they are likely to speak and write English better than

their heritage language, and may even be unable or unwilling to use the heritage language with their own children

(the grandchildren of the original immigrants).

This situation might not at first seem like a problem for which we, as teachers, need to take responsibility, since

the children immigrants, as students, are acquiring the dominant language of instruction. In fact, however, things

are not that simple. Research finds that language loss limits students’ ability to learn English as well or as quickly as

they otherwise can do. Having a large vocabulary in a first language, for example, has been shown to save time in

learning vocabulary in a second language (Hansen, Umeda & McKinney, 2002). But students can only realize the

savings if their first language is preserved. Preserving the first language is also important if a student has impaired

skill in all languages and therefore needs intervention or help from a speech-language specialist. Research has

found, in such cases, that the specialist can be more effective if the specialist speaks and uses the first language as

well as English (Kohnert, et al., 2005). Generally, though also more indirectly, minimizing language loss helps all

bilingual students’ education because preservation tends to enrich students’ and parents’ ability to communicate

with each other. With two languages to work with, parents can stay “in the loop” better about their children’s

educations and support the teacher’s work—for example, by assisting more effectively with homework (Ebert,

2005).

Note that in the early years of schooling, language loss can be minimized to some extent by the additive or

parallel-track bilingual programs that I mentioned above. For a few years, though not forever, young students are

encouraged to use both of their languages. In high school, in addition, some conventional foreign language classes—

notably in Spanish—can be adjusted to include and support students who are already native speakers of the

language alongside students who are learning it for the first time (Tse, 2001). But for heritage languages not

normally offered as “foreign” languages in school, of course, this approach will not work. Such languages are

especially at risk for being lost.

Cultural differences in language use

Cultures and ethnic groups differ not only in languages, but also in how languages are used. Since some of the

patterns differ from those typical of modern classrooms, they can create misunderstandings between teachers and

students (Cazden, 2001; Rogers, et al., 2005). Consider these examples: In some cultures, it is considered polite or

even intelligent not to speak unless you have something truly important to say. “Chitchat”, or talk that simply

affirms a personal tie between people, is considered immature or intrusive (Minami, 2002). In a classroom, this

habit can make it easier for a child to learn not to interrupt others, but it can also make the child seem unfriendly.

77

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

• Eye contact varies by culture. In many African American and Latin American communities, it is considered

appropriate and respectful for a child not to look directly at an adult who is speaking to them (Torres-

Guzman, 1998). In classrooms, however, teachers often expect a lot of eye contact (as in “I want all eyes on

me!”) and may be tempted to construe lack of eye contact as a sign of indifference or disrespect.

• Social distance varies by culture. In some cultures, it is common to stand relatively close when having a

conversation; in others, it is more customary to stand relatively far apart (Beaulieu, 2004). Problems may

happen when a teacher and a student prefer different social distances. A student who expects a closer

distance than does the teacher may seem overly familiar or intrusive, whereas one who expects a longer

distance may seem overly formal or hesitant.

• Wait time varies by culture. Wait time is the gap between the end of one person’s comment or question and

the next person’s reply or answer. In some cultures wait time is relatively long—as long as three or four

seconds (Tharp & Gallimore, 1989). In others it is a “negative” gap, meaning that it is acceptable, even

expected, for a person to interrupt before the end of the previous comment. In classrooms the wait time is

customarily about one second; after that, the teacher is likely to move on to another question or to another

student. A student who habitually expects a wait time long than one second may seem hesitant, and not be

given many chances to speak. A student who expects a “negative” wait time, on the other hand, may seem

overeager or even rude.

• In most non-Anglo cultures, questions are intended to gain information, and it is assumed that a person

asking the question truly does not have the information requested (Rogoff, 2003). In most classrooms,

however, teachers regularly ask test questions, which are questions to which the teacher already knows the

answer and that simply assess whether a student knows the answer as well (Macbeth, 2003). The question:

“How much is 2 + 2?” for example, is a test question. If the student is not aware of this purpose, he or she

may become confused, or think that the teacher is surprisingly ignorant! Worse yet, the student may feel

that the teacher is trying deliberately to shame the student by revealing the student’s ignorance or

incompetence to others.

Cultural differences in attitudes and beliefs

In addition to differences in language and in practices related to language, cultural groups differ in a variety of

other attitudes and beliefs. Complete descriptions of the details of the differences have filled entire books of

encyclopedias (see, for example, Birx, 2005). For teachers, however, the most important ones center on beliefs

about identity, or the sense of self, or of “who you are”. A number of other cultural beliefs and practices can be

understood as resulting from how members of a culture think about personal identity.

In white, middle-class American culture, the self is usually thought of as unique and independent—a unitary,

living source of decisions, choices, and actions that stands (or should eventually stand) by itself (Greenfield, et al.,

2003; Rogoff, 2003). This view of the self is well entrenched in schools, as for example when students are expected

to take responsibility for their own successes or failures and when they are tested and evaluated individually rather

than as a group or team. As teachers, furthermore, most of us subscribe to the idea that all students are unique,

even if we cannot implement this idea fully in teaching because of the constraints of large classes. Whatever the

circumstances, teachers tend to believe in an independent self.

Educational Psychology 78 A Global Text

4. Student diversity

To a greater or lesser extent, however, the majority of non-white cultures and ethnic groups believe in something

closer to an interdependent self, or a belief that it is your relationships and responsibilities, and not uniqueness

and autonomy, that defines a person (Greenfield, 1994; Greenfield, et al., 2003). In these cultures, the most worthy

person is not the one who is unusual or who stands out in a crowd. Such a person might actually be regarded as

lonely or isolated. The worthy person is instead the one who gets along well with family and friends, and who meets

obligations to them reliably and skillfully. At some level, of course, we all value interpersonal skill and to this extent

think of ourselves as interdependent. The difference between individual and interdependent self is one of emphasis,

with many non-white cultures emphasizing interdependence significantly more than white middle-class society in

general and more than schools in particular.

There can be consequences of the difference in how the students respond to school. Here are some of the

possibilities—though keep in mind that there are also differences among students as individuals, whatever their

cultural background. I am talking about tendencies, not straightforward predictions.

• Preference for activities that are cooperative rather than competitive: Many activities in school are

competitive, even when teachers try to de-emphasize the competition. Once past the first year or second

year of school, students often become attentive to who receives the highest marks on an assignment, for

example, or who is the best athlete at various sports or whose contributions to class discussion the most

verbal recognition from the teacher (Johnson & Johnson, 1998). Suppose, in addition, that a teacher

deliberately organizes important activities or assignments competitively (as in “Let’s see who finishes the

math sheet first.”). Classroom life can then become explicitly competitive, and the competitive atmosphere

can interfere with cultivating supportive relationships among students or between students and the teacher

(Cohen, 2004). For students who give priority to these relationships, competition can seem confusing at

best and threatening at worst. What sort of sharing or helping with answers, the student may ask, is truly

legitimate? If the teacher answers this question more narrowly than does the student, then what the

student views as cooperative sharing may be seen by the teacher as laziness, “freeloading”, or even cheating.

• Avoidance of standing out publicly: Even when we, as teachers, avoid obvious forms of competition, we

may still interact frequently with students one at a time while allowing or inviting many others to observe

the conversation. An especially common pattern for such conversations is sometimes called the IRE cycle,

an abbreviation for the teacher initiating, a student responding, and the teacher then evaluating the

response (Mehan, 1979). What is sometimes taken for granted is how often IRE cycles are witnessed

publicly, and how much the publicity can be stressful or embarrassing for students who do not value

standing out in a group but who do value belonging to the group. The embarrassment can be especially

acute if they feel unsure about whether they have correct knowledge or skill to display. To keep such

students from “clamming up” completely, therefore, teachers should consider limiting IRE cycles to times

when they are truly productive. IRE conversations may often work best when talking with a student

privately, or when confirming knowledge that the student is likely to be able to display competently already,

or when “choral” speaking (responding together in unison) is appropriate.

• Interpersonal time versus clock time: In order to function, all schools rely on fairly precise units of time as

measured on clocks. Teachers typically allot a fixed number of minutes to one lesson or class, another fixed

79

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

number of minutes for the next, another for recess or lunch time, and so on. In more ways than one,

therefore, being on time becomes especially valued in schools, as it is in many parts of society. Punctuality

is not always conducive, however, to strong personal relationships, which develop best when individuals do

not end joint activities unilaterally or arbitrarily, but allow activities to “finish themselves”, so to speak—to

finish naturally. If personal relationships are a broad, important priority for a student, therefore, it may

take effort and practice by the student to learn the extent to which schools and teachers expect punctuality.

Punctuality includes the obvious, like showing up for school when school is actually scheduled to begin. But

it also includes subtleties, like starting and finishing tasks when the teacher tells students to do so, or

answering a question promptly at the time it is asked rather than sometime later when discussion has

already moved on.

Accommodating diversity in practice

Hopefully I have persuaded you—if you did need persuading—that students are indeed diverse. The important

question that follows from this point is what to do about the diversity. I have begun answering that question by

including a number of suggestions in the sections and paragraphs of this chapter. But there is obviously more to be

said about accommodating diversity—about actually working with students’ diversity and turning it into a resource

rather than a burden or challenge. In the rest of this book therefore I offer more suggestions not only about

knowing how different one student can be from another, but also about diversifying teaching to acknowledge this

fact. Differences among students remain a challenge during all phases of teaching, from planning instruction, to

implementing lessons and activities, to assessing students’ learning after lessons or activities are all finished. In the

next chapter, I illustrate this reality by describing how students with disabilities can be included in classroom life—

one of the more telling examples of accommodating to diversity.

Chapter summary

Students differ in a multitude of ways, both individually and as groups. Individually, for example, students have

a preferred learning style as well as preferred cognitive or thinking styles. They also have unique profiles or

intelligence or competence that affect how and what they learn most successfully.

In addition to individual diversity, students tend to differ according to their gender, although there are

numerous individual exceptions. Motor abilities as well as motivation and experience with athletics gradually

differentiate boys and girls, especially when they reach and begin high school. Socially, boys tend to adopt

relationships that are more active and wide-ranging than do girls. Academically, girls tend to be a bit more

motivated to receive slightly higher marks in school. Teachers sometimes contribute to gender role differences—

perhaps without intending—by paying attention to boys more frequently and more publicly in class, and by

distributing praise and criticism in ways differentiated by sex.

Students also differ according to cultures, language, and ethnic groups of their families. Many students are

bilingual, with educational consequences that depend on their fluency in each of their two languages. If they have

more difficulty with English, then programs that add their first language together with English have proved to be

helpful. If they have more difficulty with their first language, they are risk for language loss, and the consequences

are also negative even if more hidden from teachers’ views.

Educational Psychology 80 A Global Text

4. Student diversity

In addition to language differences as such, students differ according to culture in how language is used or

practiced—in taking turns at speaking, in eye contact, social distance, wait time, and the use of questions. Some of

these differences in practice stem from cultural differences in attitudes about self-identity, with non-Anglo

culturally tending to support a more interdependent view of the self than Anglo culture or the schools. Differences

in attitudes and in use of language have several consequences for teachers. In particular—where appropriate—they

should consider using cooperative activities, avoid highlighting individuals’ accomplishments or failures, and be

patient about students’ learning to be punctual.

On the Internet

This is the website for the National Association of Bilingual Educators, which represents

both English Language Learners and their teachers. The website offers a variety of information, free of charge,

about all aspects of bilingual education, including introductory summaries of the field, position papers released to

the government and the press, and research articles from their journals.

This website represents the National Association for Single Sex Public

Education, which as its name implies advocates for all-girl and all-boy classes and schools. The website contains

thoughtful summaries of the advantages to both boys and girls if they are educated separately and in public schools.

Whether you agree with their point of view or not, their point of view is worth considering; though keep in mind

that their supporting information tends to come from media sources (e.g. newspapers) instead of full-fledged

research studies.

Key termsAfrican-American English

Balanced bilingualismBilingual

Cognitive stylesCulture

Dialect Ebonics

English language learner (ELL)Ethnicity

Eye contactField dependence

Field independenceGender roles

Group differencesIdentity

Impulsivity

Independent selfIndividual differences

Interdependent selfIRE cycle

Language lossLearning styles

Limited English learner (LEL)Metacognition

Multiple intelligencesReflectivity

Social distanceTest questions

Unbalanced bilingualismWait time

References

Basow, S. & Rubenfeld, K. (2003). “Troubles talk”: Effects of gender and gender-typing. Sex Roles, 48(3/4),

183-188.

Beykont, Z. (Ed.). (2002). The power of culture: Teaching across language difference. Cambridge, MA:

Harvard Education Publishing Group.

Beaulieu, C. (2004). Intercultural study of personal space: A case study. Journal of Applied Social

Psychology, 34(4), 794-805.

81

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Birx, H. J. (2005). Encyclopedia of human anthropology. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.

Bohn, A. (2003). Familiar voices: Using Ebonics communication techniques in the primary classroom.

Urban Education, 38(6), 688-707.

Braddock, J., Sokol-Katz, J., Greene, A., & Basinger-Fleischman, L. (2005). Uneven playing fields: State

variations in boys’ and girls’ access to and participation in high school interscholastic sports. Sociological

Spectrum, 25(2), 231-250.

Cazden, C. (2001). Classroom discourse, 2nd edition. Portsmouth, NH: Heineman Publishers.

Cohen, E. (2004). Teaching cooperative learning: The challenge for teacher education. Albany, NY: State

University of New York Press.

Davies, J. (2005). Expressions of gender: An analysis of pupils’ gendered discourse styles in small group

classroom discussions. Discourse and Society, 14(2), 115-132.

Davis, G. & Rimm, S. (2004). Education of the gifted and talented, 5th edition. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.

Delamont, S. (1996). Women’s place in education. Brookfield, MA: Avebury Publishers.

Ebert, J. (2005). Linguistics: Tongue tied. Nature, 438, 148-149.

Erden, F. & Wolfgang, C. (2004). An exploration of the differences in teachers’ beliefs related to discipline

when dealing with male and female students. Early Child Development and Care, 174(1), 3-11.

Eisner, E. (2004). Multiple intelligences: Its tensions and possibilities. Teachers College Record, 106(1), 31.

Espelage, D. & Swearer, S. (2004). Bullying in American schools: A socio-ecological perspective on

prevention and intervention. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Evans, C. (2004). Exploring the relationship between cognitive style and teaching style. Educational

psychology, 24(4), 509-530.

Francis, N. (2006). The development of secondary discourse ability and metalinguistic awareness in second

language learners. International Journal of Applied Linguistics, 16, 37-47.

Freeman, D. (2004). Trends in educational equity of girls and women. Washington, D.C.: United States

Department of Education, National Center for Educational Statistics.

Friend, M. (2007). Special education: Contemporary perspectives for school professionals, 2nd edition.

Boston: Allyn & Bacon.

Gardner, H. (1983). Frames of mind: The theory of multiple intelligences. New York: Basic Books.

Gardner, H. (2003, April 21). Multiple intelligences after twenty years. Paper presented at the American

Educational Research Association, Chicago, IL.

Garlick, K. (2002). Understanding the nature of the general factor of intelligence. Psychological review,

109(1), 116-136.

Golombok, S. & Fivush, R. (1994). Gender development. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Educational Psychology 82 A Global Text

4. Student diversity

Greenfield, P. (1994). Independence and interdependence as cultural scripts. In P. Greenfield & R. Cocking

(Eds.), Cross-cultural roots of minority child development, pp. 1-40. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Greenfield, P., Keller, H., Fuligni, A., & Maynard, A. (2003). Cultural pathways through universal

development. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 461-490.

Gottfredson, L. (2004). Intelligence: Is it the epidemiologists’ elusive “fundamental cause” of social class

inequalities in health? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86(1), 174-199.

Hansen, L., Umeda, Y., & McKinney, M. (2002). Savings in the relearning of second language vocabulary:

The effects of time and proficiency. Language Learning, 52, 653-663.

Hyde, J. (2005). The gender similarities hypothesis. American Psychologist, 60(6), 581-592.

Jimenez, R., Garcia, G., & Pearson. D. (1995). Three children, two languages, and strategic reading: Case

studies in bilingual/monolingual reading. American Educational Research Journal, 32(1), 67-97.

Johnson, D. & Johnson, R. (1998). Learning together and alone: Cooperative, competitive, and

individualistic learning, 5th edition. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.

Kohn, A. (2004). Test today, privatize tomorrow. Phi Delta Kappan, 85(8), 568-577.

Kohnert, K., Yim, D., Nett, K., Kan, P., & Duran, L. (2005). Intervention with linguistically diverse preschool

children. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 36, 251-263.

Loo, R. (2004). Kolb’s learning styles and learning preferences: Is there a linkage? Educational psychology,

24(1), 99-108.

Lubinski, D. (2004). 100 years after Spearman’s “‘General Intelligence,’ Objectively Determined and

Measured”. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86(1), 96-111.

Macbeth, D. (2003). Hugh Mehan’s “Learning Lessons” reconsidered: On the differences between

naturalistic and critical analysis of classroom discourse. American Educational Research Journal, 40(1),

239-280.

Maccoby, E. (2002). Gender and social exchange: A developmental perspective. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Martinez-Roldan, C. & Malave, G. (2004). Language ideologies mediating literacy and identity in bilingual

contexts. Journal of early childhood literacy, 4(2), 155-180.

Measor, L. & Sykes, P. (1992). Gender and schools. New York: Cassell.

Mehan, H. (1979). Learning lessons: social organization in the classroom. Cambridge, MA: Harvard

University Press.

Messner, M., Dunca, M., & Cooky, C. (2003). Silence, sports bras, and wrestling porn. Journal of Sport and

Social Issues, 27(1), 38-51.

Meyers-Sutton, C. (2005). Multiple voices: An introduction to bilingualism. Malden, MA: Blackwell

Publishers.

83

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Minami, M. (2002). Culture-specific language styles: The development of oral narrative and literacy.

Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters.

Myaskovsky, L, Unikel, E., & Dew, M. (2005). Effects of gender diversity on performance and interpersonal

behavior in small work groups. Sex Roles, 52(9/10), 645-657.

Pritchard, A. (2005). Ways of learning: Learning theories and learning styles in the classroom. London,

UK: David Fulton.

Rogers, R., Malancharuvil-Berkes, E., Mosely, M., Hui, D., & O’Garro, G. (2005). Critical discourse analysis

in education: A review of the literature. Review of Educational Research, 75(3), 365-416.

Rogoff, B. (2003). The culture of human development. New York: Oxford University Press.

Sadker, D. (2002). An educator’s primer on the gender war. Phi Delta Kappan, 84(3), 235-240.

Shiever, S. & Maker, C. (2003). New directions in enrichment and acceleration. In N. Colangelo & G. Davis

(Eds.), Handbook fo gifted education, 3rd edition (pp. 163-173). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.

Stahl, S. (2002). Different strokes for different folks? In L. Abbeduto (Ed.), Taking sides: Clashing on

controversial issue sin educational psychology (pp. 98-107). Guilford, CT: McGraw Hill.

Steiner, H. & Carr, M. (2003). Cognitive development in gifted children: Toward a more precise

understanding of emerging differences in intelligence. Educational Psychology Review, 15, 215-246.

Tannen, D. (2001). You just don’t understand: Men and women in conversation. New York: Quill.

Tharp, R. & Gallimore, R. (1989). Rousing minds to life. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Torres-Guzman, M. (1998). Language culture, and literacy in Puerto Rican communities. In B. Perez (Ed.),

Sociocultural contexts of language and literacy. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Tse, L. (2001). Why don’t they learn English? New York: Teachers’ College Press.

United States Department of Commerce, Bureau of the Census. (2003). American community survey.

Washington, D.C.: Author.

Wilkinson, L. & Marrett, C. (Eds.). (1985). Gender influences in classroom interaction. Orlando, FL:

Academic Press.

Zhang, L. & Sternberg, R. (2005). Three-fold model of intellectual styles. Educational psychology review,

17(1).

Zhang, L. & Sternberg, R. (2006). The nature of intellectual styles. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum

Educational Psychology 84 A Global Text

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

5. Students with special educational needs

Three people on the margins

The first person: In 1761 a six-year-old girl was captured from West Africa, given the name Phillis

Wheatley, and sold into slavery in the City of Boston in Colonial America. By the time she was 17,

Phillis had taught herself to read and write and had developed a special love and talent for poetry. Her

owner was a wealthy businessman and sought to improve his reputation by publishing an anthology of

her poems. Unfortunately he encountered stiff resistance from publishers because few people at that

time believed Africans to be capable of the thought and imagination needed to write poetry. People

who heard of her poetry were skeptical and inclined to think that it was faked. Eventually, to save his

own reputation, the owner assembled a tribunal of 18 prominent judges—including the governor of

Massachusetts and John Hancock, one of the signers of the Declaration of Independence—to assess the

young woman’s mental capacity. After cross-examining her, the judges finally decided that Ms

Wheatley was, after all, capable of writing poetry (Robinson, 1982).

The second person: A century later, a child named Helen Keller lost her sight and hearing as a

result of illness during infancy. In spite of this misfortune, though, Helen devised a language of

gestural signs for communicating with a tutor, and was soon also using Braille to study both French

and Latin. At ten, she wrote and published a short story. Yet like Ms Wheatley, Ms Keller also faced

substantial, chronic skepticism about her capacities. Prominent educators accused her of plagiarizing

others’ writings and merely “parroting” others’ ideas without understanding them (Keller, 1954;

Bogdan, 2006). Eventually, as with Wheatley, a panel was assembled—though this time the members

were professional experts about disabilities—to determine whether Ms Keller was in fact capable of

writing what she published. The panel decided that she was indeed capable, though only by a slim

margin (five judges vs four).

The third person: In 1978, Sue Rubin was born with a disability that limited her speech to

disordered bursts of sound and occasionally echoing phrases of other people. She was labeled autistic

because of her symptoms, and assumed to be profoundly retarded. With support and encouragement

from her mother and others, however, Sue eventually learned to type on a keyboard without assistance.

She learned to communicate effectively when she was about 13 and was able to go to school. Since then

she has made many presentations about autism at conferences and recently co-edited a book about

autism, titled Autism: The Myth of the Person Alone (Bogdan, et al., 2005).

One of these individuals experienced racial discrimination and the other two experienced physical disabilities,

but notice something important: that all three were defined by society as disabled intellectually. Initially, their

Educational Psychology 85 A Global Text

5. Students with special educational needs

achievements were dismissed because of widespread assumptions—whether about race or disability—of their

inherent incompetence. All three had to work harder than usual, not only to acquire literacy itself, but also to prove

that their literacy was genuine and worthy of respect.

Since the time of Phillis Wheatley, North American society has eliminated slavery and made some progress at

reducing certain forms of racism, though much remains to be done. In 1954, for example, the United States

Supreme Court ruled that public schools could not be segregated by race, and in doing so recognized, at least

legally, the intellectual competence of African-Americans as well as the moral obligation of society to provide all

citizens with the best possible education. It has taken longer to recognize legally the rights and competence of

persons with disabilities, but events and trends beginning in the 1970s have begun to make it happen. This chapter

begins by explaining some of these and how they have altered the work of teachers.

Growing support for people with disabilities: legislation and its effects

Since the 1970s political and social attitudes have moved increasingly toward including people with disabilities

into a wide variety of “regular” activities. In the United States, the shift is illustrated clearly in the Federal

legislation that was enacted during this time. The legislation partly stimulated the change in attitudes, but at the

same time they partly resulted from the change. Three major laws were passed that guaranteed the rights of

persons with disabilities, and of children and students with disabilities in particular. Although the first two affected

teachers’ work in the classroom, the third has had the biggest impact on education.

Rehabilitation Act of 1973, Section 504

This law—the first of its kind—required that individuals with disabilities be accommodated in any program or

activity that receives Federal funding (PL 93-112, 1973). Although this law was not intended specifically for

education, in practice it has protected students’ rights in some extra-curricular activities (for older students) and in

some child care or after-school care programs (for younger students). If those programs receive Federal funding of

any kind, the programs are not allowed to exclude children or youths with disabilities, and they have to find

reasonable ways to accommodate the individuals’ disabilities.

Americans with Disabilities Act of 1990 (or ADA).

This legislation also prohibited discrimination on the basis of disability, just as Section 504 of the Rehabilitation

Act had done (PL 101-336, 1990). Although the ADA also applies to all people (not just to students), its provisions

are more specific and “stronger” than those of Section 504. In particular, ADA extends to all employment and jobs,

not just those receiving Federal funding. It also specifically requires accommodations to be made in public facilities

such as with buses, restrooms, and telephones. ADA legislation is therefore responsible for some of the “minor”

renovations in schools that you may have noticed in recent years, like wheelchair-accessible doors, ramps, and

restrooms, and public telephones with volume controls.

Individuals with Disabilities Education Act (or IDEA)

As its name implied this legislation was more focused on education than either Section 504 or ADA. It was first

passed in 1975 and has been amended several times since, including most recently in 2004 (PL 108-446, 2004). In

its current form, the law guarantees the following rights related to education for anyone with a disability from birth

to age 21. The first two influence schooling in general, but the last three affect the work of classroom teachers rather

directly:

86

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

• Free, appropriate education: An individual or an individual’s family should not have to pay for education

simply because the individual has a disability, and the educational program should be truly educational (i.e.

not merely care-taking or “babysitting” of the person).

• Due process: In case of disagreements between an individual with a disability and the schools or other

professionals, there must be procedures for resolving the disagreements that are fair and accessible to all

parties—including the person himself or herself or the person’s representative.

• Fair evaluation of performance in spite of disability: Tests or other evaluations should not assume test-

taking skills that a person with a disability cannot reasonably be expected to have, such as holding a pencil,

hearing or seeing questions, working quickly, or understanding and speaking orally. Evaluation procedures

should be modified to allow for these differences. This provision of the law applies both to evaluations made

by teachers and to school-wide or “high-stakes” testing programs.

• Education in the “least restrictive environment”: Education for someone with a disability should provide as

many educational opportunities and options for the person as possible, both in the short term and in the

long term. In practice this requirement has meant including students in regular classrooms and school

activities as much as possible, though often not totally.

• An individualized educational program: Given that every disability is unique, instructional planning for a

person with a disability should be unique or individualized as well. In practice this provision has led to

classroom teachers planning individualized programs jointly with other professionals (like reading

specialists, psychologists, or medical personnel) as part of a team.

Considered together, these provisions are both a cause and an effect of basic democratic philosophy. The

legislation says, in effect, that all individuals should have access to society in general and to education in particular.

Although teachers certainly support this philosophy in broad terms, and many have welcomed the IDEA legislation,

others have found the prospect of applying it in classrooms leads to a number of questions and concerns. Some ask,

for example, whether a student with a disability will disrupt the class; others, whether the student will interfere

with covering the curriculum; still others, whether the student might be teased by classmates. Since these are

legitimate concerns, I will return to them at the end of this chapter. First, however, let me clarify exactly how the

IDEA legislation affects the work of teachers, and then describe in more detail the major disabilities that you are

likely to encounter in students.

Responsibilities of teachers for students with disabilities

The IDEA legislation has affected the work of teachers by creating three new expectations. The first expectation

is to provide alternative methods of assessment for students with disabilities; the second is to arrange a learning

environment that is as normal or as “least restrictive” as possible; and the third is to participate in creating

individual educational plans for students with disabilities.

Alternative assessments

In the context of students with disabilities, assessment refers to gathering information about a student in

order both to identify the strengths of the student, and to decide what special educational support, if any, the

student needs. In principle, of course, these are tasks that teachers have for all students: assessment is a major

Educational Psychology 87 A Global Text

5. Students with special educational needs

reason why we give tests and assignments, for example, and why we listen carefully to the quality of students’

comments during class discussions. For students with disabilities, however, such traditional or conventional

strategies of assessment often seriously underestimate the students’ competence (Koretz & Barton, 2003/2004;

Pullin, 2005). Depending on the disability, a student may have trouble with (a) holding a pencil, (b) hearing a

question clearly, (c) focusing on a picture, (d) marking an answer in time even when he or she knows the answer,

(e) concentrating on a task in the presence of other people, or (f) answering a question at the pace needed by the

rest of the class. Traditionally, teachers have assumed that all students either have these skills or can learn them

with just modest amounts of coaching, encouragement, and will power. For many other students, for example, it

may be enough to say something like: “Remember to listen to the question carefully!” For students with disabilities,

however, a comment like this may not work and may even be insensitive. A student with visual impairment does

not need be reminded to “look closely at what I am writing on the board”; doing so will not cause the student to see

the chalkboard more clearly—though the reminder might increase the student’s anxiety and self-consciousness.

There are a number of strategies for modifying assessments in ways that attempt to be fair and that at the same

time recognize how busy teachers usually are. One is to consider supplementing conventional assignments or tests

with portfolios, which are collections of a student’s work that demonstrate a student’s development over time, and

which usually include some sort of reflective or evaluative comments from the student, the teacher, or both

(Carothers & Taylor, 2003; Wesson & King, 1996). Another is to devise a system for observing the student regularly,

even if briefly, and informally recording notes about the observations for later consideration and assessment. A

third strategy is to recruit help from teacher assistants, who are sometimes present to help a student with a

disability; an assistant can often conduct a brief test or activity with the student, and later report on and discuss the

results with you.

If you reflect on these strategies, you may realize that they may sometimes create issues about fairness. If a

student with a disability demonstrates competence one way but other students demonstrate it another, should they

be given similar credit? On the other hand, is it fair for one student to get a lower mark because the student lacks an

ability—such as normal hearing—that teachers cannot, in principle, ever teach? These ethical issues are legitimate

and important, and I therefore return to them in Chapters 11 and 12, which discuss assessment in much more

detail.

Least restrictive environment

The IDEA legislation calls for placing students with disabilities in the least restrictive environment (or

LRE), defined as the combination of settings that involve the student with regular classrooms and school programs

as much as possible. The precise combination is determined by the circumstances of a particular school and of the

student. A kindergarten child with a mild cognitive disability, for example, may spend the majority of time in a

regular kindergarten class, working alongside and playing with non-disabled classmates and relying on a teacher

assistant for help where needed. An individual with a similar disability in high school, however, might be assigned

primarily to classes specially intended for slow learners, but nonetheless participate in some school-wide activities

alongside non-disabled students. The difference in LREs might reflect teachers’ perceptions of how difficult it is to

modify the curriculum in each case; rightly or wrongly, teachers are apt to regard adaptation as more challenging at

“higher” grade levels. By the same token, a student with a disability that is strictly physical might spend virtually all

88

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

his or her time in regular classes throughout the student’s school career; in this case, adjustment of the curriculum

would not be an issue.

For you, the policy favoring the least restrictive environment means that if you continue teaching long enough,

you will very likely encounter a student with a disability in one or more of your classes, or at least have one in a

school-related activity for which you are responsible. It also means that the special educational needs of these

students will most often be the “mildest”. Statistically, the most frequent forms of special needs are learning

disabilities, which are impairments in specific aspects of learning, and especially of reading. Learning disabilities

account for about half of all special educational needs—as much as all other types put together. Somewhat less

common are speech and language disorders, cognitive disabilities, and attention deficit hyperactivity disorders

(or ADHD). Because of their frequency and of the likelihood that you will meet students for whom these labels have

been considered, I describe them more fully later in this chapter, along with other disability conditions that you will

encounter much less frequently.

Individual educational plan

The third way that IDEA legislation and current educational approaches affect teachers is by requiring teachers

and other professional staff to develop an annual individual educational plan (or IEP) for each student with a

disability. The plan is created by a team of individuals who know the student’s strengths and needs; at a minimum

it includes one or more classroom teachers, a “resource” or special education teacher, and the student’s parents or

guardians. Sometimes, too, the team includes a school administrator (like a vice-principal) or other professionals

from outside the school (like a psychologist or physician), depending on the nature of the child’s disability. An IEP

can take many forms, but it always describes a student’s current social and academic strengths as well as the

student’s social or academic needs. It also specifies educational goals or objectives for the coming year, lists special

services to be provided, and describes how progress toward the goals will be assessed at the end of the year. Exhibit

6 shows a simple, imaginary IEP. (But keep in mind that the actual visual formats of IEP plans vary widely among

states, provinces, and school jurisdictions.) This particular plan is for a student named Sean, a boy having

difficulties with reading. IEPs, like the one in the figure, originally served mainly students in the younger grades,

but more recently they have been extended and modified to serve transition planning for adolescents with

disabilities who are approaching the end of their public schooling (West, et al., 1999). For these students, the goals

of the plan often include activities (like finding employment) to extend beyond schooling. See below.

Educational Psychology 89 A Global Text

5. Students with special educational needs

Student: Sean Cortinez Birth Date: 26 May 2002 Period Covered by IEP:

September 20xx – July 20xy

Address: Phone:

School: Grant Park Middle

School

Grade Level: 3 Teacher(s): G. Eidse

Support Team

List specialists (educational, medical, or other) involved in assisting the student:

Resource teacher, instructional aide (part time):

Special Curriculum Needs to be Addressed:

List general needs here; use separate sheet(s) for specific, short-term objectives as appropriate:

Sean can read short, familiar words singly, but cannot read connected text even when familiar. Needs help

especially with decoding and other “word attack” skills. Some trouble focusing on reading tasks. Sean speaks

clearly and often listens well when the topic interests him.

Special Materials or Equipment Needed:

Modified test procedures and reading materials as required.

Signatures:

Parent or guardian: K. Cortinez

Teacher(s): G. Eidse

Principal: L. Stauffer

Date of IEP Meeting: 26 October 20xx

Exhibit 6: A sample individual educational plan. (Note that actual visual formats of IEP plans vary.)

If you have a student with an IEP, you can expect two consequences for teaching. The first is that you should

expect to make definite, clear plans for the student, and to put the plans in writing. This consequence does not, of

90

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

course, prevent you from taking advantage of unexpected or spontaneous classroom events as well in order to

enrich the curriculum. But it does mean that an educational program for a student with a disability cannot consist

only of the unexpected or spontaneous. The second consequence is that you should not expect to construct an

educational plan alone, as is commonly done when planning regular classroom programs. When it comes to

students with disabilities, expect instead to plan as part of a team. Working with others ensures that everyone who

is concerned about the student has a voice. It also makes it possible to improve the quality of IEPs by pooling ideas

from many sources—even if, as you might suspect, it also challenges professionals to communicate clearly and

cooperate respectfully with team members in order to serve a student as well as possible.

Categories of disabilities—and their ambiguities

So far I have said a lot about why inclusion has come to be important for teachers, but not much about the actual

nature of students’ disabilities. Part of the reason for delaying was because, to put it simply, disabilities are

inherently ambiguous. Naming and describing “types” of them implies that disabilities are relatively fixed, stable,

and distinct, like different kinds of fruit or vegetables. As many teachers discover, though, the reality is somewhat

different. The behavior and qualities of a particular student with a disability can be hard to categorize. The student

may be challenged not only by the disability, but also by experiences common to all students, disabled or not. Any

particular disability, furthermore, poses problems more in some situations than in others. A student with a reading

difficulty may have trouble in a language arts class, for example, but not in a physical education class; a student

with a hearing impairment may have more trouble “hearing” a topic that he dislikes compared to one that he likes.

Because official descriptions of types or categories of disabilities overlook these complexities, they risk stereotyping

the real, live people to whom they are applied (Green, et al., 2005). Even the simplifications might not be a serious

problem if the resulting stereotypes were complimentary—most of us would not mind being called a “genius”, for

example, even if the description is not always true. Stereotypes about disabilities, however, are usually stigmatizing,

not complimentary.

Still, categories of disabilities do serve useful purposes by giving teachers, parents, and other professionals a

language or frame of reference for talking about disabilities. They also can help educators when arranging special

support services for students, since a student has to “have” an identifiable, nameable need if professionals are to

provide help. Educational authorities have therefore continued to use categories (or “labels”) to classify disabilities

in spite of expressing continuing concern about whether the practice hurts students’ self-esteem or standing in the

eyes of peers (Biklen & Kliewer, 2006). For classroom teachers, the best strategy may be simply to understand how

categories of disabilities are defined, while also keeping their limitations in mind and being ready to explain their

limitations (tactfully, of course) to parents or others who use the labels inappropriately.

That said, what in fact are the major types of disabilities encountered by teachers? Let us take them one at a

time, beginning with the more common ones.

Learning disabilities

A learning disability (or LD) is a specific impairment of academic learning that interferes with a specific

aspect of schoolwork and that reduces a student’s academic performance significantly. An LD shows itself as a

major discrepancy between a student’s ability and some feature of achievement: the student may be delayed in

reading, writing, listening, speaking, or doing mathematics, but not in all of these at once. A learning problem is not

Educational Psychology 91 A Global Text

5. Students with special educational needs

considered a learning disability if it stems from physical, sensory, or motor handicaps, or from generalized

intellectual impairment (or mental retardation). It is also not an LD if the learning problem really reflects the

challenges of learning English as a second language. Genuine LDs are the learning problems left over after these

other possibilities are accounted for or excluded. Typically, a student with an LD has not been helped by teachers’

ordinary efforts to assist the student when he or she falls behind academically—though what counts as an “ordinary

effort”, of course, differs among teachers, schools, and students. Most importantly, though, an LD relates to a fairly

specific area of academic learning. A student may be able to read and compute well enough, for example, but not be

able to write.

LDs are by far the most common form of special educational need, accounting for half of all students with

special needs in the United States and anywhere from 5 to 20 per cent of all students, depending on how the

numbers are estimated (United States Department of Education, 2005; Ysseldyke & Bielinski, 2002). Students with

LDs are so common, in fact, that most teachers regularly encounter at least one per class in any given school year,

regardless of the grade level they teach.

Defining learning disabilities clearly

With so many students defined as having learning disabilities, it is not surprising that the term itself becomes

ambiguous in the truest sense of “having many meanings”. Specific features of LDs vary considerably. Any of the

following students, for example, qualify as having a learning disability, assuming that they have no other disease,

condition, or circumstance to account for their behavior:

• Albert, an eighth-grader, has trouble solving word problems that he reads, but can solve them easily if he

hears them orally.

• Bill, also in eighth grade, has the reverse problem: he can solve word problems only when he can read them,

not when he hears them.

• Carole, a fifth-grader, constantly makes errors when she reads textual material aloud, either leaving out

words, adding words, or substituting her own words for the printed text.

• Emily, in seventh grade, has terrible handwriting; her letters vary in size and wobble all over the page,

much like a first- or second-grader.

• Denny reads very slowly, even though he is in fourth grade. His comprehension suffers as a result, because

he sometimes forgets what he read at the beginning of a sentence by the time he reaches the end.

• Garnet’s spelling would have to be called “inventive”, even though he has practiced conventionally correct

spelling more than other students. Garnet is in sixth grade.

• Harmin, a ninth-grader has particular trouble decoding individual words and letters if they are unfamiliar;

he reads conceal as “concol” and alternate as “alfoonite”.

• Irma, a tenth-grader, adds multiple-digit numbers as if they were single-digit numbers stuck together: 42 +

59 equals 911 rather than 101, though 23 + 54 correctly equals 77.

With so many expressions of LDs, it is not surprising that educators sometimes disagree about their nature and

about the kind of help students need as a consequence. Such controversy may be inevitable because LDs by

92

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

definition are learning problems with no obvious origin. There is good news, however, from this state of affairs, in

that it opens the way to try a variety of solutions for helping students with learning disabilities.

Assisting students with learning disabilities

There are various ways to assist students with learning disabilities, depending not only on the nature of the

disability, of course, but also on the concepts or theory of learning guiding you. Take Irma, the girl mentioned above

who adds two-digit numbers as if they were one digit numbers. Stated more formally, Irma adds two-digit numbers

without carrying digits forward from the ones column to the tens column, or from the tens to the hundreds column.

Exhibit 7 shows the effect that her strategy has on one of her homework papers. What is going on here and how

could a teacher help Irma?

Directions: Add the following numbers.

42 23 11 47 97 41

+ 59 + 54 + 48 + 23 + 64 + 27

911 77 59 610 1511 68

Three out of the six problems are done correctly, even though Irma seems to use an incorrect

strategy systematically on all six problems.

Exhibit 7: Irma’s math homework about two-digit addition

Behaviorism: reinforcement for wrong strategies

One possible approach comes from the behaviorist theory discussed in Chapter 2. Irma may persist with the

single-digit strategy because it has been reinforced a lot in the past. Maybe she was rewarded so much for adding

single-digit numbers (3+5, 7+8 etc.) correctly that she generalized this skill to two-digit problems—in fact over

generalized it. This explanation is plausible because she would still get many two-digit problems right, as you can

confirm by looking at it. In behaviorist terms, her incorrect strategy would still be reinforced, but now only on a

“partial schedule of reinforcement”. As I pointed out in Chapter 2, partial schedules are especially slow to

extinguish, so Irma persists seemingly indefinitely with treating two-digit problems as if they were single-digit

problems.

From the point of view of behaviorism, changing Irma’s behavior is tricky since the desired behavior (borrowing

correctly) rarely happens and therefore cannot be reinforced very often. It might therefore help for the teacher to

reward behaviors that compete directly with Irma’s inappropriate strategy. The teacher might reduce credit for

simply finding the correct answer, for example, and increase credit for a student showing her work—including the

work of carrying digits forward correctly. Or the teacher might make a point of discussing Irma’s math work with

Irma frequently, so as to create more occasions when she can praise Irma for working problems correctly.

Metacognition and responding reflectively

Part of Irma’s problem may be that she is thoughtless about doing her math: the minute she sees numbers on a

worksheet, she stuffs them into the first arithmetic procedure that comes to mind. Her learning style, that is, seems

Educational Psychology 93 A Global Text

5. Students with special educational needs

too impulsive and not reflective enough, as discussed in Chapter 4. Her style also suggests a failure of

metacognition (remember that idea from Chapter 2?), which is her self-monitoring of her own thinking and its

effectiveness. As a solution, the teacher could encourage Irma to think out loud when she completes two-digit

problems—literally get her to “talk her way through” each problem. If participating in these conversations was

sometimes impractical, the teacher might also arrange for a skilled classmate to take her place some of the time.

Cooperation between Irma and the classmate might help the classmate as well, or even improve overall social

relationships in the classroom.

Constructivism, mentoring, and the zone of proximal development

Perhaps Irma has in fact learned how to carry digits forward, but not learned the procedure well enough to use it

reliably on her own; so she constantly falls back on the earlier, better-learned strategy of single-digit addition. In

that case her problem can be seen in the constructivist terms, like those that I discussed in Chapter 2. In essence,

Irma has lacked appropriate mentoring from someone more expert than herself, someone who can create a “zone of

proximal development” in which she can display and consolidate her skills more successfully. She still needs

mentoring or “assisted coaching” more than independent practice. The teacher can arrange some of this in much

the way she encourages to be more reflective, either by working with Irma herself or by arranging for a classmate or

even a parent volunteer to do so. In this case, however, whoever serves as mentor should not only listen, but also

actively offer Irma help. The help has to be just enough to insure that Irma completes two-digit problems correctly

—neither more nor less. Too much help may prevent Irma from taking responsibility for learning the new strategy,

but too little may cause her to take the responsibility prematurely.

Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder

Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (or ADHD) is a problem with sustaining attention and controlling

impulses. As students, almost all of us have these problems at one time or another, but a student with ADHD shows

them much more frequently than usual, and often at home as well as at school. In the classroom, the student with

ADHD may fidget and squirm a lot, or have trouble remaining seated, or continually get distracted and off task, or

have trouble waiting for a turn, or blurt out answers and comments. The student may shift continually from one

activity to another, or have trouble playing quietly, or talk excessively without listening to others. Or the student

may misplace things and seem generally disorganized, or be inclined to try risky activities without enough thought

to the consequences. Although the list of problem behaviors is obviously quite extensive, keep in mind that the

student will not do all of these things. It is just that over time, the student with ADHD is likely to do several of them

chronically or repeatedly, and in more than one setting (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). In the classroom,

of course, the behaviors may annoy classmates and frustrate teachers.

Differences in perceptions: ADHD versus high activity

It is important to note that classrooms are places that make heavy demands on not showing ADHD-like

behaviors: students are often supposed to sit for long periods, avoid interrupting others, finish tasks after beginning

them, and keep their minds (and materials) organized. Ironically, therefore, classroom life may sometimes

aggravate ADHD without the teacher intending for it to do so. A student with only a mild or occasional tendency to

be restless, for example, may fit in well outdoors playing soccer, but feel unusually restless indoors during class. It

also should not be surprising that teachers sometimes mistake a student who is merely rather active for a student

with ADHD, since any tendency to be physically active may contribute to problems with classroom management.

94

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

The tendency to “over-diagnose” is more likely for boys than for girls (Maniadaki, et al., 2003), presumably because

gender role expectations cause teachers to be especially alert to high activity in boys. Over-diagnosis is also

especially likely for students who are culturally or linguistically non-Anglo (Chamberlain, 2005), presumably

because cultural and language differences may sometimes lead teachers to misinterpret students’ behavior. To

avoid making such mistakes, it is important to keep in mind that in true ADHD, restlessness, activity, and

distractibility are widespread and sustained. A student who shows such problems at school but never at home, for

example, may not have ADHD; he may simply not be getting along with his teacher or classmates.

Causes of ADHD

Most psychologists and medical specialists agree that true ADHD, as opposed to “mere” intermittent

distractibility or high activity, reflects a problem in how the nervous system functions, but they do not know the

exact nature or causes of the problem (Rutter, 2004, 2005). Research shows that ADHD tends to run in families,

with children—especially boys—of parents who had ADHD somewhat more likely than usual to experience the

condition themselves. The association does not necessarily mean, though, that ADHD is inborn or genetic. Why? It

is because it is possible that parents who formerly had ADHD may raise their children more strictly in an effort to

prevent their own condition in their children; yet their strictness, ironically, may trigger a bit more tendency, rather

than less, toward the restless distractibility characteristic of ADHD. On the other hand (or is it “on the third

hand”?), the parents’ strictness may also be a result, as well as a cause of, a child’s restlessness. The bottom line for

teachers: sorting out causes from effects is confusing, if not impossible, and in any case may not help much to

determine actual teaching strategies to help the students learn more effectively.

Teaching students with ADHD

Research also shows that ADHD can be reduced for many students if they take certain medications, of which the

most common is methylphenidate, commonly known by the name Ritalin (Wilens, 2005; Olfson, 2003). This drug

and others like it act by stimulating the nervous system, which reduces symptoms by helping a student pay better

attention to the choices he or she makes and to the impact of actions on others. Unfortunately the medications do

not work on all students with ADHD, especially after they reach adolescence, and in any case has certain practical

problems. Drugs cost money, for one thing, which is a problem for a family without much money to begin with, or

for a family lacking medical insurance that pays for medications—a particularly common situation in the United

States. For another thing, drugs must be taken regularly in order to be effective, including on weekends. Keeping a

regular schedule can be difficult if parents’ own schedules are irregular or simply differ from the child’s, for

example because of night shifts at work or because parents are separated and share custody of the child.

In any case, since teachers are not doctors and medications are not under teachers’ control, it may be more

important simply to provide an environment where a student with ADHD can organize choices and actions easily

and successfully. Clear rules and procedures, for example, can reduce the “noise” or chaotic quality in the child’s

classroom life significantly. The rules and procedures can be generated jointly with the child; they do not have to be

imposed arbitrarily, as if the student were incapable of thinking about them reasonably. Sometimes a classmate can

be enlisted to model slower, more reflective styles of working, but in ways that do not imply undue criticism of the

student with ADHD. The more reflective student can complete a set of math problems, for example, while

explaining what he or she is thinking about while doing the work. Sometimes the teacher can help by making lists of

tasks or of steps in long tasks. It can help to divide focused work into small, short sessions rather than grouping it

Educational Psychology 95 A Global Text

5. Students with special educational needs

into single, longer sessions. Whatever the strategies that you use, they should be consistent, predictable, and

generated by the student as much as possible. By having these qualities, the strategies can strengthen the student’s

self-direction and ability to screen out the distractions of classroom life. The goal for teachers, in essence, is to build

the student’s metacognitive capacity, while at the same time, of course, treating the student with respect.

Intellectual disabilities

An intellectual disability is a significant limitation in a student’s cognitive functioning and daily adaptive

behaviors (Schalock & Luckasson, 2004; American Association on Mental Retardation, 2002). The student may

have limited language or impaired speech and may not perform well academically. Compared to students with

learning disabilities discussed earlier, students with intellectual disabilities have impairments to learning that are

broader and more significant. They score poorly on standardized tests of intelligence (like the ones discussed later,

in Chapter 12). Everyday tasks that most people take for granted, like getting dressed or eating a meal, may be

possible, but they may also take more time and effort than usual. Health and safety can sometimes be a concern (for

example, knowing whether it is safe to cross a street). For older individuals, finding and keeping a job may require

help from supportive others. The exact combination of challenges varies from one person to another, but it always

(by definition) involves limitations in both intellectual and daily functioning.

As a teacher, you may hear more than one term for describing students with intellectual disabilities. If the

disability is mild, teachers sometimes refer to a student with the disability simply as a slow learner, particularly if

the student has no formal, special supports for the disability, such as a teaching assistant hired specifically to assist

the student. If the disability is more marked, then the student is more likely to be referred to either as having an

intellectual disability or as having mental retardation. In this chapter I primarily use the term intellectual

disability, because it has fewer negative connotations while still describing one key educational aspect of the

disability, cognitive impairment. Keep in mind, however, that actual intellectual disabilities are always more than

cognitive: they also involve challenges about adapting to everyday living.

Levels of support for individuals with intellectual disabilities

Intellectual disabilities happen in different degrees or amounts, though most often are relatively mild.

Traditionally the intensity or “amount” of the disability was defined by scores on a standardized test of scholastic

aptitude (or “IQ test”), with lower scores indicating more severe disability. (More about these tests in Chapter 12.)

Because of the insensitivity of such tests to individuals’ daily social functioning, however, current trends are toward

defining intensities by the amount of support needed by the individual. Table 13 summarizes the most commonly

used scheme for this purpose, one created by the American Association on Intellectual and Developmental

Disabilities (AAMR, 2002). Levels of support range from intermittent (just occasional or “as needed” for specific

activities) to pervasive (continuous in all realms of living).

Table 13: Levels and areas of support for intellectual disabilities

Level of

support

Duration of

support

Frequency of

support

Setting of

support

Amount of

professional

assistance

Intermittent Only as needed Occasional or Usually only one Occasional

96

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

infrequent or two (e.g. 1-2

classes or activities)

consultation or

monitoring by

professional

Limited As needed, but

sometimes continuing

Regular, but

frequency varies

Several settings,

but not usually all

Occasional or

regular contact with

professionals

Extensive Usually continuing Regular, but

frequency varies

Several settings,

but not usually all

Regular contact

with professionals at

least once a week

Pervasive May be lifelong Frequent or

continuous

Nearly all settings Continuous

contact and

monitoring by

professionals

Source: American Association on Mental Retardation, 2002: Schalock & Luckassen, 2004.

As a classroom teacher, the intellectual disabilities that you are most likely to see are the ones requiring the least

support in your classroom. A student requiring only intermittent support may require special help with some

learning activities or classroom routines, but not others; he or she might need help with reading or putting on

winter clothes, for example, but primarily on occasions when there is pressure to do these things relatively quickly.

Students requiring somewhat more support are likely to spend somewhat less time in your classroom and more

time receiving special help from other professionals, such as a special education teacher, a speech and language

specialist, or an assistant to these professionals. These circumstances have distinct implications for ways of

teaching these students.

Teaching students with intellectual disabilities

There are many specific techniques that can help in teaching students with mild or moderate intellectual

disabilities, but most can be summarized into three more general strategies. The first is to give more time and

practice than usual; the second is to embed activities into the context of daily life or functioning where possible; and

the third is to include the child both in social and in academic activities, rather than just one or the other. Let us

look briefly at each of these ideas.

Giving more time and practice than usual

If a student has only a mild intellectual disability, he or she can probably learn important fundamentals of the

academic curriculum—basic arithmetic, for example, and basic reading. Because of the disability, though, the

student may need more time or practice than most other students. He or she may be able to read many words by

sight (day, night, morning, afternoon, etc.), but need longer than other students to recognize and say them. Or the

student may know that 2 + 3 = 5, but need help applying this math fact to real objects; you (or a helper) might need

to show the student that two pencils plus three pencils make five pencils.

Educational Psychology 97 A Global Text

5. Students with special educational needs

Giving extra help takes time and perseverance, and can try the patience of the student (and of you, too). To deal

with this problem, it may help to reward the student frequently for effort and successes with well-timed praise,

especially if it is focused on specific, actual achievements; “You added that one correctly”, may be more helpful than

“You’re a hard worker”, even if both comments are true. Giving appropriate praise is in turn easier if you set

reasonable, “do-able” goals by breaking skills or tasks into steps that the student is likely to learn without becoming

overly discouraged. At the same time, it is important not to insult the student with goals or activities that are too

easy or by using curriculum materials clearly intended for children who are much younger. Setting expectations too

low actually deprives a student with an intellectual disability of rightful opportunities to learn—a serious ethical and

professional mistake (Bogdan, 2006). In many curriculum areas, fortunately, there already existing materials that

are simplified, yet also appropriate for older students (Snell, et al., 2005). Special education teacher-specialists can

often help in finding them and in devising effective ways of using them.

Adaptive and functional skills

Students with intellectual disabilities present especially clear examples of a universal dilemma of teaching: since

there is not enough time to teach everything, how do we choose what to teach? One basis for selecting activities is to

relate learning goals to students’ everyday lives and activities, just as you would with all students. This strategy

addresses the other defining feature of mental retardation, the student’s difficulties with adapting to and

functioning in everyday living. In teaching addition and subtraction, for example, you can create examples about

the purchasing of common familiar objects (e.g. food) and about the need to make or receive change for the

purchases. Similar considerations apply to learning new reading or oral language vocabulary. Instead of simply

learning words in a “basic reading” series (or reading textbook), try encouraging the student to learn words that are

especially useful to the student’s own life. Often the student, not you yourself, is the best person to decide what

these words actually are.

An adaptive, functional approach can help in nonacademic areas as well. In learning to read or “tell time” on a

clock, for example, try focusing initially on telling the times important to the student, such as when he or she gets

up in the morning or when schools starts. As you add additional times that are personally meaningful to the

student, he or she works gradually towards full knowledge of how to read the hands on a clock. Even if the full

knowledge proves slow to develop, however, the student will at least have learned the most useful clock knowledge

first.

Include the student deliberately in group activities

The key word here is inclusion: the student should participate in and contribute to the life of the class as much

as possible. This means that wherever possible, the student attends special events (assemblies, field days) with the

class; that if the class plays a group game, then the student with the disability is part of the game; that if classmates

do an assignment as a group, then if at all possible the student is assigned to one of the groups. The changes

resulting from these inclusions are real, but can be positive for everyone. On the one hand, they foster acceptance

and helpfulness toward the child with the disability; classmates learn that school is partly about providing

opportunities for everyone, and not just about evaluating or comparing individuals’ skills. On the other hand, the

changes caused by inclusion stimulate the student with the disability to learn as much as possible from classmates,

socially and academically. Among other benefits, group activities can give the student chances to practice

“belonging” skills—how to greet classmates appropriately, or when and how to ask the teacher a question. These are

98

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

skills, I might add, that are beneficial for everyone to learn, disabled or not. (I discuss group work more thoroughly

in Chapter 9, “Facilitating complex thinking”)

Behavioral disorders

Behavioral disorders are a diverse group of conditions in which a student chronically performs highly

inappropriate behaviors. A student with this condition might seek attention, for example, by acting out disruptively

in class. Other students with the condition might behave aggressively, be distractible and overly active, seem

anxious or withdrawn, or seem disconnected from everyday reality. As with learning disabilities, the sheer range of

signs and symptoms defies concise description. But the problematic behaviors do have several general features in

common (Kauffman, 2005; Hallahan & Kauffman, 2006):

• they tend to be extreme

• they persist for extended periods of time

• they tend to be socially unacceptable (e.g. unwanted sexual advances or vandalism against school property)

• they affect school work

• they have no other obvious explanation (e.g. a health problem or temporary disruption in the family)

The variety among behavioral disorders means that estimates of their frequency also tend to vary among states,

cities, and provinces. It also means that in some cases, a student with a behavioral disorder may be classified as

having a different condition, such as ADHD or a learning disability. In other cases, a behavioral problem shown in

one school setting may seem serious enough to be labeled as a behavioral disorder, even though a similar problem

occurring in another school may be perceived as serious, but not serious enough to deserve the label. In any case,

available statistics suggest that only about one to two per cent of students, or perhaps less, have true behavioral

disorders—a figure that is only about one half or one third of the frequency for intellectual disabilities (Kauffman,

2005). Because of the potentially disruptive effects of behavioral disorders, however, students with this condition

are of special concern to teachers. Just one student who is highly aggressive or disruptive can interfere with the

functioning of an entire class, and challenge even the best teacher’s management skills and patience.

Strategies for teaching students with behavioral disorders

The most common challenges of teaching students with behavioral disorders have to do with classroom

management—a topic discussed more thoroughly in Chapter 7 (“Classroom management”). Three important ideas

discussed there, however, also deserve special emphasis here: (1) identifying circumstances that trigger

inappropriate behaviors, (2) teaching of interpersonal skills explicitly, and (3) disciplining a student fairly.

Identifying circumstances that trigger inappropriate behaviors

Dealing with a disruption is more effective if you can identify the specific circumstances or event that triggers it,

rather than focusing on the personality of the student doing the disrupting. A wide variety of factors can trigger

inappropriate behavior (Heineman, Dunlap, & Kincaid, 2005):

• physiological effects—including illness, fatigue, hunger, or side-effects from medications

• physical features of the classroom—such as the classroom being too warm or too cold, the chairs being

exceptionally uncomfortable for sitting, or seating patterns that interfere with hearing or seeing

Educational Psychology 99 A Global Text

5. Students with special educational needs

• instructional choices or strategies that frustrate learning—including restricting students’ choices unduly,

giving instructions that are unclear, choosing activities that are too difficult or too long, or preventing

students from asking questions when they need help

By identifying the specific variables often associated with disruptive behaviors, it is easier to devise ways to

prevent the behaviors, either by avoiding the triggers if this is possible, or by teaching the student alternative but

quite specific ways of responding to the triggering circumstance.

Teaching interpersonal skills explicitly

Because of their history and behavior, some students with behavior disorders have had little opportunity to learn

appropriate social skills. Simple courtesies (like remembering to say please or thanks) may not be totally unknown,

but may be unpracticed and seem unimportant to the student, as might body language (like eye contact or sitting up

to listen to a teacher rather than slouching and looking away). These skills can be taught in ways that do not make

them part of punishment, make them seem “preachy”, or put a student to shame in front of classmates. Depending

on the age or grade-level of the class, one way is by reading or assigning books and stories in which the characters

model good social skills. Another is through games that require courteous language to succeed; one that I recall

from my own school days, for example, was called “Mother, May I?” (Sullivan & Strang, 2002). Still another is

through programs that link an older student or adult from the community as a partner to the student at risk for

behavior problems; a prominent example of such a program in the United States is Big Brothers Big Sisters of

America, which arranges for older individuals to act as mentors for younger boys and girls (Tierney, Grossman, &

Resch, 1995; Newburn & Shiner, 2006).

In addition, strategies based on behaviorist theory have proved effective for many students, especially if the

student needs opportunities simply to practice social skills that he has learned only recently and may still feel

awkward or self-conscious in using (Algozzine & Ysseldyke, 2006). Several behaviorist techniques were discussed in

Chapter 2, including the use of positive reinforcement, extinction, generalization, and the like. In addition to these,

teachers can arrange for contingency contracts, which are agreements between the teacher and a student about

exactly what work the student will do, how it will be rewarded, and what the consequences will be if the agreement

is not fulfilled (Wilkinson, 2003). An advantage of all such behaviorist techniques is their precision and clarity:

there is little room for misunderstanding about just what your expectations are as the teacher. The precision and

clarity in turn makes it less tempting or necessary for you, as teacher, to become angry about infractions of rules or

a student’s failure to fulfill contracts or agreements, since the consequences tend already to be relatively obvious

and clear. “Keeping your cool” can be especially helpful when dealing with behavior that is by nature annoying or

disrupting.

Fairness in disciplining

Many strategies for helping a student with a behavior disorder may be spelled out in the student’s individual

educational plan, such as discussed earlier in this chapter. The plan can (and indeed is supposed to) serve as a

guide in devising daily activities and approaches with the student. Keep in mind, however, that since an IEP is akin

to a legal agreement among a teacher, other professionals, a student and the student’s parents, departures from it

should be made only cautiously and carefully, if ever. Although such departures may seem unlikely, a student with a

behavior disorder may sometimes be exasperating enough to make it tempting to use stronger or more sweeping

100

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

punishments than usual (for example, isolating a student for extended times). In case you are tempted in this

direction, remember that every IEP also guarantees the student and the student’s parents due process before an

IEP can be changed. In practice this means consulting with everyone involved in the case—especially parents, other

specialists, and the student himself—and reaching an agreement before adopting new strategies that differ

significantly from the past.

Instead of “increasing the volume” of punishments, a better approach is to keep careful records of the student’s

behavior and of your own responses to it, documenting the reasonableness of your rules or responses to any major

disruptions. By having the records, collaboration with parents and other professionals can be more productive and

fair-minded, and increase others’ confidence in your judgments about what the student needs in order to fit in more

comfortably with the class. In the long term, more effective collaboration leads both to better support and to more

learning for the student (as well as to better support for you as teacher!).

Physical disabilities and sensory impairments

A few students have serious physical, medical, or sensory challenges that interfere with their learning. Usually,

the physical and medical challenges are medical conditions or diseases that require ongoing medical care. The

sensory challenges are usually a loss either in hearing or in vision, or more rarely in both. Whatever the specific

problem, it is serious enough to interfere with activities in regular classroom programs and to qualify the student

for special educational services or programs.

Physical challenges that are this serious are relatively infrequent compared to some of the other special needs

discussed in this chapter, though they are of course important in the lives of the students and their families, as well

as important for teachers to accommodate. Only about one per cent of US students have a hearing loss serious

enough to be served by special programs for such students (United States Department of Education, 2005). Only

about half that number have visual impairments that lead them to be served by special programs. For two reasons,

though, these figures are a bit misleading. One reason is that many more students have vision or hearing problems

that are too mild (such as wearing eyeglasses for “ordinary” nearsightedness). Another is that some students with

serious sensory impairments may also have other disabilities and therefore not be counted in statistics about

sensory impairments.

Hearing loss

A child can acquire a hearing loss for a variety of reasons, ranging from disease early in childhood, to difficulties

during childbirth, to reactions to toxic drugs. In the classroom, however, the cause of the loss is virtually irrelevant

because it makes little difference in how to accommodate a student’s educational needs. More important than the

cause of the loss is its extent. Students with only mild or moderate loss of hearing are sometimes called hearing

impaired or hard of hearing; only those with nearly complete loss are called deaf. As with other sorts of

disabilities, the milder the hearing loss, the more likely you are to encounter the student in a regular classroom, at

least for part of the day.

Signs of hearing loss

Although determining whether a student has a hearing loss may seem straightforward (“Just give a hearing

test!”), the assessment is often not clear cut if it takes the student’s daily experiences into account. A serious or

profound hearing loss tends to be noticed relatively quickly and therefore often receive special help (or at least

Educational Psychology 101 A Global Text

5. Students with special educational needs

receives additional diagnosis) sooner. Mild or moderate hearing loss is much more common, however, and is more

likely to be overlooked or mistaken for some other sort of learning problem (Sherer, 2004). Students with a mild

hearing loss sometimes have somewhat depressed (or lowered) language and literacy skills—though not always, and

in any case so do some students without any loss. They may also seem not to listen or attend to a speaker because of

trouble in locating the source of sounds—but then again, sometimes students without loss also fail to listen, though

for entirely different reasons. Students with hearing loss may frequently give incorrect answers to questions—but so

do certain other students with normal hearing. In addition, partial hearing loss can be hidden if the student teaches

himself or herself to lip read, for example, or is careful in choosing which questions to answer in a class discussion.

And so on. Systematic hearing tests given by medical or hearing specialists can resolve some of these ambiguities.

But even they can give a misleading impression, since students’ true ability to manage in class depends on how well

they combine cues and information from the entire context of classroom life.

In identifying a student who may have a hearing loss, therefore, teachers need to observe the student over an

extended period of time and in as many situations as possible. In particular, look for a persistent combination of

some of the following, but look for them over repeated or numerous occasions (Luckner & Carter, 2001):

• delayed language or literacy skills, both written and oral

• some ability (usually partial) to read lips

• less worldly knowledge than usual because of lack of involvement with oral dialogue and/or delayed literacy

• occasionally, tendency to social isolation because of awkwardness in communication

Teaching students with hearing loss

In principle, adjustments in teaching students with hearing loss are relatively easy to make though they do

require deliberate actions or choices by the teacher and by fellow students. Interestingly, many of the strategies

make good advice for teaching all students!

• Take advantage of the student’s residual hearing. Seat the student close to you if you are doing the talking,

or close to key classmates if the students are in a work group. Keep competing noise, such as unnecessary

talking or whispering, to a minimum (because such noise is especially distracting to someone with a

hearing loss). Keep instructions concise and to-the-point. Ask the student occasionally whether he or she is

understanding.

• Use visual cues liberally. Make charts and diagrams wherever appropriate to illustrate what you are saying.

Look directly at the student when you are speaking to him or her (to facilitate lip reading). Gesture and

point to key words or objects—but within reason, not excessively. Provide handouts or readings to review

visually the points that you make orally.

• Include the student in the community of the classroom. Recruit one or more classmates to assist in

“translating” oral comments that the student may have missed. If the student uses American Sign Language

(ASL) at home or elsewhere, then learn a few basic, important signs of ASL yourself (“Hello” “thank you”

“How are you?”). Teach them to classmates as well.

102

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Visual impairment

Students with visual impairments have difficulty seeing even with corrective lenses. Most commonly the

difficulty has to do with refraction (the ability to focus), but some students may also experience a limited field of

view (called tunnel vision) or be overly sensitive to light in general. As with hearing loss, labels for visual

impairment depend somewhat on the extent and nature of the problem. Legal blindness means that the person has

significant tunnel vision or else visual acuity (sharpness of vision) of 20/200 or less, which means that he or she

must be 20 feet away from an object that a person with normal eyesight can see at 200 feet. Low vision means that

a person has some vision usable for reading, but often needs a special optical device such as a magnifying lens for

doing so. As with hearing loss, the milder the impairment, the more likely that a student with a vision problem will

spend some or even all the time in a regular class.

Signs of visual impairment

Students with visual impairments often show some of the same signs as students with simple, common

nearsightedness. The students may rub their eyes a lot, for example, blink more than usual, or hold books very close

to read them. They may complain of itchiness in their eyes, or of headaches, dizziness, or even nausea after doing a

lot of close eye work. The difference between the students with visual impairment and those with “ordinary”

nearsightedness is primarily a matter of degree: the ones with impairment show the signs more often and more

obviously. If the impairment is serious enough or has roots in certain physical conditions or disease, they may also

have additional symptoms, such as crossed eyes or swollen eyelids. As with hearing loss, the milder forms ironically

can be the most subtle to observe and therefore the most prone to being overlooked at first. For classroom teachers,

the best strategy may be to keep track of a student whose physical signs happen in combination with learning

difficulties, and for whom the combination persists for many weeks.

Teaching students with visual impairment

In general, advice for teaching students with mild or moderate visual impairment parallels the advice for

teaching students with hearing loss, though with obvious differences because of the nature of the students’

disabilities.

• Take advantage of the student’s residual vision. If the student still has some useful vision, place him or her

where he can easily see the most important parts of the classroom—whether that is you, the chalkboard, a

video screen, or particular fellow students. Make sure that the classroom, or at least the student’s part of it,

is well lit (because good lighting makes reading easier with low vision). Make sure that handouts, books and

other reading materials have good, sharp contrast (also helpful with a visual impairment).

• Use non-visual information liberally. Remember not to expect a student with visual impairment to learn

information that is by nature only visual, such as the layout of the classroom, the appearance of

photographs in a textbook or of story lines in a video. Explain these to the student somehow. Use hands-on

materials wherever they will work, such as maps printed in three-dimensional relief or with different

textures. If the student knows how to read Braille (an alphabet for the blind using patterns of small bumps

on a page), allow him to do so.

• Include the student in the community of the classroom. Make sure that the student is accepted as well as

possible into the social life of the class. Recruit classmates to help explain visual material when necessary.

Educational Psychology 103 A Global Text

5. Students with special educational needs

Learn a bit of basic Braille and encourage classmates to do the same, even if none of you ever become as

skilled with it as the student himself or herself.

The value of including students with special needs

I have hinted at it already in this chapter, but it is worth saying again: including students with disabilities in

regular classrooms is valuable for everyone concerned. The students with disabilities themselves tend to experience

a richer educational environment, both socially and academically. Just as with racial segregation, separate

education is not equal education, or at least cannot be counted on to be equal. But classmates of students with

disabilities also experience a richer educational environment; they potentially meet a wider range of classmates and

to see a wider range of educational purposes in operation. Teachers also experience these benefits, but their

programs often benefit in other ways as well. The most notable additional benefit is that many teaching strategies

that are good for students with disabilities also turn out to benefit all students—benefits like careful planning of

objectives, attention to individual differences among students, and establishment of a positive social atmosphere in

the classroom. Later (in Chapters 9 and 10) we will return to these topics because of their importance for high-

quality teaching. But at that point we will frame the topics around the needs of all students, whatever their

individual qualities.

Chapter summary

Since the 1970s support for people with disabilities has grown significantly, as reflected in the United States by

three key pieces of legislation: the Rehabilitation Act of 1973, Americans with Disabilities Act of 1990, and the

Individuals with Disabilities Education Act (IDEA). The support has led to new educational practices, including

alternative assessments for students with disabilities, placement in the least restrictive environment, and individual

educational plans.

There are many ways of classifying people with disabilities, all of which carry risks of stereotyping and

oversimplifying individuals’ strengths and needs. For the purposes of education, the most frequent category is

learning disabilities, which are difficulties with specific aspects of academic work. The high prevalence of learning

disabilities makes this category especially ambiguous as a description of particular students. Assistance for students

with learning disabilities can be framed in terms of behaviorist reinforcement, metacognitive strategies, or

constructivist mentoring.

Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a problem in sustaining attention and controlling impulses.

It can often be controlled with medications, but usually it is also important for teachers to provide a structured

environment for the student as well.

Intellectual disabilities (or mental retardation) are general limitations in cognitive functioning as well as in the

tasks of daily living. Contemporary experts tend to classify individuals with these disabilities according to the

amount and frequency of support they need from others. Teachers can assist these students by giving more time

and practice than usual, by including adaptive and functional skills in what they teach, and by making sure that the

student is included in the daily life of the classroom.

Behavioral disorders are conditions in which students chronically perform highly inappropriate behaviors.

Students with these problems present challenges for classroom management, which teachers can meet by

104

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

identifying circumstances that trigger inappropriate behaviors, by teaching interpersonal skills explicitly, and by

making sure that punishments or disciplinary actions are fair and have been previously agreed upon.

Physical and sensory disabilities are significant limitations in health, hearing, or vision. The signs both of

hearing loss and of vision loss can be subtle, but can sometimes be observed over a period of time. Teaching

students with either a hearing loss or a vision loss primarily involves making use of the students’ residual sensory

abilities and insuring that the student is included in and supported by the class as well as possible.

Key termsAlternative assessment

Americans with Disabilities Act of 1990Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)

Behavioral disordersContingency contracts

Hearing lossIndividuals with Disabilities Education Act

Individual educational plan (IEP)Intellectual disabilities

Least restrictive environment (LRE)

Learning disabilitiesMental retardation

Portfolio assessmentRehabilitation Act of 1973

Sensory impairmentTransition planning

Visual impairment

On the Internet

Each of the following websites represents an organization focused on the needs of people with one particular

type of disability. Each includes free access to archives of non-current journals and other publications, as well as

information about conferences, professional training events, and political news relevant to persons with disabilities.

(Note that the sponsoring organizations about hearing loss and about intellectual disabilities changed their names

recently, though not their purposes, so their websites may eventually change names as well.)

This is primarily about learning disabilities, but also somewhat about ADHD.

This website is primarily about ADHD. Note that its website name uses an older terminology

for this disability, ADD (no “H”) for attention deficit disorder (with the term hyperactivity).

This one primarily discusses about hearing loss.

This website is primarily about visual impairment.

This one is primarily about intellectual disabilities or mental retardation.

References

Algozzine, R. & Ysseldyke, J. (2006). Teaching students with emotional disturbance: A practical guide for

every teacher. Thousand Oaks, CA: Corwin Press.

American Association on Mental Retardation. (2002). Definition, classification, and system of supports, 10th

edition. Washington, D.C.: Author.

American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, DSM-IV-

TR (text revision). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association.

Biklen, S. & Kliewer, C. (2006). Constructing competence: Autism, voice and the “disordered” body.

International Journal of Inclusive Education, 10(2/3), 169-188.

Educational Psychology 105 A Global Text

5. Students with special educational needs

Bogdan, D., Attfield, R., Bissonnette, L., Blackman, L., Burke, J., Mukopadhyay, T., & Rubin, S. (Eds.).

(2005). Autism: The myth of the person alone. New York: New York University Press.

Bogdan, D. (2006). Who may be literate? Disability and resistance to the cultural denial of competence.

American Educational Research Journal, 43(2), 163-192.

Bradley, M. & Mandell, D. (2005). Oppositional defiant disorder: A systematic review of the evidence of

intervention effectiveness. Journal of Experimental Criminology, 34(1), 343-365.

Carothers, D. & Taylor, R. (2003). Use of portfolios for students with autism. Focus on Autism and Other

Developmental Disorders, 18(2), 121-124.

Chamberlain, S. (2005). Recognizing and responding to cultural differences in the education of culturally and

linguistically diverse learners. Intervention in School and Clinic, 40(4), 195-211.

Green, S., Davis, C., Karshmer, E., March, P. & Straight, B. (2005). Living stigma: The impact of labeling,

stereotyping, separation, status loss, and discrimination in the lives of individuals with disabilities and

their families. Sociological Inquiry, 75(2), 197-215.

Hallahan, D. & Kauffman, J. (2006). Exceptional learners: Introduction to special education, 10th edition.

Boston: Allyn & Bacon.

Heineman, M., Dunlap, G., & Kincaid, D. (2005). Positive support strategies for students with behavioral

disorders in regular classrooms. Psychology in the Schools, 42(8), 779-794.

Kauffman, J. (2005). Characteristics of children with emotional and behavioral disorders, 8th edition.

Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson/Merrill Prentice Hall.

Keller, H. (1952). The story of my life. New York: Doubleday.

Kelly, S. (2004). Are teachers tracked? On what basis and with what consequences. Social psychology in

education, 7(1), 55-72.

Koretz, D. & Barton, K. (2003/2004). Assessing students with disabilities: Issues and evidence. Assessment

and Evaluation, 9(1 & 2), 29-60.

Luckner, J. L. & Carter, K. (2001). Essential Competencies for Teaching Students with Hearing Loss and

Additional Disabilities. 146(1), 7-15.

Newburn, T. & Shiner, M. (2006). Young people, mentoring and social inclusion. Youth Justice, 6(1), 23-41.

Oakes, J. (2005). Keeping track: How schools structure inequality, 2nd edition. New Haven, CT: Yale

University Press.

Olfson, M., Gameroff, M., Marcus, S., & Jensen, P. (2003). National trends in the treatment of ADHD.

American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 1071-1077.

Public Law 93-112, 87 Stat. 394 (Sept. 26, 1973). Rehabilitation Act of 1973. Washington, D.C.: United States

Government Printing Office.

106

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Public Law 101-336, 104 Stat. 327 (July 26, 1990). Americans with Disabilities Act of 1990. Washington,

D.C.: United States Government Printing Office.

Public Law 108-446, 118 Stat. 2647 (December 3, 2004). Individuals with Disabilities Education

Improvement Act. Washington, D.C.: United States Government Printing Office.

Pullin, D. (2005). When one size does not fit all: The special challenges of accountability testing for students

with disabilities. Yearbook of the National Society for Studies in Education, 104(2), 199.

Quinn, M. (2002). Changing antisocial behavior patterns in young boys: a structured cooperative learning

approach. Education and treatment of young children, 25(4), 380-395.

Robinson, W. (1982). Critical essays on Phillis Wheatley. Boston: Hall Publishers.

Rutter, M. (2004). Pathways of genetic influences in psychopathology. European Review, 12, 19-33.

Rutter, M. (2005). Multiple meanings of a developmental perspective on psychopathology. European

Journal of Developmental Psychology, 2(3), 221-252.

Schalock, R. & Luckasson, R. (2004). American Association on Mental Retardation’s Definition,

Classification, & System of Supports, 10th edition. Journal of Policy and Practice in Intellectual

Disabilities, 1(3/4), 136-146.

Sherer, M. (2004). Connecting to learn: Educational and assistive technology for people with disabilities.

Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association.

Snell, M., Janney, R., Elliott, J., Beck, M., Colley, K., & Burton, C. (2005). Collaborative teaming: Teachers’

guide to inclusive practices. Baltimore, MD: Brookes Publishing Co.

Stowitschek, J., Lovitt, T., & Rodriguez, J. (2001). Patterns of collaboration in secondary education for youth

with special needs: Profiles of three high schools. Urban Education, 36(1), 93-128.

Sullivan, A. K. & Strang, H. R. (2002/2003). Bibliotherapy in the Classroom: Using Literature to Promote the

Development of Emotional Intelligence. Childhood Education 79(2), 74-80.

Tierney, J., Grossman, J., & Resch, N. (1995). Making a difference: An impact study of big brothers big

sisters. Philadelphia: Public/Private Ventures.

United States Department of Education. (2005). 27th Annual Report to Congress on the implementation of

the Individuals with Disabilities Education Act. Washington, D.C.: Author.

Wesson, C. & King, R. (1996). Portfolio assessment and special education students. Teaching Exceptional

Children, 28(2), 44-48.

West, L., Corbey, S., Boyer-Stephens, A., Jones, B. Miller, R., & Sarkees-Wircenski, M. (1999). Integrating

transition planning into the IEP process, 2nd edition. Alexandria, VA: Council for Exceptional Children.

Wilens, T., McBurnett, K., Stein, M., Lerner, M., Spencer, T., & Wolraich, M. (2005). ADHD treatment with

once-daily methylphenidate. Journal of American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 44(10),

1015-1023.

Educational Psychology 107 A Global Text

5. Students with special educational needs

Wilkinson, L. (2003). Using behavioral consultation to reduce challenging behavior in the classroom.

Psychology in the schools, 47(3), 100-105.

Ysseldyke, J. & Bielinski, J. (2002). Effect of different methods of reporting and reclassification on trends in

test scores for students with disabilities. Exceptional Children, 68(2), 189-201.

108

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

6. Student motivationNot so long ago, a teacher named Barbara Fuller taught general science to elementary years

students, and one of her units was about insects and spiders. As part of the unit she had students

search for insects and spiders around their own homes or apartments. They brought the creatures to

school (safely in jars), answered a number of questions about them in their journals, and eventually

gave brief oral reports about their findings to the class. The assignment seemed straightforward, but

Barbara found that students responded to it in very different ways. Looking back, here is how

Barbara described their responses:

“I remember Jose couldn’t wait to get started, and couldn’t bear to end the assignment either! Every

day he brought more bugs or spiders—eventually 25 different kinds. Every day he drew pictures of

them in his journal and wrote copious notes about them. At the end he gave the best oral

presentation I’ve ever seen from a third-grader; he called it ‘They Have Us Outnumbered!’ I wish I

had filmed it, he was so poised and so enthusiastic.

“Then there was Lindsey—the one who was always wanted to be the best in everything, regardless of

whether it interested her. She started off the work rather slowly—just brought in a few bugs and only

one spider. But she kept an eye on what everyone else was bringing, and how much. When she saw

how much Jose was doing, though, she picked up her pace, like she was trying to match his level.

Except that instead of bringing a diversity of creatures as Jose was doing, she just brought more and

more of the same ones—almost twenty dead house flies, as I recall! Her presentation was OK—I

really could not give her a bad mark for it—but it wasn’t as creative or insightful as Jose’s. I think she

was more concerned about her mark than about the material.

“And there was Tobias—discouraging old Tobias. He did the work, but just barely. I noticed him

looking a lot at other students’ insect collections and at their journal entries. He wasn’t cheating, I

believe, just figuring out what the basic level of work was for the assignment—what he needed to do

simply to avoid failing it. He brought in fewer bugs than most others, though still a number that was

acceptable. He also wrote shorter answers in his journal and gave one of the shortest oral reports. It

was all acceptable, but not much more than that.

“And Zoey: she was quite a case! I never knew whether to laugh or cry about her. She didn’t exactly

resist doing the assignment, but she certainly liked to chat with other students. So she was easily

distracted, and that cut down on getting her work done, especially about her journal entries. What

really saved her—what kept her work at a reasonably high level of quality—were the two girls she

ended up chatting with. The other two were already pretty motivated to do a lot with the assignment

—create fine looking bug collections, write good journal entries, and make interesting oral

presentations. So when Zoey attempted chitchat with them, the conversations often ended up

Educational Psychology 109 A Global Text

6. Student motivation

focusing on the assignment anyway! She had them to thank for keeping her mind on the work. I don’t

know what Zoey would have done without them.”

As Barbara Fuller’s recollections suggest, students assign various meanings and attitudes to academic activities

—personal meanings and attitudes that arouse and direct their energies in different ways. We call these and their

associated energizing and directing effects by the term motivation, or sometimes motivation to learn. As you

will see, differences in motivation are an important source of diversity in classrooms, comparable in importance to

differences in prior knowledge, ability, or developmental readiness. When it comes to school learning, furthermore,

students’ motivations take on special importance because students’ mere presence in class is (of course) no

guarantee that students really want to learn. It is only a sign that students live in a society requiring young people to

attend school. Since modern education is compulsory, teachers cannot take students’ motivation for granted, and

they have a responsibility to insure students’ motivation to learn. Somehow or other, teachers must persuade

students to want to do what students have to do anyway. This task—understanding and therefore influencing

students’ motivations to learn—is the focus of this chapter. Fortunately, as you will see, there are ways of

accomplishing this task that respect students’ choices, desires, and attitudes.

Like motivation itself, theories of it are full of diversity. For convenience in navigating through the diversity, we

have organized the chapter around six major theories or perspectives about motives and their sources. We call the

topics (1) motives as behavior change, (2) motives as goals, (3) motives as interests, (4) motives as attributions

about success, (5) motives as beliefs about self-efficacy, and (6) motives as self-determination. We end with a

perspective called expectancy-value theory which integrates ideas from some of the other six theories, and partly as

a result implies some additional suggestions for influencing students’ motivations to learn in positive ways.

Motives as behavior

Sometimes it is useful to think of motivation not as something “inside” a student driving the student’s behavior,

but as equivalent to the student’s outward behaviors. This is the perspective of behaviorism, which we discussed in

Chapter 1 (“Student learning”) as a way to think about the learning process. In its most thorough-going form,

behaviorism focuses almost completely on what can be directly seen or heard about a person’s behavior, and has

relatively few comments about what may lie behind (or “underneath” or “inside”) the behavior. When it comes to

motivation, this perspective means minimizing or even ignoring the distinction between the inner drive or energy of

students, and the outward behaviors that express the drive or energy. The two are considered the same, or nearly

so.

Equating the inner and the outward might seem to violate common sense. How can a student do something

without some sort of feeling or thought to make the action happen? As we will explain, this very question has led to

alternative models of motivation that are based on cognitive rather than behaviorist theories of learning. We will

explain some of these later in this chapter. Before getting to them, however, we encourage you to consider the

advantages of a behaviorist perspective on motivation.

Sometimes the circumstances of teaching limit teachers’ opportunities to distinguish between inner motivation

and outward behavior. Certainly teachers see plenty of student behaviors—signs of motivation of some sort. But the

multiple demands of teaching can limit the time needed to determine what the behaviors mean. If a student asks a

lot of questions during discussions, for example, is he or she curious about the material itself, or just wanting to

110

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

look intelligent in front of classmates and the teacher? In a class with many students and a busy agenda, there may

not be a lot of time for a teacher to decide between these possibilities. In other cases, the problem may not be

limited time as much as communication difficulties with a student. Consider a student who is still learning English,

or who belongs to a cultural community that uses patterns of conversation that are unfamiliar to the teacher, or

who has a disability that limits the student’s general language skill. In these cases discerning the student’s inner

motivations may take more time and effort. It is important to invest the extra time and effort for such students, but

while a teacher is doing so, it is also important for her to guide and influence the students’ behavior in constructive

directions. That is where behaviorist approaches to motivation can help.

Operant conditioning as a way of motivating

The most common version of the behavioral perspective on motivation is the theory of operant conditioning

associated with B. F. Skinner (1938, 1957), which we discussed in Chapter 1 (“Learning process”). The description in

that chapter focused on behavioral learning, but the same operant model can be transformed into an account of

motivation. In the operant model, you may recall, a behavior being learned (the “operant”) increases in frequency

or likelihood because performing it makes a reinforcement available. To understand this model in terms of

motivation, think of the likelihood of response as the motivation and the reinforcement as the motivator. Imagine,

for example, that a student learns by operant conditioning to answer questions during class discussions: each time

the student answers a question (the operant), the teacher praises (reinforces) this behavior. In addition to thinking

of this situation as behavioral learning, however, you can also think of it in terms of motivation: the likelihood of

the student answering questions (the motivation) is increasing because of the teacher’s praise (the motivator).

Many concepts from operant conditioning, in fact, can be understood in motivational terms. Another one, for

example, is the concept of extinction, which we defined in Chapter 1 as the tendency for learned behaviors to

become less likely when reinforcement no longer occurs—a sort of “unlearning”, or at least a decrease in

performance of previously learned. The decrease in performance frequency can be thought of as a loss of

motivation, and removal of the reinforcement can be thought of as removal of the motivator. Table 14 summarizes

this way of reframing operant conditioning in terms of motivation, both for the concepts discussed in Chapter 1 and

for other additional concepts.

Table 14: Operant conditioning as learning and as motivation

Concept Definition phrased

in terms of learning

Definition phrased

in terms of motivation

Classroom example

Operant Behavior that becomes

more likely because of

reinforcement

Behavior that suggests

an increase in motivation

Student listens to

teacher’s comments during

lecture or discussion

Reinforcement Stimulus that increases

likelihood of a behavior

Stimulus that motivates Teacher praises student

for listening

Positive reinforcement Stimulus that increases

likelihood of a behavior by

being introduced or added

Stimulus that motivates

by its presence; an

“incentive”

Teacher makes

encouraging remarks

about student’s homework

Educational Psychology 111 A Global Text

6. Student motivation

to a situation

Negative reinforcement Stimulus that increases

the likelihood of a behavior

by being removed or taken

away from a situation

Stimulus that motivates

by its absence or

avoidance

Teacher stops nagging

student about late

homework

Punishment Stimulus that

decreases the likelihood of

a behavior by being

introduced or added to a

situation

Stimulus that

decreases motivation by its

presence

Teacher deducts points

for late homework

Extinction Removal of

reinforcement for a

behavior

Removal of motivating

stimulus that leads to

decrease in motivation

Teacher stops

commenting altogether

about student’s homework

Shaping successive

approximations

Reinforcements for

behaviors that gradually

resemble (approximate) a

final goal behavior

Stimuli that gradually

shift motivation toward a

final goal motivation

Teacher praises student

for returning homework a

bit closer to the deadline;

gradually she praises for

actually being on time

Continuous

reinforcement

Reinforcement that

occurs each time that an

operant behavior occurs

Motivator that occurs

each time that a behavioral

sign of motivation occurs

Teacher praises highly

active student for every

time he works for five

minutes without

interruption

Intermittent

reinforcement

Reinforcement that

sometimes occurs

following an operant

behavior, but not on every

occasion

Motivator that occurs

sometimes when a

behavioral sign of

motivation occurs, but not

on every occasion

Teacher praises highly

active student sometimes

when he works without

interruption, but not every

time

Cautions about behavioral perspectives on motivation

As we mentioned, behaviorist perspectives about motivation do reflect a classroom reality: that teachers

sometimes lack time and therefore must focus simply on students’ appropriate outward behavior. But there are

nonetheless cautions about adopting this view. An obvious one is the ambiguity of students’ specific behaviors;

what looks like a sign of one motive to the teacher may in fact be a sign of some other motive to the student

(DeGrandpre, 2000). If a student looks at the teacher intently while she is speaking, does it mean the student is

motivated to learn, or only that the student is daydreaming? If a student invariably looks away while the teacher is

112

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

speaking, does it mean that the student is disrespectful of the teacher, or that student comes from a family or

cultural group where avoiding eye contact actually shows more respect for a speaker than direct eye contact?

Another concern about behaviorist perspectives, including operant conditioning, is that it leads teachers to

ignore students’ choices and preferences, and to “play God” by making choices on their behalf (Kohn, 1996).

According to this criticism, the distinction between “inner” motives and expressions of motives in outward behavior

does not disappear just because a teacher (or a psychological theory) chooses to treat a motive and the behavioral

expression of a motive as equivalent. Students usually do know what they want or desire, and their wants or desires

may not always correspond to what a teacher chooses to reinforce or ignore. This, in a new guise, is once again the

issue of intrinsic versus extrinsic motivation that we discussed in Chapter 1. Approaches that are exclusively

behavioral, it is argued, are not sensitive enough to students’ intrinsic, self-sustaining motivations.

As we pointed out in Chapter 1, there is truth to this allegation if a teacher actually does rely on rewarding

behaviors that she alone has chosen, or even if she persists in reinforcing behaviors that students already find

motivating without external reinforcement. In those cases reinforcements can backfire: instead of serving as an

incentive to desired behavior, reinforcement can become a reminder of the teacher’s power and of students’ lack of

control over their own actions. A classic research study of intrinsic motivation illustrated the problem nicely. In the

study, researchers rewarded university students for two activities—solving puzzles and writing newspaper headlines

—that they already found interesting. Some of the students, however, were paid to do these activities, whereas

others were not. Under these conditions, the students who were paid were less likely to engage in the activities

following the experiment than were the students who were not paid, even though both groups had been equally

interested in the activities to begin with (Deci, 1971). The extrinsic reward of payment, it seemed, interfered with

the intrinsic reward of working the puzzles.

Later studies confirmed this effect in numerous situations, though they have also found certain conditions where

extrinsic rewards do not reduce intrinsic rewards. Extrinsic rewards are not as harmful, for example, if a person is

paid “by the hour” (i.e. by a flat rate) rather than piecemeal (by the number of items completed) (Cameron & Pierce,

1994; Eisenberger & Cameron, 1996). They also are less harmful if the task itself is relatively well-defined (like

working math problems or playing solitaire) and high-quality performance is expected at all times. So there are still

times and ways when externally determined reinforcements are useful and effective. In general, however, extrinsic

rewards do seem to undermine intrinsic motivation often enough that they need to be used selectively and

thoughtfully (Deci, Koestner, & Ryan, 2001). As it happens, help with being selective and thoughtful can be found in

the other, more cognitively oriented theories of motivation. These use the goals, interests, and beliefs of students as

ways of explaining differences in students’ motives and in how the motives affect engagement with school. We turn

to these cognitively oriented theories next, beginning with those focused on students’ goals.

Motives as goals

One way motives vary is by the kind of goals that students set for themselves, and by how the goals support

students’ academic achievement. As you might suspect, some goals encourage academic achievement more than

others, but even motives that do not concern academics explicitly tend to affect learning indirectly.

Educational Psychology 113 A Global Text

6. Student motivation

Goals that contribute to achievement

What kinds of achievement goals do students hold? Imagine three individuals, Maria, Sara, and Lindsay, who

are taking algebra together. Maria’s main concern is to learn the material as well as possible because she finds it

interesting and because she believes it will be useful to her in later courses, perhaps at university. Hers is a mastery

goal because she wants primarily to learn or master the material. Sara, however, is concerned less about algebra

than about getting top marks on the exams and in the course. Hers is a performance goal because she is focused

primarily on looking successful; learning algebra is merely a vehicle for performing well in the eyes of peers and

teachers. Lindsay, for her part, is primarily concerned about avoiding a poor or failing mark. Hers is a performance-

avoidance goal or failure-avoidance goal because she is not really as concerned about learning algebra, as Maria is,

or about competitive success, as Sara is; she is simply intending to avoid failure.

As you might imagine, mastery, performance, and performance-avoidance goals often are not experienced in

pure form, but in combinations. If you play the clarinet in the school band, you might want to improve your

technique simply because you enjoy playing as well as possible—essentially a mastery orientation. But you might

also want to look talented in the eyes of classmates—a performance orientation. Another part of what you may wish,

at least privately, is to avoid looking like a complete failure at playing the clarinet. One of these motives may

predominate over the others, but they all may be present.

Mastery goals tend to be associated with enjoyment of learning the material at hand, and in this sense represent

an outcome that teachers often seek for students. By definition therefore they are a form of intrinsic motivation. As

such mastery goals have been found to be better than performance goals at sustaining students’ interest in a

subject. In one review of research about learning goals, for example, students with primarily mastery orientations

toward a course they were taking not only tended to express greater interest in the course, but also continued to

express interest well beyond the official end of the course, and to enroll in further courses in the same subject

(Harackiewicz, et al., 2002; Wolters, 2004).

Performance goals, on the other hand, imply extrinsic motivation, and tend to show the mixed effects of this

orientation. A positive effect is that students with a performance orientation do tend to get higher grades than those

who express primarily a mastery orientation. The advantage in grades occurs both in the short term (with

individual assignments) and in the long term (with overall grade point average when graduating). But there is

evidence that performance oriented students do not actually learn material as deeply or permanently as students

who are more mastery oriented (Midgley, Kaplan, & Middleton, 2001). A possible reason is that measures of

performance—such as test scores—often reward relatively shallow memorization of information and therefore guide

performance-oriented students away from processing the information thoughtfully or deeply. Another possible

reason is that a performance orientation, by focusing on gaining recognition as the best among peers, encourages

competition among peers. Giving and receiving help from classmates is thus not in the self-interest of a

performance-oriented student, and the resulting isolation limits the student’s learning.

Goals that affect achievement indirectlyFailure-avoidant goals

As we mentioned, failure-avoidant goals by nature undermine academic achievement. Often they are a negative

byproduct of the competitiveness of performance goals (Urdan, 2004). If a teacher (and sometimes also fellow

114

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

students) put too much emphasis on being the best in the class, and if interest in learning the material as such

therefore suffers, then some students may decide that success is beyond their reach or may not be desirable in any

case. The alternative—simply avoiding failure—may seem wiser as well as more feasible. Once a student adopts this

attitude, he or she may underachieve more or less deliberately, doing only the minimum work necessary to avoid

looking foolish or to avoid serious conflict with the teacher. Avoiding failure in this way is an example of self-

handicapping—deliberate actions and choices that the reduce chances of success. Students may self-handicap in a

number of ways; in addition to not working hard, they may procrastinate about completing assignments, for

example, or set goals that are unrealistically high.

Social goals

Most students need and value relationships, both with classmates and with teachers, and often (though not

always) they get a good deal of positive support from the relationships. But the effects of social relationships are

complex, and at times can work both for and against academic achievement. If a relationship with the teacher is

important and reasonably positive, then the student is likely to try pleasing the teacher by working hard on

assignments (Dowson & McInerney, 2003). Note, though, that this effect is closer to performance than mastery; the

student is primarily concerned about looking good to someone else. If, on the other hand, a student is especially

concerned about relationships with peers, the effects on achievement depend on the student’s motives for the

relationship, as well as on peers’ attitudes. Desiring to be close to peers personally may lead a student to ask for

help from, and give help to peers—a behavior that may support higher achievement, at least up to a point. But

desiring to impress peers with skills and knowledge may lead to the opposite: as we already mentioned, the

competitive edge of such a performance orientation may keep the student from collaborating, and in this indirect

way reduce a student’s opportunities to learn. The abilities and achievement motivation of peers themselves can

also make a difference, but once again the effects vary depending on the context. Low achievement and motivation

by peers affects an individual’s academic motivation more in elementary school than in high school, more in

learning mathematics than learning to read, and more if their is a wide range of abilities in a classroom than if

there is a more narrow range (Burke & Sass, 2006).

In spite of these complexities, social relationships are valued so highly by most students that teachers should

generally facilitate them, though also keep an eye on their nature and their consequent effects on achievement. As

we explain further, many assignments can be accomplished productively in groups, for example, as long as the

groups are formed thoughtfully; in that chapter we discuss some ways of insuring that such groups are successful,

such as by choosing group tasks wisely and recognizing all members’ contributions are fully as possible.

Relationships can also be supported with activities that involve students or adults from another class or from

outside the school, as often happens with school or community service projects. These can provide considerable

social satisfaction and can sometimes be connected to current curriculum needs (Butin, 2005). But the majority of

students’ social contacts are likely always to come from students’ own initiatives with each other in simply taking

time to talk and interact. The teacher’s job is to encourage these informal contacts, especially when they happen at

times that support rather than interfere with learning.

Encouraging mastery goals

Even though a degree of performance orientation may be inevitable in school because of the mere presence of

classmates, it does not have to take over students’ academic motivation completely. Teachers can encourage

Educational Psychology 115 A Global Text

6. Student motivation

mastery goals in various ways, and should in fact do so because a mastery orientation leads to more sustained,

thoughtful learning, at least in classrooms, where classmates may sometimes debate and disagree with each other

(Darnon, Butera, & Harackiewicz, 2006).

How can teachers do so? One way is to allow students to choose specific tasks or assignments for themselves,

where possible, because their choices are more likely than usual to reflect prior personal interests, and hence be

motivated more intrinsically than usual. The limitation of this strategy, of course, is that students may not see some

of the connections between their prior interests and the curriculum topics at hand. In that case it also helps for the

teacher to look for and point out the relevance of current topics or skills to students’ personal interests and goals.

Suppose, for example, that a student enjoys the latest styles of music. This interest may actually have connections

with a wide range of school curriculum, such as:

• biology (because of the physiology of the ear and of hearing)

• physics or general science (because of the nature of musical acoustics)

• history (because of changes in musical styles over time)

• English (because of relationships of musical lyrics and themes with literary themes)

• foreign languages (because of comparisons of music and songs among cultures)

Still another way to encourage mastery orientation is to focus on students’ individual effort and improvement as

much as possible, rather than on comparing students’ successes to each other. You can encourage this orientation

by giving students detailed feedback about how they can improve performance, or by arranging for students to

collaborate on specific tasks and projects rather than to compete about them, and in general by showing your own

enthusiasm for the subject at hand.

Motives as interests

In addition to holding different kinds of goals—with consequent differences in academic motivation—students

show obvious differences in levels of interest in the topics and tasks of the classroom. Suppose that two high school

classmates, Frank and Jason, both are taking chemistry, and specifically learning how to balance chemical

equations. Frank finds the material boring and has to force himself to study it; as a result he spends only the time

needed to learn the basic material and to complete the assignments at a basic level. Jason, on the other hand,

enjoys the challenges of balancing chemical equations. He thinks of the task as an intriguing puzzle; he not only

solves each of them, but also compares the problems to each other as he goes through them.

Frank’s learning is based on effort compared to Jason’s, whose learning is based more fully on interest. As the

example implies, when students learn from interest they tend to devote more attention to the topic than if they

learn from effort (Hidi & Renninger, 2006). The finding is not surprising since interest is another aspect of

intrinsic motivation—energy or drive that comes from within. A distinction between effort and interest is often

artificial, however, because the two motives often get blended or combined in students’ personal experiences. Most

of us can remember times when we worked at a skill that we enjoyed and found interesting, but that also required

effort to learn. The challenge for teachers is therefore to draw on and encourage students’ interest as much as

possible, and thus keep the required effort within reasonable bounds—neither too hard nor too easy.

116

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Situational interest versus personal interest

Students’ interests vary in how deeply or permanently they are located within students. Situational interests

are ones that are triggered temporarily by features of the immediate situation. Unusual sights, sounds, or words can

stimulate situational interest. A teacher might show an interesting image on the overhead projector, or play a brief

bit of music, or make a surprising comment in passing. At a more abstract level, unusual or surprising topics of

discussion can also arouse interest when they are first introduced. Personal interests are relatively permanent

preferences of the student, and are usually expressed in a variety of situations. In the classroom, a student may (or

may not) have a personal interest in particular topics, activities, or subject matter. Outside class, though, he or she

usually has additional personal interests in particular non-academic activities (e.g. sports, music) or even in

particular people (a celebrity, a friend who lives nearby). The non-academic personal interests may sometimes

conflict with academic interest; it may be more interesting to go to the shopping mall with a friend than to study

even your most favorite subject.

Benefits of personal interest

In general, personal interest in an academic topic or activity tends to correlate with achievement related to the

topic or activity. As you might suppose, a student who is truly interested is more likely to focus on the topic or

activity more fully, to work at it for longer periods, to use more thoughtful strategies in learning—and to enjoy

doing so (Hidi, 2001; Hidi & Renninger, 2006). Small wonder that the student achieves more! Note, though, a

persistent ambiguity about this benefit: it is often not clear whether personal interest leads to higher achievement,

or higher achievement leads to stronger interest. Either possibility seems plausible. Research to sort them out,

however, has suggested that at least some of the influence goes in the direction from interest to achievement; when

elementary students were given books from which to learn about a new topic, for example, they tended to learn

more from books which they chose themselves than from books that were simply assigned (Reynolds & Symons,

2001). So interest seemed to lead to learning. But this conclusion does not rule out its converse, that achievement

may stimulate interest as well. As Joe learns more about history, he steadily finds history more interesting; as

McKenzie learns more about biology, she gradually wants to learn more of it.

Stimulating situational interests

If a student has little prior personal interest in a topic or activity, the teacher is faced with stimulating initial,

situational interest, in hopes that the initial interest will gradually become more permanent and personal. There are

a number of strategies for meeting this challenge:

• It helps to include surprises in your comments and in classroom activities from time to time: tell students

facts that are true but counter-intuitive, for example, or demonstrate a science experiment that turns out

differently than students expect (Guthrie, Wigfield, & Humenick, 2006).

• It also helps to relate new material to students’ prior experiences even if their experiences are not related to

academics or to school directly. The concepts of gravitation and acceleration, for example, operate every

time a ball is hit or thrown in a softball game. If this connection is pointed out to a student who enjoys

playing a lot of softball, the concepts can make concepts more interesting.

Educational Psychology 117 A Global Text

6. Student motivation

• It helps to encourage students to respond to new material actively. By having students talk about the

material together, for example, students can begin making their own connections to prior personal

interests, and the social interaction itself helps to link the material to their personal, social interests as well.

A caution: seductive details

Even though it is important to stimulate interest in new material somehow, it is also possible to mislead or

distract students accidentally by adding inappropriate, but stimulating features to new material (Garner, et al.,

1992; Harp & Mayer, 1998). Distractions happen a number of ways, such as any of these among others:

• deliberately telling jokes in class

• using colorful illustrations or pictures

• adding interesting bits of information to a written or verbal explanation

When well chosen, all of these moves can indeed arouse students’ interest in a new topic. But if they do not

really relate to the topic at hand, they may simply create misunderstandings or prevent students from focusing on

key material. As with most other learning processes, however, there are individual differences among students in

distractability, students who are struggling, and are more prone to distraction and misunderstanding than students

who are already learning more successfully (Sanchez & Wiley, 2006). On balance the best advice is probably

therefore to use strategies to arouse situational interest, but to assess students’ responses to them continually and

as honestly as possible. The key issue is whether students seem to learn because of stimulating strategies that you

provide, or in spite of them.

Motives related to attributions

Attributions are perceptions about the causes of success and failure. Suppose that you get a low mark on a test

and are wondering what caused the low mark. You can construct various explanations for—make various

attributions about—this failure. Maybe you did not study very hard; maybe the test itself was difficult; maybe you

were unlucky; maybe you just are not smart enough. Each explanation attributes the failure to a different factor.

The explanations that you settle upon may reflect the truth accurately—or then again, they may not. What is

important about attributions is that they reflect personal beliefs about the sources or causes of success and failure.

As such, they tend to affect motivation in various ways, depending on the nature of the attribution (Weiner, 2005).

Locus, stability, and controllability

Attributions vary in three underlying ways: locus, stability, and controllability. Locus of an attribution is the

location (figuratively speaking) of the source of success or failure. If you attribute a top mark on a test to your

ability, then the locus is internal; if you attribute the mark to the test’s having easy questions, then the locus is

external. The stability of an attribution is its relative permanence. If you attribute the mark to your ability, then

the source of success is relatively stable—by definition, ability is a relatively lasting quality. If you attribute a top

mark to the effort you put in to studying, then the source of success is unstable—effort can vary and has to be

renewed on each occasion or else it disappears. The controllability of an attribution is the extent to which the

individual can influence it. If you attribute a top mark to your effort at studying, then the source of success is

relatively controllable—you can influence effort simply by deciding how much to study. But if you attribute the

118

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

mark to simple luck, then the source of the success is uncontrollable—there is nothing that can influence random

chance.

As you might suspect, the way that these attributions combine affects students’ academic motivations in major

ways. It usually helps both motivation and achievement if a student attributes academic successes and failures to

factors that are internal and controllable, such as effort or a choice to use particular learning strategies (Dweck,

2000). Attributing successes to factors that are internal but stable or controllable (like ability), on the other hand, is

both a blessing and a curse: sometimes it can create optimism about prospects for future success (“I always do

well”), but it can also lead to indifference about correcting mistakes (Dweck, 2006), or even create pessimism if a

student happens not to perform at the accustomed level (“Maybe I’m not as smart as I thought”). Worst of all for

academic motivation are attributions, whether stable or not, related to external factors. Believing that performance

depends simply on luck (“The teacher was in a bad mood when marking”) or on excessive difficulty of material

removes incentive for a student to invest in learning. All in all, then, it seems important for teachers to encourage

internal, stable attributions about success.

Influencing students’ attributions

How can they do so? One way or another, the effective strategies involve framing teachers’ own explanations of

success and failure around internal, controllable factors. Instead of telling a student: “Good work! You’re smart!”,

try saying: “Good work! Your effort really made a difference, didn’t it?” If a student fails, instead of saying,“Too

bad! This material is just too hard for you,” try saying, “Let’s find a strategy for practicing this more, and then you

can try again.” In both cases the first option emphasizes uncontrollable factors (effort, difficulty level), and the

second option emphasizes internal, controllable factors (effort, use of specific strategies).

Such attributions will only be convincing, however, if teachers provide appropriate conditions for students to

learn—conditions in which students’ efforts really do pay off. There are three conditions that have to be in place in

particular. First, academic tasks and materials actually have to be at about the right level of difficulty. If you give

problems in advanced calculus to a first-grade student, the student will not only fail them but also be justified in

attributing the failure to an external factor, task difficulty. If assignments are assessed in ways that produce highly

variable, unreliable marks, then students will rightly attribute their performance to an external, unstable source:

luck. Both circumstances will interfere with motivation.

Second, teachers also need to be ready to give help to individuals who need it—even if they believe that an

assignment is easy enough or clear enough that students should not need individual help. Readiness to help is

always essential because it is often hard to know in advance exactly how hard a task will prove to be for particular

students. Without assistance, a task that proves difficult initially may remain difficult indefinitely, and the student

will be tempted to make unproductive, though correct, attributions about his or her failure (“I will never

understand this”, “I’m not smart enough”, or “It doesn’t matter how hard I study”).

Third, teachers need to remember that ability—usually considered a relatively stable factor—often actually

changes incrementally over the long term. Recognizing this fact is one of the best ways to bring about actual

increases in students’ abilities (Blackwell, Trzniewski, & Dweck, 2007; Schunk, Pintrich, & Meese, 2008). A middle-

years student might play the trumpet in the school band at a high level of ability, but this ability actually reflects a

lot of previous effort and a gradual increase in ability. A second grade student who reads fluently, in this sense may

Educational Psychology 119 A Global Text

6. Student motivation

have high current ability to read; but at some point in the distant past that same student could not read as well, and

even further back he may not have been able to read at all. The increases in ability have happened at least in part

because of effort. While these ideas may seem obvious, they can easily be forgotten in the classroom because effort

and ability evolve according to very different time frames. Effort and its results appear relatively immediately; a

student expends effort this week, this day, or even at this very moment, and the effort (if not the results) are visible

right away. But ability may take longer to show itself; a student often develops it only over many weeks, months, or

years.

Motivation as self-efficacy

In addition to being influenced by their goals, interests, and attributions, students’ motives are affected by

specific beliefs about the student’s personal capacities. In self-efficacy theory the beliefs become a primary,

explicit explanation for motivation (Bandura, 1977, 1986, 1997). Self-efficacy is the belief that you are capable of

carrying out a specific task or of reaching a specific goal. Note that the belief and the action or goal are specific. Self-

efficacy is a belief that you can write an acceptable term paper, for example, or repair an automobile, or make

friends with the new student in class. These are relatively specific beliefs and tasks. Self-efficacy is not about

whether you believe that you are intelligent in general, whether you always like working with mechanical things, or

think that you are generally a likeable person. These more general judgments are better regarded as various

mixtures of self-concepts (beliefs about general personal identity) or of self-esteem (evaluations of identity). They

are important in their own right, and sometimes influence motivation, but only indirectly (Bong & Skaalvik, 2004).

Self-efficacy beliefs, furthermore, are not the same as “true” or documented skill or ability. They are self-

constructed, meaning that they are personally developed perceptions. There can sometimes therefore be

discrepancies between a person’s self-efficacy beliefs and the person’s abilities. You can believe that you can write a

good term paper, for example, without actually being able to do so, and vice versa: you can believe yourself

incapable of writing a paper, but discover that you are in fact able to do so. In this way self-efficacy is like the

everyday idea of confidence, except that it is defined more precisely. And as with confidence, it is possible to have

either too much or too little self-efficacy. The optimum level seems to be either at or slightly above true capacity

(Bandura, 1997). As we indicate below, large discrepancies between self-efficacy and ability can create motivational

problems for the individual.

Effects of self-efficacy on students’ behavior

Self-efficacy may sound like a uniformly desirable quality, but research as well as teachers’ experience suggests

that its effects are a bit more complicated than they first appear. Self-efficacy has three main effects, each of which

has both a “dark” or undesirable side and a positive or desirable side.

Choice of tasks

The first effect is that self-efficacy makes students more willing to choose tasks where they already feel confident

of succeeding. This effect is almost inevitable, given the definition of the concept of self-efficacy, it has also been

supported by research on self-efficacy beliefs (Pajares & Schunk, 2001). For teachers, the effect on choice can be

either welcome or not, depending on circumstances. If a student believes that he or she can solve mathematical

problems, then the student is more likely to attempt the mathematics homework that the teacher assigns.

Unfortunately the converse is also true. If a student believes that he or she is incapable of math, then the student is

120

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

less likely to attempt the math homework (perhaps telling himself, “What’s the use of trying?”), regardless of the

student’s actual ability in math.

Since self-efficacy is self-constructed, furthermore, it is also possible for students to miscalculate or misperceive

their true skill, and the misperceptions themselves can have complex effects on students’ motivations. From a

teacher’s point of view, all is well even if students overestimate their capacity but actually do succeed at a relevant

task anyway, or if they underestimate their capacity, yet discover that they can succeed and raise their self-efficacy

beliefs as a result. All may not be well, though, if students do not believe that they can succeed and therefore do not

even try, or if students overestimate their capacity by a wide margin, but are disappointed unexpectedly by failure

and lower their self-efficacy beliefs.

Persistence at tasks

A second effect of high self-efficacy is to increase a persistence at relevant tasks. If you believe that you can solve

crossword puzzles, but encounter one that takes longer than usual, then you are more likely to work longer at the

puzzle until you (hopefully) really do solve it. This is probably a desirable behavior in many situations, unless the

persistence happens to interfere with other, more important tasks (what if you should be doing homework instead

of working on crossword puzzles?). If you happen to have low self-efficacy for crosswords, on the other hand, then

you are more likely to give up early on a difficult puzzle. Giving up early may often be undesirable because it

deprives you of a chance to improve your skill by persisting. Then again (on the third hand?), the consequent lack of

success because of giving up may provide a useful incentive to improve your crossword skills. And again,

misperceptions of capacity make a difference. Overestimating your capacity by a lot (excessively high self-efficacy)

might lead you not to prepare for or focus on a task properly, and thereby impair your performance. So as with

choosing tasks, the effects of self-efficacy vary from one individual to another and one situation to another. The

teacher’s task is therefore two-fold: first, to discern the variations, and second, to encourage the positive self-

efficacy beliefs. Table 15 offers some additional advice about how to do this.

Table 15: Ways of encouraging self-efficacy beliefs

Strategy Example of what the teacher might say

1. Set goals with students, and get a commitment

from them to reach the goals.

“By the end of the month, I want you to know all of

the times table up to 25 x 25. Can I count on you to do

that?”

2. Encourage students to compare their performance

with their own previous performance, not with other

students.

“Compare that drawing against the one that you

made last semester. I think you’ll find improvements!”

3. Point out links between effort and improvement. “I saw you studying for this test more this week. No

wonder you did better this time!”

4. In giving feedback about performance, focus on

information, not evaluative judgments.

“Part 1 of the lab write-up was very detailed, just as

the assignment asked. Part 2 has a lot of good ideas in

it, but it needs to be more detailed and stated more

explicitly.”

Educational Psychology 121 A Global Text

6. Student motivation

5. Point out that increases in knowledge or skill

happen gradually by sustained effort, not because of

inborn ability.

“Every time I read another one of your essays, I see

more good ideas than the last time. They are so much

more complete than when you started the year.”

Response to failure

High self-efficacy for a task not only increases a person’s persistence at the task, but also improves their ability

to cope with stressful conditions and to recover their motivation following outright failures. Suppose that you have

two assignments—an essay and a science lab report—due on the same day, and this circumstance promises to make

your life hectic as you approach the deadline. You will cope better with the stress of multiple assignments if you

already believe yourself capable of doing both of the tasks, than if you believe yourself capable of doing just one of

them or (especially) of doing neither. You will also recover better in the unfortunate event that you end up with a

poor grade on one or even both of the tasks.

That is the good news. The bad news, at least from a teacher’s point of view, is that the same resilience can

sometimes also serve non-academic and non-school purposes. How so? Suppose, instead of two school assignments

due on the same day, a student has only one school assignment due, but also holds a part-time evening job as a

server in a local restaurant. Suppose, further, that the student has high self-efficacy for both of these tasks; he

believes, in other words, that he is capable of completing the assignment as well as continuing to work at the job.

The result of such resilient beliefs can easily be a student who devotes less attention to school work than ideal, and

who even ends up with a lower grade on the assignment than he or she is capable of.

Learned helplessness and self-efficacy

If a person’s sense of self-efficacy is very low, he or she can develop learned helplessness, a perception of

complete lack of control in mastering a task. The attitude is similar to depression, a pervasive feeling of apathy and

a belief that effort makes no difference and does not lead to success. Learned helplessness was originally studied

from the behaviorist perspective of classical and operant conditioning by the psychologist Martin Seligman (1995).

The studies used a somewhat “gloomy” experimental procedure in which an animal, such as a rat or a dog, was

repeatedly shocked in a cage in a way that prevented the animal from escaping the shocks. In a later phase of the

procedure, conditions were changed so that the animal could avoid the shocks by merely moving from one side of

the cage to the other. Yet frequently they did not bother to do so! Seligman called this behavior learned

helplessness.

In people, learned helplessness leads to characteristic ways of dealing with problems. They tend to attribute the

source of a problem to themselves, to generalize the problem to many aspects of life, and to see the problem as

lasting or permanent. More optimistic individuals, in contrast, are more likely to attribute a problem to outside

sources, to see it as specific to a particular situation or activity, and to see it as temporary or time-limited. Consider,

for example, two students who each fail a test. The one with a lot of learned helplessness is more likely to explain

the failure by saying something like: “I’m stupid; I never perform well on any schoolwork, and I never will perform

well at it.” The other, more optimistic student is more likely to say something like: “The teacher made the test too

hard this time, so the test doesn’t prove anything about how I will do next time or in other subjects.”

122

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

What is noteworthy about these differences in perception is how much the more optimistic of these perspectives

resembles high self-efficacy and how much learned helplessness seems to contradict or differ from it. As already

noted, high self-efficacy is a strong belief in one’s capacity to carry out a specific task successfully. By definition

therefore self-efficacy focuses attention on a temporary or time-limited activity (the task), even though the cause of

successful completion (oneself) is “internal”. Teachers can minimize learned helplessness in students, therefore, by

encouraging their self-efficacy beliefs. There are several ways of doing this, as we explain next.

Sources of self-efficacy beliefs

Psychologists who study self-efficacy have identified four major sources of self-efficacy beliefs (Pajares &

Schunk, 2001, 2002). In order of importance they are (1) prior experiences of mastering tasks, (2) watching others’

mastering tasks, (3) messages or “persuasion” from others, and (4) emotions related to stress and discomfort.

Fortunately the first three can be influenced by teachers directly, and even the fourth can sometimes be influenced

indirectly by appropriate interpretive comments from the teacher or others.

Prior experiences of mastery

Not surprisingly, past successes at a task increase students’ beliefs that they will succeed again in the future. The

implication of this basic fact means that teachers need to help students build a history of successes. Whether they

are math problems, reading assignments, or athletic activities, tasks have to end with success more often than with

failure. Note, though, that the successes have to represent mastery that is genuine or competence that is truly

authentic. Success at tasks that are trivial or irrelevant do not improve self-efficacy beliefs, nor does praise for

successes that a student has not really had (Erikson, 1968/1994).

As a practical matter, creating a genuine history of success is most convincing if teachers also work to broaden a

student’s vision of “the past”. Younger students (elementary-age) in particular have relatively short or limited ideas

of what counts as “past experience”; they may go back only a few occasions when forming impressions of whether

they can succeed again in the future (Eccles, et al., 1998). Older students (secondary school) gradually develop

longer views of their personal “pasts”, both because of improvements in memory and because of accumulating a

personal history that is truly longer. The challenge for working with any age, however, is to insure that students

base self-efficacy beliefs on all relevant experiences from their pasts, not just on selected or recent experiences.

Watching others’ experiences of mastery

A second source of efficacy beliefs comes from vicarious experience of mastery, or observing others’ successes

(Schunk & Zimmerman, 1997). Simply seeing someone else succeed at a task, in other words, can contribute to

believing that you, too, can succeed. The effect is stronger when the observer lacks experience with the task and

therefore may be unsure of his or her own ability. It is also stronger when the model is someone respected by the

observer, such as a student’s teacher, or a peer with generally comparable ability. Even under these conditions,

though, vicarious experience is not as influential as direct experience. The reasons are not hard to imagine.

Suppose, for example, you witness both your teacher and a respected friend succeed at singing a favorite tune, but

you are unsure whether you personally can sing. In that case you may feel encouraged about your own potential,

but are likely still to feel somewhat uncertain of your own efficacy. If on the other hand you do not witness others’

singing, but you have a history of singing well yourself, it is a different story. In that case you are likely to believe in

your efficacy, regardless of how others perform.

Educational Psychology 123 A Global Text

6. Student motivation

All of which suggests that to a modest extent, teachers may be able to enhance students’ self-efficacy by

modeling success at a task or by pointing out classmates who are successful. These strategies can work because they

not only show how to do a task, but also communicate a more fundamental message, the fact that the task can in

fact be done. If students are learning a difficult arithmetic procedure, for example, you can help by demonstrating

the procedure, or by pointing out classmates who are doing it. Note, though, that vicarious mastery is helpful only if

backed up with real successes performed by the students themselves. It is also helpful only if the “model

classmates” are perceived as truly comparable in ability. Overuse of vicarious models, especially in the absence of

real success by learners, can cause learners to disqualify a model’s success; students may simply decide that the

model is “out of their league” in skills and is therefore irrelevant to judging their own potential.

Social messages and persuasion

A third source of efficacy beliefs are encouragements, both implied and stated, that persuade a person of his or

her capacity to do a task. Persuasion does not create high efficacy by itself, but it often increases or supports it when

coupled with either direct or vicarious experience, especially when the persuasion comes from more than one

person (Goddard, Hoy, & Hoy, 2004).

For teachers, this suggests two things. The first, of course, is that encouragement can motivate students,

especially when it is focused on achievable, specific tasks. It can be motivating to say things like: “I think you can do

it” or “I’ve seen you do this before, so I know that you can do it again”. But the second implication is that teachers

should arrange wherever possible to support their encouragement by designing tasks at hand that are in fact

achievable by the student. Striking a balance of encouragement and task difficulty may seem straightforward, but

sometimes it can be challenging because students can sometimes perceive teachers’ comments and tasks quite

differently from how teachers intend. Giving excessive amounts of detailed help, for example, may be intended as

support for a student, but be taken as a lack of confidence in the student’s ability to do the task independently.

Emotions related to success, stress or discomfort

The previous three sources of efficacy beliefs are all rather cognitive or “thinking oriented”, but emotions also

influence expectations of success or failure. Feeling nervous or anxious just before speaking to a large group

(sometimes even just a class full of students!) can function like a message that says “I’m not going to succeed at

doing this”, even if there is in fact good reason to expect success. But positive feelings can also raise beliefs about

efficacy. When recalling the excitement of succeeding at a previous, unrelated task, people may overestimate their

chances of success at a new task with which they have no previous experience, and are therefore in no position to

predict their efficacy.

For teachers, the most important implication is that students’ motivation can be affected when they generalize

from past experience which they believe, rightly or wrongly, to be relevant. By simply announcing a test, for

example, a teacher can make some students anxious even before the students find out anything about the test—

whether it is easy or difficult, or even comparable in any way to other experiences called “tests” in their pasts.

Conversely, it can be misleading to encourage students on the basis of their success at past academic tasks if the

earlier tasks were not really relevant to requirements of the new tasks at hand. Suppose, for example, that a middle-

years student has previously written only brief opinion-based papers, and never written a research-based paper. In

124

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

that case boosting the student’s confidence by telling him that “it is just like the papers you wrote before” may not

be helpful or even honest.

A caution: motivation as content versus motivation as process

A caution about self-efficacy theory is its heavy emphasis on just the process of motivation, at the expense of the

content of motivation. The basic self-efficacy model has much to say about how beliefs affect behavior, but

relatively little to say about which beliefs and tasks are especially satisfying or lead to the greatest well-being in

students. The answer to this question is important to know, since teachers might then select tasks as much as

possible that are intrinsically satisfying, and not merely achievable.

Another way of posing this concern is by asking: “Is it possible to feel high self-efficacy about a task that you do

not enjoy?” It does seem quite possible for such a gap to exist. As a youth, for example, one of us (Kelvin Seifert)

had considerable success with solving mathematics problems in high school algebra, and expended considerable

effort doing algebra assignments as homework. Before long, he had developed high self-efficacy with regard to

solving such problems. But Kelvin never really enjoyed solving the algebra problems, and later even turned away

permanently from math or science as a career (much to the disappointment of his teachers and family). In this case

self-efficacy theory nicely explained the process of his motivation—Kelvin’s belief in his capacity led to persistence

at the tasks. But it did not explain the content of his motivation—his growing dislike of the tasks. Accounting for

such a gap requires a different theory of motivation, one that includes not only specific beliefs, but “deeper”

personal needs as well. An example of this approach is self-determination theory, where we turn next.

Motivation as self-determination

Common sense suggests that human motivations originate from some sort of inner “need”. We all think of

ourselves as having various “needs”, a need for food, for example, or a need for companionship—that influences our

choices and activities. This same idea also forms part of some theoretical accounts of motivation, though the

theories differ in the needs that they emphasize or recognize. In Chapter 2, for example, we talked about Maslow’s

hierarchy of needs as an example of motivations that function like needs that influence long-term personal

development. According to Maslow, individuals must satisfy physical survival needs before they seek to satisfy

needs of belonging, they satisfy belonging needs before esteem needs, and so on. In theory, too, people have both

deficit needs and growth needs, and the deficit needs must be satisfied before growth needs can influence behavior

(Maslow, 1970). In Maslow’s theory, as in others that use the concept, a need is a relatively lasting condition or

feeling that requires relief or satisfaction and that tends to influence action over the long term. Some needs may

decrease when satisfied (like hunger), but others may not (like curiosity). Either way, needs differ from the self-

efficacy beliefs discussed earlier, which are relatively specific and cognitive, and affect particular tasks and

behaviors fairly directly.

A recent theory of motivation based on the idea of needs is self-determination theory, proposed by the

psychologists Edward Deci and Richard Ryan (2000), among others. The theory proposes that understanding

motivation requires taking into account three basic human needs:

• autonomy—the need to feel free of external constraints on behavior

• competence—the need to feel capable or skilled

Educational Psychology 125 A Global Text

6. Student motivation

• relatedness—the need to feel connected or involved with others

Note that these needs are all psychological, not physical; hunger and sex, for example, are not on the list. They

are also about personal growth or development, not about deficits that a person tries to reduce or eliminate. Unlike

food (in behaviorism) or safety (in Maslow’s hierarchy), you can never get enough of autonomy, competence, or

relatedness. You (and your students) will seek to enhance these continually throughout life.

The key idea of self-determination theory is that when persons (such as you or one of your students) feel that

these basic needs are reasonably well met, they tend to perceive their actions and choices to be intrinsically

motivated or “self-determined”. In that case they can turn their attention to a variety of activities that they find

attractive or important, but that do not relate directly to their basic needs. Among your students, for example, some

individuals might read books that you have suggested, and others might listen attentively when you explain key

concepts from the unit that you happen to be teaching. If one or more basic needs are not met well, however, people

will tend to feel coerced by outside pressures or external incentives. They may become preoccupied, in fact, with

satisfying whatever need has not been met and thus exclude or avoid activities that might otherwise be interesting,

educational, or important. If the persons are students, their learning will suffer.

Self-determination and intrinsic motivation

In proposing the importance of needs, then, self-determination theory is asserting the importance of intrinsic

motivation, an idea that has come up before in this book (see especially Chapter 1, about learning theory), and that

will come again later (see especially Chapter 9, about planning instruction). The self-determination version of

intrinsic motivation, however, emphasizes a person’s perception of freedom, rather than the presence or absence of

“real” constraints on action. Self-determination means a person feels free, even if the person is also operating

within certain external constraints. In principle, a student can experience self-determination even if the student

must, for example, live within externally imposed rules of appropriate classroom behavior. To achieve a feeling of

self-determination, however, the student’s basic needs must be met—needs for autonomy, competence, and

relatedness. In motivating students, then, the bottom line is that teachers have an interest in helping students to

meet their basic needs, and in not letting school rules or the teachers’ own leadership styles interfere with or block

satisfaction of students’ basic needs.

“Pure” self-determination may be the ideal for most teachers and students, of course, but the reality is usually

different. For a variety of reasons, teachers in most classrooms cannot be expected to meet all students’ basic needs

at all times. One reason is the sheer number of students, which makes it impossible to attend to every student

perfectly at all times. Another reason is teachers’ responsibility for a curriculum, which can require creating

expectations for students’ activities that sometimes conflict with students’ autonomy or makes them feel

(temporarily) less than fully competent. Still another reason is students’ personal histories, ranging from divorce to

poverty, which may create needs in some individuals which are beyond the power of teachers to remedy.

The result from students’ point of view is usually only a partial perception of self-determination, and therefore a

simultaneous mix of intrinsic and extrinsic motivations. Self-determination theory recognizes this reality by

suggesting that the “intrinsic-ness” of motivation is really a matter of degree, extending from highly extrinsic,

through various mixtures of intrinsic and extrinsic, to highly intrinsic (Koestner & Losier, 2004). At the extrinsic

end of the scale is learning that is regulated primarily by external rewards and constraints, whereas at the intrinsic

126

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

end is learning regulated primarily by learners themselves. Table 16 summarizes and gives examples of the various

levels and their effects on motivation. By assuming that motivation is often a mix of the intrinsic and extrinsic, the

job of the teacher becomes more realistic; the job is not to expect purely intrinsic motivation from students all the

time, but simply to arrange and encourage motivations that are as intrinsic as possible. To do this, the teacher

needs to support students’ basic needs for autonomy, competence, and relatedness.

Table 16: Combinations of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation

Source of regulation of

action

Description Example

“Pure” extrinsic motivation Person lacks the intention to take

any action, regardless of pressures

or incentives

Student completes no work even

when pressured or when incentives

are offered

Very external to person Actions regulated only by outside

pressures and incentives, and

controls

Student completes assignment

only if reminded explicitly of the

incentive of grades and/or negative

consequences of failing

Somewhat external Specific actions regulated

internally, but without reflection or

connection to personal needs

Student completes assignment

independently, but only because of

fear of shaming self or because of

guilt about consequences of not

completing assignment

Somewhat internal Actions recognized by individual

as important or as valuable as a

means to a more valued goal

Student generally completes

school work independently, but only

because of its value in gaining

admission to college

Very internal Actions adopted by individual as

integral to self-concept and to

person’s major personal values

Student generally completes

school work independently, because

being well educated is part of the

student’s concept of himself

“Pure” intrinsic regulation Actions practiced solely because

they are enjoyable and valued for

their own sake

Student enjoys every topic,

concept, and assignment that every

teacher ever assigns, and completes

school work solely because of his

enjoyment

Educational Psychology 127 A Global Text

6. Student motivation

Using self-determination theory in the classroom

What are some teaching strategies for supporting students’ needs? Educational researchers have studied this

question from a variety of directions, and their resulting recommendations converge and overlap in a number of

ways. For convenience, the recommendations can be grouped according to the basic need that they address,

beginning with the need for autonomy.

Supporting autonomy in learners

A major part of supporting autonomy is to give students choices wherever possible (Ryan & Lynch, 2003). The

choices that encourage the greatest feelings of self-control, obviously, are ones that are about relatively major issues

or that have relatively significant consequences for students, such as whom to choose as partners for a major group

project. But choices also encourage some feeling of self-control even when they are about relatively minor issues,

such as how to organize your desk or what kind of folder to use for storing your papers at school. It is important,

furthermore, to offer choices to all students, including students needing explicit directions in order to work

successfully; avoid reserving choices for only the best students or giving up offering choices altogether to students

who fall behind or who need extra help. All students will feel more self-determined and therefore more motivated if

they have choices of some sort.

Teachers can also support students’ autonomy more directly by minimizing external rewards (like grades) and

comparisons among students’ performance, and by orienting and responding themselves to students’ expressed

goals and interests. In teaching elementary students about climate change, for example, you can support autonomy

by exploring which aspects of this topic have already come to students’ attention and aroused their concern. The

point of the discussion would not be to find out “who knows the most” about this topic, but to build and enhance

students’ intrinsic motivations as much as possible. In reality, of course, it may not be possible to succeed at this

goal fully—some students may simply have no interest in the topic, for example, or you may be constrained by time

or resources from individualizing certain activities fully. But any degree of attention to students’ individuality, as

well as any degree of choice, will support students’ autonomy.

Supporting the need for competence

The most obvious way to make students feel competent is by selecting activities which are challenging but

nonetheless achievable with reasonable effort and assistance (Elliott, McGregor, & Thrash, 2004). Although few

teachers would disagree with this idea, there are times when it is hard to put into practice, such as when you first

meet a class at the start of a school year and therefore are unfamiliar with their backgrounds and interests. But

there are some strategies that are generally effective even if you are not yet in a position to know the students well.

One is to emphasize activities that require active response from students. Sometimes this simply means selecting

projects, experiments, discussions and the like that require students to do more than simply listen. Other times it

means expecting active responses in all interactions with students, such as by asking questions that call for

“divergent” (multiple or elaborated) answers. In a social studies class, for example, try asking “What are some ways

we could find out more about our community?” instead of “Tell me the three best ways to find out about our

community.” The first question invites more divergent, elaborate answers than the second.

Another generally effective way to support competence is to respond and give feedback as immediately as

possible. Tests and term papers help subsequent learning more if returned, with comments, sooner rather than

128

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

later. Discussions teach more if you include your own ideas in them, while still encouraging students’ input. Small

group and independent activities are more effective if you provide a convenient way for students to consult

authoritative sources for guidance when needed, whether the source is you personally, a teaching assistant, a

specially selected reading, or even a computer program. In addition, you can sometimes devise tasks that create a

feeling of competence because they have a “natural” solution or ending point. Assembling a jigsaw puzzle of the

community, for example, has this quality, and so does creating a jigsaw puzzle of the community if the students

need a greater challenge.

Supporting the need to relate to others

The main way of support students’ need to relate to others is to arrange activities in which students work

together in ways that are mutually supportive, that recognize students’ diversity, and minimize competition among

individuals. We will have more to say about this strategy in Chapter 8 (“Instructional strategies”), where we

describe several varieties of cooperative learning, as well as some of their pitfalls to be avoided. For now, simply

note that having students work together can happen in many ways. You can, for example, deliberately arrange

projects that require a variety of talents; some educators call such activities “rich group work” (Cohen, 1994; Cohen,

Brody, & Sapon-Shevin, 2004). In studying in small groups about medieval society, for example, one student can

contribute his drawing skills, another can contribute his writing skills, and still another can contribute his dramatic

skills. The result can be a multi-faceted presentation—written, visual, and oral. The groups needed for rich group

work provide for students’ relationships with each other, whether they contain six individuals or only two.

There are other ways to encourage relationships among students. In the jigsaw classroom (Aronson & Patnoe,

1997), for example, students work together in two phases. In the first phase, groups of “experts” work together to

find information on a specialized topic. In a second phase the expert groups split up and reform into “generalist”

groups containing one representative from each former expert group. In studying the animals of Africa, for

example, each expert group might find information about a different particular category of animal or plant; one

group might focus on mammal, another on bird, a third on reptiles, and so on. In the second phase of the jigsaw,

the generalist groups would pool information from the experts to get a more well-rounded view of the topic. The

generalist groups would each have an expert about mammals, for example, but also an expert about birds and about

reptiles.

As a teacher, you can add to these organizational strategies by encouraging the development of your own

relationships with class members. Your goal, as teacher, is to demonstrate caring and interest in your students not

just as students, but as people. The goal also involves behaving as if good relationships between and among class

members are not only possible, but ready to develop and perhaps even already developing. A simple tactic, for

example, is to speak of “we” and “us” as much as possible, rather than speaking of “you students”. Another tactic is

to present cooperative activities and assignments without apology, as if they are in the best interests not just of

students, but of “us all” in the classroom, yourself included.

Keeping self-determination in perspective

In certain ways self-determination theory provides a sensible way to think about students’ intrinsic motivation

and therefore to think about how to get them to manage their own learning. A particular strength of the theory is

that it recognizes degrees of self-determination and bases many ideas on this reality. Most people recognize

Educational Psychology 129 A Global Text

6. Student motivation

combinations of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation guiding particular activities in their own lives. We might enjoy

teaching, for example, but also do this job partly to receive a paycheck. To its credit, self-determination theory also

relies on a list of basic human needs—autonomy, competence, and relatedness—that relate comfortably with some

of the larger purposes of education.

Although these are positive features for understanding and influencing students’ classroom motivation, some

educators and psychologists nonetheless have lingering questions about the limitations of self-determination

theory. One is whether merely providing choices actually improves students’ learning, or simply improves their

satisfaction with learning. There is evidence supporting both possibilities (Flowerday & Schraw, 2003; Deci &

Ryan, 2003), and it is likely that there are teachers whose classroom experience supports both possibilities as well.

Another question is whether it is possible to overdo attention to students’ needs—and again there is evidence for

both favoring and contradicting this possibility. Too many choices can actually make anyone (not just a student)

frustrated and dissatisfied with a choice the person actually does make (Schwartz, 2004). Furthermore,

differentiating activities to students’ competence levels may be impractical if students are functioning at extremely

diverse levels within a single class, as sometimes happens. Differentiating may be inappropriate, too, if it holds a

teacher back from covering key curriculum objectives which students need and which at least some students are

able to learn. These are serious concerns, though in our opinion not serious enough to give up offering choices to

students or to stop differentiating instruction altogether. In Chapter 7 (“Classroom management and the learning

environment”), therefore, we explain the practical basis for this opinion, by describing workable ways for offering

choices and recognizing students’ diversity.

Expectancy x value: effects on students’ motivation

As we have explained in this chapter, motivation is affected by several factors, including reinforcement for

behavior, but especially also students’ goals, interests, and sense of self-efficacy and self-determination. The factors

combine to create two general sources of motivation: students’ expectation of success and the value that students

place on a goal. Viewing motivation in this way is often called the expectancy-value model of motivation (Wigfield &

Eccles, 2002; Wigfield, Tonk, & Eccles, 2004), and sometimes written with a multiplicative formula: expectancy x

value = motivation. The relationship between expectation and value is “multiplicative” rather than additive because

in order to be motivated, it is necessary for a person to have at least a modest expectation of success and to assign a

task at least some positive value. If you have high expectations of success but do not value a task at all (mentally

assign it a “0” value), then you will not feel motivated at all. Likewise, if you value a task highly but have no

expectation of success about completing it (assign it a “0” expectancy), then you also will not feel motivated at all.

Expectancies are the result of various factors, but particularly the goals held by a student, and the student’s self-

efficacy, which we discussed earlier in this chapter. A student with mastery goals and strong self-efficacy for a task,

for example, is likely to hold high expectations for success—almost by definition. Values are also the result of

various factors, but especially students’ interests and feelings of self-determination. A student who has a lasting

personal interest in a task or topic and is allowed to choose it freely is especially likely to value the task—and

therefore to feel motivated.

Ideally both expectancies and values are high in students on any key learning task. The reality, however, is that

students sometimes do not expect success, nor do they necessarily value it when success is possible. How can a

130

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

teacher respond to low expectations and low valuing? We have offered a number of suggestions to meet this

challenge throughout this chapter. In brief, raising low expectations depends on adjusting task difficulty so that

success becomes a reasonable prospect: a teacher must make tasks neither too hard nor too easy. Reaching this

general goal depends in turn on thoughtful, appropriate planning—selecting reasonable objectives, adjusting them

on the basis of experience, finding supportive materials, and providing students with help when needed.

Raising the value of academic tasks is equally important, but the general strategies for doing so are different

than for raising expectations. Increasing value requires linking the task to students’ personal interests and prior

knowledge, showing the utility of the task to students’ future goals, and showing that the task is valuable to other

people whom students’ respect. Some of these strategies were discussed earlier in this chapter, but others (e.g.

linking new learning with prior knowledge) are discussed in Chapter 2, which is called “The learning process”.

TARGET: a model for integrating ideas about motivationA model of motivation that integrates many ideas about motivation, including those in this chapter, has been

developed by Carole Ames (1990, 1992). The acronym or abbreviated name for the program is TARGET, which

stands for six elements of effective motivation:

• Task

• Authority

• Recognition

• Grouping

• Evaluating

• Time

Each of the elements contributes to students' motivation either directly or indirectly.

Task

As explained earlier, students experience tasks in terms of their value, their expectation of success, and their

authenticity. The value of a task is assessed by its importance, interest to the student, usefulness or utility, and the

cost in terms of effort and time to achieve it. Expectation of success is assessed by a student's perception of the

difficulty of a task. Generally a middling level of difficulty is optimal for students; too easy, and the task seems

trivial (not valuable or meaningful), and too hard, and the task seems unlikely to succeed and in this sense useless.

Authenticity refers to how much a task relates to real-life experiences of students; the more it does so, the more it

can build on students' interests and goals, and the more meaningful and motivating it becomes.

Autonomy

Motivation is enhanced if students feel a degree of autonomy or responsibility for a learning task. Autonomy

strengthens self-efficacy and self-determination—two valued and motivating attitudes described earlier in this

chapter. Where possible, teachers can enhance autonomy by offering students' choices about assignments and by

encouraging them to take initiative about their own learning.

Recognition

Teachers can support students' motivation by recognizing their achievements appropriately. Much depends,

however, on how this is done; as discussed earlier, praise sometimes undermines performance. It is not especially

effective if praise is very general and lacking in detailed reasons for the praise; or if praise is for qualities which a

Educational Psychology 131 A Global Text

6. Student motivation

student cannot influence (like intelligence instead of effort); or if praise is offered so widely that it loses meaning or

even becomes a signal that performance has been substandard. Many of these paradoxical effects are described by

self-determination and self-efficacy theory (and were explained earlier in this chapter).

Grouping

Motivation is affected by how students are grouped together for their work—a topic discussed in more detail in

Chapter 8 (“Instructional Strategies”). There are many ways to group students, but they tend to fall into three types:

cooperative, competitive, and individualistic (Johnson & Johnson, 1999). In cooperative learning, a set of students

work together to achieve a common goal (for example, producing a group presentation for the class); often they

receive a final grade, or part of a final grade, in common. In competitive learning, students work individually, and

their grades reflect comparisons among the students (for example, their performances are ranked relative to each

other, or they are “graded on a curve”). In individualistic learning, students work by themselves, but their grades

are unrelated to the performance of classmates. Research that compares these three forms of grouping tends to

favor cooperative learning groups, which apparently supports students' need for belonging—an idea important in

self-determination theory discussed earlier in this chapter.

Evaluation

Grouping structures obviously affect how students' efforts are evaluated. A focus on comparing students, as

happens with competitive structures, can distract students from thinking about the material to be learned, and to

focus instead on how they appear to external authorities; the question shifts from “What am I learning?” to “What

will the teacher think about my performance?” A focus on cooperative learning, on the other hand, can have double-

edged effects: students are encouraged to help their group mates, but may also be tempted to rely excessively on

others' efforts or alternatively to ignore each other's contributions and overspecialize their own contributions. Some

compromise between cooperative and individualistic structures seems to create optimal motivation for learning

(Slavin, 1995).

Time

As every teacher knows, students vary in the amount of time needed to learn almost any material or task.

Accommodating the differences can be challenging, but also important for maximizing students' motivation. School

days are often filled with interruptions and fixed intervals of time devoted to non-academic activities—facts that

make it difficult to be flexible about granting individuals different amounts of time to complete academic tasks.

Nonetheless a degree of flexibility is usually possible: larger blocks of time can sometimes be created for important

activities (for example, writing an essay), and sometimes enrichment activities can be arranged for some students

while others receive extra attention from the teacher on core or basic tasks. More about such strategies is discussed

in Chapter 8 (“Instructional Strategies”).

The bottom line about motivation: sustaining focus on learning

Sooner or later when you teach, there will be situations appropriate for each perspective about motivation

described in this chapter. There will be times when focusing exclusively on students’ appropriate behavior (or lack

thereof) will be both necessary and sufficient evidence of motivation. But there will be other times when it is

important to encourage students’ beliefs that they can accomplish specific tasks, and still other times when

132

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

providing for students’ underlying needs for competence or social connection is important. Think of these

perspectives as alternatives to be used either singly or in combination when the time is right.

Because of your own values, attitudes, or beliefs, you may find one perspective more personally compatible than

another. Even if you settle on favorite ways of motivating students, though, we encourage you to keep the other, less

favored approaches in reserve anyway, and to experiment with them. We believe that an eclectic approach to

motivation will enrich your teaching the most, and enrich your students’ motivation and learning as well. If there is

a single lesson from the concepts about motivation outlined in this chapter, it is this: academic motivation has no

single source, and teachers motivate students the best when they assume motivation is complex. The next two

chapters look at ways of realizing such “broad-mindedness” in practice, first when you prepare activities and classes

and later when you actually teach them.

Chapter summary

Motivation—the energy or drive that gives behavior direction and focus—can be understood in a variety of ways,

each of which has implications for teaching. One perspective on motivation comes from behaviorism, and equates

underlying drives or motives with their outward, visible expression in behavior. Most others, however, come from

cognitive theories of learning and development. Motives are affected by the kind of goals set by students—whether

they are oriented to mastery, performance, failure-avoidance, or social contact. They are also affected by students’

interests, both personal and situational. And they are affected by students’ attributions about the causes of success

and failure—whether they perceive the causes are due to ability, effort, task difficulty, or luck.

A major current perspective about motivation is based on self-efficacy theory, which focuses on a person’s belief

that he or she is capable of carrying out or mastering a task. High self-efficacy affects students’ choice of tasks, their

persistence at tasks, and their resilience in the face of failure. It helps to prevent learned helplessness, a perception

of complete lack of control over mastery or success. Teachers can encourage high self-efficacy beliefs by providing

students with experiences of mastery and opportunities to see others’ experiences of mastery, by offering well-

timed messages persuading them of their capacity for success, and by interpreting students’ emotional reactions to

success, failure and stress.

An extension of self-efficacy theory is self-determination theory, which is based on the idea that everyone has

basic needs for autonomy, competence, and relatedness to others. According to the theory, students will be

motivated more intrinsically if these three needs are met as much as possible. A variety of strategies can assist

teachers in doing so. As a practical matter, the strategies can encourage motivation that is more intrinsic to

students, but usually not completely intrinsic.

On the Internet

This is a rather extensive site maintained about all

aspects of self-efficacy theory. The site gives access to a number of published articles on the subject as well as to

extensive “lecture” notes by Frank Pajares, who publishes and teaches about self-efficacy theory.

This, too, is a rather extensive site, maintained at

the University of Rochester by Edward Deci and Richard Ryan, two psychologists who have published extensively

about self-determination theory. The site is especially thorough in reviewing evidence contrary to the theory and in

offering many of the actual research questionnaires which have been used to study self-determination.

Educational Psychology 133 A Global Text

6. Student motivation

Here is a website that discusses many aspects of

motivation in education. It is not limited to any one theory, perspective, or concept about this topic. Many of the

references are to citations from the ERIC database (also available at ), and there are links to

bibliographies on additional topics about education.

Key termsAlbert Bandura

Attributions of success or failureAutonomy, need for

Behaviorist perspective on motivationCompetence, need for

Failure-avoidant goalsIntrinsic motivation

Jigsaw classroomLearned helplessness

Mastery goals

MotivationNeed for relatedness

Performance goalsPersonal interests

Self-determination theorySelf-efficacy

Situational interestsTARGET

References

Allison, K., Dwyer, J., & Makin, S. (1999). Self-efficacy and participation in vigorous physical activity by high

school students. Health Education and Behavior, 26(1), 12-24.

Ames, C. (1990). Motivation: What teachers need to know. Teachers College Record, 91, 409-421.

Ames. C. (1992). Classrooms: Goals, structures, and student motivation. Journal of Educational Psychology,

84, 261-271.

Aronson, E. & Patnoe, S. (1997). The Jigsaw classroom: Building cooperation in the classroom, 2nd edition.

New York: Longman.

Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84,

191-215.

Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs,

NJ: Prentice Hall.

Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: Freeman.

Blackwell, L., Trzniewski, K., & Dweck, C. (2007). Implicit theories predict achievement across an adolescent

transition: a longitudinal study. Child Development, 78, 246-263.

Bong, M. & Skaalvik, E. (2004). Academic self-concept and self-efficacy: How different are they really?

Educational psychology review, 15(1), 1-40.

Burke, M. & Sass, T. (2006). Classroom peer effects and student achievement. Paper presented at the annual

meeting of the American Economic Association, Boston, USA.

Butin, D. (2005). Service learning in higher education. New York: Palgrave Macmillan.

Cameron, J. & Pierce, W. (1994). Reinforcement, reward, and intrinsic motivation: A meta-analysis. Review

of Educational Research, 64, 363-423.

134

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Cohen, E. (1994). Designing groupwork: Strategies for the heterogeneous classroom, 2nd edition. New York:

Teachers’ College Press.

Darnon, C., Butera, F., & Harackiewicz, J. (2006). Achievement goals in social interactions: Learning with

mastery versus performance goals. Motivation and Emotion, 31, 61-70.

Deci, E. (1971). Effects of externally mediated rewards on intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality and

Social Psychology, 18, 105-115.

Deci, E., Koestner, R., & Ryan, R. (2001). Extrinsic rewards and intrinsic motivation in education:

Reconsidered once again. Review of Educational Research, 71(1), 1-27.

Deci, E. & Ryan, R. (2003). The paradox of achievement: The harder you push, the worse it gets. In E.

Aronson (Ed.), Improving academic achievement: Impact of psychological factors in education (pp. 62-

90). Boston: Academic Press.

DeGranpre, R. (2000). A science of meaning: Can behaviorism bring meaning to psychological science?

American Psychologist, 55(7), 721-736.

Dowson, M. & McInerney, D. (2003). What do students say about their motivational goals? Toward a more

complex and dynamic perspective on student motivation. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 28, 91-

113.

Dweck, C. (2000). Self-theories: Their role in motivation, personality, and development. Philadelphia:

Psychology Press.

Dweck, C. (2006). Mindset: The new psychology of success. New York: Random House.

Eccles, J., Wigfield, A., & Schiefele, U. (1998). Motivation to succeed. In W. Damon & N. Eisenberg (Eds.),

Handbook of child psychology, Volume 3: Social, emotional, and personality development, 5th edition

(pp. 1017-1095). New York: Wiley.

Eisenberger, R. & Cameron, J. (1996). Detrimental effects of reward: Reality or myth? American

Psychologist, 51, 1153-1166.

Elliott, A., McGregor, H., & Thrash, T. (2004). The need for competence. In E. Deci & R. Ryan (Eds.),

Handbook of self-determination research (pp. 361-388). Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press.

Erikson, E. (1968/1994). Identity, youth, and crisis. New York: Norton.

Flowerday, T., Shraw, G., & Stevens, J. (2004). Role of choice and interest in reader engagement. Journal of

Educational Research, 97, 93-103.

Garner, R., Brown, R., Sanders, S. & Menke, D. (1992). “Seductive details” and learning from text. In A.

Renninger, S. Hidi, & A. Krapp (Eds.), The role of interest in learning and development, pp. 239-254.

Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Goddard, R., Hoy, W., & Hoy, A. (2004). Collective efficacy beliefs: Theoretical developments, empirical

evidence, and future directions. Educational Researcher, 33(3), 3-13.

Educational Psychology 135 A Global Text

6. Student motivation

Guthrie, J., Wigfield, A., & Humenick, N. (2006). Influences of stimulating tasks on reading motivation and

comprehension. Journal of Educational Research, 99, 232-245.

Harp, S. & Mayer, R. (1998). How seductive details do their damage. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90,

414-434.

Harzckiewicz, J., Barron, K., Tauer, J., & Elliot, A. (2002). Short-term and long-term consequences of

achievement goals. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 316-320.

Hidi, S. & Renninger, A. (2006). A four-phase model of interest development. Educational Psychology, 41,

111-127.

Johnson, D. & Johnson, R. (1999). Learning together and alone: Cooperative, competitive, and

individualistic learning, 5th edition. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.

Kohn, A. (1996). No contest: The case against competition. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

Koestner, R. & Losier, G. (2004). Distinguishing three ways of being highly motivated: a closer look at

introjection, identification, and intrinsic motivation. In E. Deci & R. Ryan (Eds.), Handbook of self-

determination research (pp. 101-122). Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press.

Lent, R., Brown, S., Nota, L., & Soresi, S. (2003). Teaching social cognitive interest and choice hypotheses

across Holland types in Italian high school students. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 62, 101-118.

Lindley, L. (2006). The paradox of self-efficacy: Research with diverse populations. Journal of Career

Assessment, 14(1), 143-160.

Mau, W.-C. (2003). Factors that influence persistence in science and engineering career aspirations. Career

Development Quarterly, 51, 234-243.

Midgley, C., Kaplan, A., & Middleton, M. (2001). Performance-approach goals: Good for what, for whom, and

under what conditions, and at what cost? Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 77-86.

Pajares, F. & Schunk, D. (2001). Self-beliefs and school success: Self-efficacy, self-concept, and school

achievement. In . Riding & S. Rayner (Eds.), Perception (pp. 239-266). London: Ablex Publishing.

Pajares, F. & Schunk, D. (2002). Self-beliefs in psychology and education: An historical perspective. In J.

Aronson (Ed.), Improving academic achievement (pp. 3-21). New York: Academic Press.

Reynolds, P. & Symons, S. (2001). Motivational variables and children’s text search. Journal of Educational

Psychology, 93, 14-22.

Ryan, R. & Lynch, M. (2003). Philosophies of motivation and classroom management. In R. Curren (Ed.),

Blackwell companion to philosophy: A companion to the philosophy of education (pp. 260-271). New

York, NY: Blackwell.

Sanchez, C. & Wiley, J. (2006). An examination of the seductive details effect in terms of working memory

capacity. Memory and Cognition, 34, 344-355.

136

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Sapon-Shevin, M. & Cohen, E. (2004). Conclusion. In Cohen, E., Brody, C., & Sapon-Shevin, M. (Eds.),

Teaching cooperative learning: The challenge for teacher education (pp. 217-224). Albany, NY: State

University of New York Press.

Schunk, D. & Zimmerman, B. (1997). Social origins of self-regulatory competence. Educational psychologist,

34(4), 195-208.

Schwartz, B. (2004). The paradox of choice: Why more is less. New York: Ecco/Harper Collins.

Schunk, D., Pintrich, P., Meese, J. (2008). Motivation in education: Theory, research and applications. New

York: Pearson Professional.

Skinner, B. F. (1938). The behavior of organisms. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

Slavin, R. (1995). Cooperative learning, 2nd edition. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.

Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

Urdan, T. (2004). Predictors of self-handicapping and achievement: Examining achievement goals,

classroom goal structures, and culture. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 251-254.

Weiner, B. (2005). Motivation from an attribution perspective and the social psychology of perceived

competence. In A. Elliot & C. Dweck (Eds.), Handbook of Competence and Motivation, pp. 73-84. New

York: Guilford Press.

Wigfield, A. & Eccles, J. (2002). The development of achievement motivation. San Diego, CA: Academic

Press.

Wigfield, A., Tonk, S., & Eccles, J. (2004). Expectancy-value theory in cross-cultural perspective. In D.

McInerney & S. van Etten (Eds.), Research on Sociocultural Influences on Motivation and Learning.

Greenwich, CT: Information Age Publishers.

Wolters, C. (2004). Advancing achievement goal theory: Using goal structures and goal orientations to

predict students’ motivation, cognition, and achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 236-

250.

Educational Psychology 137 A Global Text

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

7. Classroom management and the learning environment

This is an excerpt from a professional journal kept by one of us (Kelvin Seifert) when he was teaching

kindergarten:

20xx-11-14: Today my student Carol sat in the circle, watching others while we all played Duck,

Duck, Goose (in this game, one student is outside the circle, tags another student who then chases the

first person around the circle). Carol’s turn had already passed. Apparently she was bored, because

she flopped on her back, smiling broadly, rolling around luxuriously on the floor in the path of the

other runners. Several classmates noticed her, smiled or giggled, began flopping down as well. One

chaser tripped over a “flopper”.

“Sit up, Carol”, said I, the ever-vigilant teacher. “You’re in the way.” But no result. I repeated this

twice, firmly; then moved to pick her up.

Instantly Carol ran to the far side of the gym, still smiling broadly. Then her best friend ran off with

her. Now a whole new game was launched, or really two games: “Run-from-the-teacher” and

“Enjoy-being-watched-by-everybody”. A lot more exciting, unfortunately, than Duck, Duck, Goose!

An excerpt from Kelvin’s same journal several years later, when he was teaching math in high school:

20xx-3-4: The same four students sat in the back again today, as usual. They seem to look in every

direction except at me, even when I’m explaining material that they need to know. The way they

smile and whisper to each other, it seems almost like they are “in love” with each other, though I can’t

be sure who loves whom the most.

Others—students not part of the foursome—seem to react variously. Some seem annoyed, turn the

other way, avoid talking with the group, and so on. But others seem almost envious—as if they want

to be part of the “in” group, too, and were impressed with the foursome’s ability to get away with

being inattentive and almost rude. Either way, I think a lot of other students are being distracted.

Twice during the period today, I happened to notice members of the group passing a note, and then

giggling and looking at me. By the end, I had had enough of this sort of thing, so I kept them in

briefly after class and asked one of them to read the note. They looked a bit embarrassed and

hesitant, but eventually one of them opened the note and read it out loud. “Choose one”, it said. “Mr

Seifert looks (1) old ____, (2) stupid____, or (3) clueless____.”

Kelvin's experiences in managing these very different classrooms taught him what every teacher knows or else

quickly learns: management matters a lot. But his experiences also taught him that management is about more

than correcting the misbehaviors of individuals, more than just discipline. Classroom management is also

Educational Psychology 138 A Global Text

7. Classroom management and the learning environment

about orchestrating or coordinating entire sets or sequences of learning activities so that everyone, misbehaving or

not, learns as easily and productively as possible. Educators sometimes therefore describe good management as the

creation of a positive learning environment, because the term calls attention to the totality of activities and

people in a classroom, as well as to their goals and expectations about learning (Jones & Jones, 2007). When one of

us (Kelvin) was teaching, he used both terms almost interchangeably, though in speaking of management he more

often was referring to individual students’ behavior and learning, and in speaking of the learning environment he

more often meant the overall “feel” of the class as a whole.

Why classroom management matters

Managing the learning environment is both a major responsibility and an on-going concern for all teachers, even

those with years of experience (Good & Brophy, 2002). There are several reasons. In the first place, a lot goes on in

classrooms simultaneously, even when students seem to be doing only one task in common. Twenty-five students

may all seem to be working on a sheet of math problems. But look more closely: several may be stuck on a

particular problem, each for different reasons. A few others have worked only the first problem or two and are now

chatting quietly with each other instead of continuing. Still others have finished and are wondering what to do next.

At any one moment each student needs something different—different information, different hints, different kinds

of encouragement. Such diversity increases even more if the teacher deliberately assigns multiple activities to

different groups or individuals (for example, if some students do a reading assignment while others do the math

problems).

Another reason that managing the environment is challenging is because a teacher can not predict everything

that will happen in a class. A well-planned lesson may fall flat on its face, or take less time than expected, and you

find yourself improvising to fill class time. On the other hand an unplanned moment may become a wonderful,

sustained exchange among students, and prompt you to drop previous plans and follow the flow of discussion.

Interruptions happen continually: a fire drill, a drop-in visit from another teacher or the principal, a call on the

intercom from the office. An activity may indeed turn out well, but also rather differently than you intended; you

therefore have to decide how, if at all, to adjust the next day's lesson to allow for this surprise.

A third reason for the importance of management is that students form opinions and perceptions about your

teaching that are inconsistent with your own. What you intend as encouragement for a shy student may seem to the

student herself like “forced participation”. An eager, outgoing classmate watching your effort to encourage the shy

student, moreover, may not see you as either encouraging or coercing, but as overlooking or ignoring other students

who already want to participate. The variety of perceptions can lead to surprises in students’ responses—most often

small ones, but occasionally major.

At the broadest, society-wide level, classroom management challenges teachers because public schooling is not

voluntary, and students’ presence in a classroom is therefore not a sign, in and of itself, that they wish to learn.

Instead, students’ presence is just a sign that an opportunity exists for teachers to motivate students to learn. Some

students, of course, do enjoy learning and being in school, almost regardless of what teachers do! Others do enjoy

school, but only because teachers have worked hard to make classroom life pleasant and interesting. Those students

become motivated because you have successfully created a positive learning environment and have sustained it

through skillful management.

139

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Fortunately it is possible to earn this sort of commitment from many students, and this chapter describes ways

of doing so. We begin with ways of preventing management problems from happening by increasing students’ focus

on learning. The methods include ideas about arranging classroom space, about establishing procedures, routines,

and rules, and about communicating the importance of learning to students and parents. After these prevention

oriented discussions, we look at ways of refocusing students when and if their minds or actions stray from the tasks

at hand. As you probably know from being a student, bringing students back on task can happen in many ways, and

the ways vary widely in the energy and persistence required of the teacher. We try to indicate some of these

variations, but because of space limitations and because of the richness of classroom life, we cannot describe them

all.

Preventing management problems by focusing students on learning

The easiest management problems to solve are ones that do not happen in the first place! Even before the school

year begins, you can minimize behavior problems by arranging classroom furniture and materials in ways that

encourage a focus on learning as much as possible. Later, once school begins, you can establish procedures and

rules that support a focus on learning even more.

Arranging classroom space

Viewed broadly, classrooms may seem to be arranged in similar ways, but there are actually important

alternative arrangements to consider. Variations exist because of grade level, the subjects taught, the teacher’s

philosophy of education, and of course the size of the room and the furniture available. Whatever the arrangement

that you choose, it should help students to focus on learning tasks as much as possible and minimize the chances of

distractions. Beyond these basic principles, however, the “best” arrangement depends on what your students need

and on the kind of teaching that you prefer and feel able to provide (Boyner, 2003; Nations & Boyett, 2002). The

next sections describe some of the options. In considering them (and before moving too much furniture around

your room!), you might want to try experimenting with spatial arrangements “virtually” by using one of the

computer programs available on the Internet (see: http://teacher.scholastic.com/tools/class_setup/).

Displays and wall space

All classrooms have walls, of course, and how you fill them can affect the mood or feeling of a classroom. Ample

displays make a room interesting and can be used to reinforce curriculum goals and display (and hence publicly

recognize) students’ work. But too many displays can also make a room seem “busy” or distracting as well as

physically smaller. They can also be more work to maintain. If you are starting a new school year, then, a good

strategy is to decorate some of the wall or bulletin board space, but not to fill it all immediately. Leaving some space

open leaves flexibility to respond to ideas and curriculum needs that emerge after the year is underway. The same

advice applies especially for displays that are high maintenance, such as aquariums, pets, and plants. These can

serve wonderfully as learning aids, but do not have to be in place on the first day of school. Not only the students,

but also you yourself, may already have enough to cope with at that time.

Computers in the classroom

If you are like the majority of teachers, you will have only one computer in your room, or at most just a few, and

their placement may be pre-determined by the location of power and cable outlets. If so, you need to think about

computer placement early in the process of setting up a room. Once the location of computers is set, locations for

Educational Psychology 140 A Global Text

7. Classroom management and the learning environment

desks, high-usage shelves, and other moveable items can be chosen more sensibly—in general, as already

mentioned, so as to minimize distractions to students and to avoid unnecessary traffic congestion.

Visibility of and interactions with students

Learning is facilitated if the furniture and space allow you to see all students and to interact with them from a

comfortable distance. Usually this means that the main, central part of the room—where desks and tables are

usually located—needs to be as open and as spacious as possible. While this idea may seem obvious, enacting it can

be challenging in practice if the room itself is small or shaped unusually. In classrooms with young students

(kindergarten), furthermore, open spaces tend to allow, if not invite, physical movement of children—a feature that

you may consider either constructive or annoying, depending on your educational goals and the actual level of

activity that occurs.

Spatial arrangements unique to grade levels or subjects

The best room arrangement sometimes depends on the grade level or subject area of the class. If you teach in

elementary school, for example, you may need to think especially about where students can keep their daily

belongings, such as coats and lunches. In some schools, these can be kept outside the classroom—but not

necessarily. Some subjects and grade levels, furthermore, lend themselves especially well to small group

interaction, in which case you might prefer not to seat students in rows, but instead around small-group tables or

work areas. The latter arrangement is sometimes preferred by elementary teachers, but is also useful in high

schools wherever students need lots of counter space, as in some shops or art courses, or where they need to

interact, as in English as a Second Language courses (McCafferty, Jacobs, & Iddings, 2006). The key issue in

deciding between tables and rows, however, is not grade level or subject as such, but the amount of small group

interaction you want to encourage, compared to the amount of whole-group instruction. As a rule, tables make

working with peers easier, and rows make listening to the teacher more likely and group work slightly more

awkward physically.

Ironically, some teachers also experience challenges about room arrangement because they do not actually have

a classroom of their own, because they must move each day among other teachers’ rooms. “Floating” is especially

likely for specialized teachers (e.g. music teachers in elementary schools, who move from class to class) and in

schools have an overall shortage of classrooms. Floating can sometimes be annoying to the teacher, though it

actually also has advantages, such as not having to take responsibility for how other teachers’ rooms are arranged.

If you find yourself floating, it helps to consider a few key strategies, such as:

• consider using a permanent cart to move crucial supplies from room to room

• make sure that every one of your rooms has an overhead projector (do not count on using chalkboards or

computers in other teachers’ rooms)

• talk to the other teachers about having at least one shelf or corner in each room designated for your

exclusive use

Establishing daily procedures and routines

Procedures or routines are specific ways of doing common, repeated classroom tasks or activities. Examples

include checking daily attendance, dealing with students who arrive late, or granting permission to leave the

141

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

classroom for an errand. Academically related procedures include ways of turning in daily homework (e.g. putting it

on a designated shelf at a particular time), of gaining the teacher’s attention during quiet seat work (e.g. raising

your hand and waiting), and of starting a “free choice” activity after completing a classroom assignment.

Procedures serve the largely practical purpose of making activities and tasks flow smoothly—a valuable and

necessary purpose in classrooms, where the actions of many people have to be coordinated within limited time and

space. As such, procedures are more like social conventions than like moral expectations. They are only indirectly

about what is ethically right or ethically desirable to do (Turiel, 2006). Most procedures or routines can be

accomplished in more than one way, with only minor differences in outcomes. There is more than one way, for

example, for the procedure of taking attendance: the teacher could call the role, delegate a student to call the role,

or note students’ presence on a seating chart. Each variation accomplishes essentially the same task, and the choice

may be less important than the fact that the class coordinates its actions somehow, by committing to some sort of

choice.

For teachers, of course, an initial management task is to establish procedures and routines as promptly as

possible. Because of the conventional quality of procedures, some teachers find that it works well simply to

announce and explain key procedures without inviting much discussion from students (“Here is how we will choose

partners for the group work”). Other teachers prefer to invite input from students when creating procedures (asking

the class, “What do you feel is the best way for students to get my attention during a quiet reading time?”). Both

approaches have advantages as well as disadvantages. Simply announcing key procedures saves time and insures

consistency in case you teach more than one class (as you would in high school). But it puts more responsibility on

the teacher to choose procedures that are truly reasonable and practical. Inviting students’ input, on the other

hand, can help students to become aware of and committed to procedures, but at the cost of requiring more time to

settle on them. It also risks creating confusion if you teach multiple classes, each of which adopts different

procedures. Whatever approach you choose, of course, they have to take into account any procedures or rules

imposed by the school or school district as a whole. A school may have a uniform policy about how to record daily

attendance, for example, and that policy may determine, either partly or completely, how you take attendance with

your particular students.

Establishing classroom rules

Unlike procedures or routines, rules express standards of behavior for which individual students need to take

responsibility. Although they are like procedures in that they sometimes help in insuring the efficiency of classroom

tasks, they are really about encouraging students to be responsible for learning and showing respect for each other.

Exhibit 8 lists a typical set of classroom rules.

Educational Psychology 142 A Global Text

7. Classroom management and the learning environment

Exhibit 8: Sample set of classroom rules

Note three things about the examples in Exhibit 8. One is that the rules are not numerous; the table lists only

five. Most educational experts recommend keeping the number of rules to a minimum in order to make them easier

to remember (Thorson, 2003; Brophy, 2004). A second feature is that they are stated in positive terms (“Do X…”)

rather than negative terms (“Do not do Y…”), a strategy that emphasizes and clarifies what students should do

rather than what they should avoid. A third feature is that each rule actually covers a collection of more specific

behaviors. The rule “Bring all materials to class”, for example, covers bringing pencils, paper, textbooks, homework

papers, and permission slips—depending on the situation. As a result of their generality, rules often have a degree

of ambiguity that sometimes requires interpretation. Infractions may occur that are marginal or “in a grey area”,

rather than clear cut. A student may bring a pen, for example, but the pen may not work properly. You may

therefore wonder whether this incident is really a failure to follow the rule, or just an unfortunate (and in this case

minor) fault of the pen manufacturer.

As with classroom procedures, rules can be planned either by the teacher alone, or by the teacher with advice

from students. The arguments for each approach are similar to the arguments for procedures: rules “laid on” by the

teacher may be more efficient and consistent, and in this sense more fair, but rules influenced by the students may

be supported more fully by the students. Because rules focus strongly on personal responsibility, however, there is a

stronger case for involving students in making them than in making classroom procedures (Brookfield, 2006;

Kohn, 2006). In any case the question of who plans classroom rules is not necessarily an either/or choice. It is

possible in principle to impose certain rules on students (for example, “Always be polite to each other”) but let the

students determine the consequences for violations of certain rules (for example, “If a student is discourteous to a

classmate, he/she must apologize to the student in writing”). Some mixture of influences is probably inevitable, in

fact, if only because the class needs to take into account your own moral commitments as the teacher as well as any

imposed by the school (like “No smoking in the school” or “Always walk in the hallways”).

Pacing and structuring lessons and activities

One of the best ways to prevent management problems is by pacing and structuring lessons or activities as

smoothly and continuously as possible. This goal depends on three major strategies:

• selecting tasks or activities at an appropriate level of difficulty for your students

• providing a moderate level of structure or clarity to students about what they are supposed to do, especially

during transitions between activities

143

• Treat others with courtesy and politeness.

• Make sure to bring required materials to class and to activities.

• Be on time for class and other activities.

• Listen to the teacher and to others when they are speaking.

• Follow all school rules.

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

• keeping alert to the flow and interplay of behaviors for the class as a whole and for individuals within it.

Each strategy presents special challenges to teachers, but also opportunities for helping students to learn.

Choosing tasks at an appropriate level of difficulty

As experienced teachers know and as research has confirmed, students are most likely to engage with learning

when tasks are of moderate difficulty, neither too easy nor too hard and therefore neither boring nor frustrating

(Britt, 2005). Finding the right level of difficulty, however, can be a challenge if you have little experience teaching a

particular grade level or curriculum, or even if students are simply new to you and their abilities unknown. Whether

familiar or not, members of any class are likely to have diverse skills and readiness–a fact that makes it challenging

to determine what level of difficulty is appropriate. A common strategy for dealing with these challenges is to begin

units, lessons, or projects with tasks that are relatively easy and familiar. Then, introduce more difficult material or

tasks gradually until students seem challenged, but not overwhelmed. Following this strategy gives the teacher a

chance to observe and diagnose students’ learning needs before adjusting content, and it gives students a chance to

orient themselves to the teacher’s expectations, teaching style, and topic of study without becoming frustrated

prematurely. Later in a unit, lesson, or project, students seem better able to deal with more difficult tasks or content

(Van Merrionboer, 2003). The principle seems to help as well with “authentic” learning tasks—ones that resemble

real-world activities, such as learning to drive an automobile or to cook a meal, and that present a variety of

complex tasks simultaneously. Even in those cases it helps to isolate and focus on the simplest subtasks first (such

as “put the key in the ignition”) and move to harder tasks only later (such as parallel parking).

Sequencing instruction is only a partial solution to finding the best “level” of difficulty, however, because it does

not deal with enduring individual differences among students. The fundamental challenge to teachers is to

individualize or differentiate instruction fully: to tailor it not only to the class as a group, but to the lasting

differences among members of the class. One way to approach this sort of diversity, obviously, is to plan different

content or activities for different students or groups of students. While one group works on Task A, another group

works on Task B; one group works on relatively easy math problems, for example, while another works on harder

ones. Differentiating instruction in this way complicates a teacher’s job, but it can be done, and has in fact been

done by many teachers (it also makes teaching more interesting!). In the next chapter, we describe some classroom

management strategies that help with such multi-tasking.

Providing moderate amounts of structure and detail

Chances are that at some point in your educational career you have wished that a teacher would clarify or

explain an assignment more fully, and perhaps give it a clearer structure or organization. Students’ desire for clarity

is especially common with assignments that are by nature open-ended, such as long essays, large projects, or

creative works. Simply being told to “write an essay critiquing the novel”, for example, leaves more room for

uncertainty (and worry) than being given guidelines about what questions the essay should address, what topics or

parts it should have, and what its length or style should be (Chesebro, 2003). As you might suspect, some students

desire clarity more than others, and improve their performance especially much when provided with plenty of

structure and clarity. Students with certain kinds of learning difficulties, in particular, often learn effectively and

stay on task only if provided with somewhat explicit, detailed instructions about the tasks expected of them (Marks,

et al., 2003).

Educational Psychology 144 A Global Text

7. Classroom management and the learning environment

As a teacher, the challenge is to accommodate students’ need for clarity without making guidance so specific or

detailed that students do little thinking for themselves. As a (ridiculously extreme) example, consider a teacher

gives “clear” instructions for an essay by announcing not only exactly which articles to read and cite in the essay and

which topics or issues to cover, but even requires specific wording of sentences in their essays. This much specificity

may reduce students’ uncertainties and make the teacher’s task of evaluating the essays relatively straightforward

and easy. But it also reduces or even eliminates the educational value of the assignment—assuming, of course, that

its purpose is to get students to think for themselves.

Ideally, then, structure should be moderate rather than extreme. There should be just enough to give students

some sense of direction and to stimulate more accomplishment than if they worked with less structure or guidance.

This ideal is an application of Vygotsky’s idea of the zone of proximal development that we discussed in Chapter 2: a

place (figuratively speaking) where students get more done with help than without it. The ideal amount of guidance

—the “location” of the zone of proximal development—varies with the assignment and the student, and it

(hopefully) decreases over time for all students. One student may need more guidance to do his or her best in math,

but less guidance in order to write her or his best essay. Another student may need the reverse. But if all goes well,

both students may need less at the end of the year than at the beginning.

Managing transitions

Transitions between activities is often full of distractions and “lost” time, and is a time when inappropriate

behaviors are especially likely to occur. Part of the problem is intrinsic to transitions: students may have to wait

before a new activity actually begins, and therefore get bored at the very moment when the teacher is preoccupied

with arranging materials for the new activity. From the point of view of the students, transitions may seem

essentially like unsupervised group time, when seemingly any behavior is tolerated.

Minimizing such problems requires two strategies, one of which is easier to implement than the other. The

easier strategy is for you, as teacher, to organize materials as well as possible ahead of time, so that you minimize

the time needed to begin a new activity. The advice sounds simple, and mostly is, but it sometimes takes a bit of

practice to implement smoothly. When one of us (Kelvin) first began teaching university, for example, particular

papers or overhead transparencies sometimes got lost in the wrong folder in spite of Kelvin's efforts to keep them

where they were easy to find. The resulting delays about finding them slowed the pace of class and caused

frustrations.

A second, more complex strategy is to teach students ways to manage their own behavior during transitions

(Marzano & Marzano, 2004). If students talk too loudly at these times, for example, then discuss with them what

constitutes appropriate levels or amounts of talk, and discuss the need for them to monitor their own sound level.

Or if students stop work early in anticipation of ending an activity, then talk about—or even practice—waiting for a

signal from yourself to indicate the true ending point for an activity. If certain students continue working beyond

the end of an activity. On the other hand, try giving them warning of the impending end in advance, and remind

them about to take responsibility for actually finishing work once they hear the advance warning, and so on. The

point of these tactics is to encourage responsibility for behavior during transitions, and thereby reduce your own

need to monitor students at that crucial time.

145

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

None of these ideas, of course, mean that you, as teacher, should give up monitoring students’ behavior entirely.

Chances are that you still will need to notice if and when someone talks too loudly, finishes too early, or continues

too long, and you will still need to give some students appropriate reminders. But the amount of reminding will be

less to the extent that students can remind and monitor themselves—a welcome trend at any time, but especially

during transitions.

Maintaining the flow of activities

A lot of classroom management is really about keeping activities flowing smoothly, both during individual

lessons and across the school day. The trouble is that there is never just “one” event happening at a time, even if

only one activity has been formally planned and is supposed to be occurring. Imagine, for example, that everyone is

supposed to be attending a single whole-class discussion on a topic; yet individual students will be having different

experiences at any one moment. Several students may be listening and contributing comments, for example, but a

few others may be planning what they want to say next and ignoring the current speakers, still others may be

ruminating about what a previous speaker said, and still others may be thinking about unrelated matters–the

restroom, food, or sex. Things get even more complicated if the teacher deliberately plans multiple activities: in that

case some students may interact with the teacher, for example, while others do work in an unsupervised group or

work independently in a different part of the room. How is a teacher to keep activities flowing smoothly in the face

of such variety?

A common mistake of beginning teachers in multi-faceted settings like these is to pay too much attention to any

one activity, student, or small group, at the expense of noticing and responding to all the others. If you are helping a

student on one side of the room when someone on the other side disturbs classmates with off-task conversation, it

can be less effective either to finish with the student you are helping before attending to the disruption, or to

interrupt yourself to solve the disruption on the other side of the room. Although one of these responses may be

necessary, either one involves disruption somewhere. There is a risk that either the student’s chatting may spread

to others, or the interrupted student may become bored with waiting for the teacher’s attention and wander off-task

herself.

A better solution, though one that at first may seem challenging, is to attend to both events at once—a strategy

that was named withitness in a series of now-classic research studies several decades ago (Kounin, 1970).

Withitness does not mean that you focus on all simultaneous activities with equal care, but only that you remain

aware of multiple activities, behaviors, and events to some degree. At a particular moment, for example, you may be

focusing on helping a student, but in some corner of your mind you also notice when chatting begins on the other

side of the room. You have, as the saying goes, “eyes in the back of your head”. Research has found that experienced

teachers are much more likely to show withitness than inexperienced teachers, and that these qualities are

associated with managing classrooms successfully (Emmer & Stough, 2001).

Simultaneous awareness—withitness—makes possible responses to the multiple events that are immediate and

nearly simultaneous—what educators sometimes called overlapping. The teacher’s responses to each event or

behavior need not take equal time, nor even be equally noticeable to all students. If you are helping one student

with seat work at the precise moment when another student begins chatting off-task, for example, a quick glance to

the second student may be enough to bring the second one back to the work at hand, and may scarcely interrupt

Educational Psychology 146 A Global Text

7. Classroom management and the learning environment

your conversation with the first student, or be noticed by others who are not even involved. The result is a smoother

flow to activities overall.

As a new teacher, you may find that withitness and overlapping develop more easily in some situations than in

others. It may be easier to keep an eye (or ear) on multiple activities during familiar routines, such as taking

attendance, but harder to do the same during activities that are unfamiliar or complex, such as introducing a new

topic or unit that you have never taught before. But skill at broadening your attention does increase with time and

practice. It helps to keep trying. Merely demonstrating to students that you are “withit”, in fact, even without

making deliberate overlapping responses, can sometimes deter students from off-task behavior. Someone who is

tempted to pass notes in class, for example, might not do so because she believes that you will probably notice her

doing it anyway, whether or not you are able to notice in fact.

Communicating the importance of learning and of positive behavior

Altogether, the factors we have discussed—arranging space, procedures, and rules, and developing withitness—

help communicate an important message: that in the classroom learning and positive social behavior are priorities.

In addition, teachers can convey this message by offering timely feedback to students about performance, by

keeping accurate records of the performance, and by deliberately communicating with parents or caregivers about

their children and about class activities.

Communicating effectively is so important for all aspects of teaching, in fact, that we discuss it more fully later in

this book (see Chapter 8,“The nature of classroom communication”). Here we focus on only one of its important

aspects: how communication contributes to a smoothly functioning classroom and in this way helps prevent

behavior problems.

Giving timely feedback

The term feedback, when used by educators, refers to responses to students about their behavior or

performance. Feedback is essential if students are to learn and if they are to develop classroom behavior that is

socially skilled and “mature”. But feedback can only be fully effective if offered as soon as possible, when it is still

relevant to the task or activity at hand (Reynolds, 1992). A score on a test is more informative immediately after a

test than after a six-month delay, when students may have forgotten much of the content of the test. A teacher’s

comment to a student about an inappropriate, off-task behavior may not be especially welcome at the moment the

behavior occurs, but it can be more influential and informative then; later, both teacher and student will have

trouble remembering the details of the off-task behavior, and in this sense may literally “not know what they are

talking about”. The same is true for comments about a positive behavior by a student: hearing a compliment right

away makes it easier to the comment with the behavior, and allows the compliment to influence the student more

strongly. There are of course practical limits to how fast feedback can be given, but the general principle is clear:

feedback tends to work better when it is timely.

The principle of timely feedback is consistent, incidentally, with a central principle of operant conditioning

discussed in Chapter 2: reinforcement works best when it follows a to-be-learned operant behavior closely (Skinner,

1957). In this case a teacher’s feedback serves as a form of reinforcement. The analogy is easiest to understand when

the feedback takes the form of praise; in operant conditioning terms, the reinforcing praise then functions like a

“reward”. When feedback is negative, it functions as an “aversive stimulus” (in operant terms), shutting down the

147

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

behavior criticized. At other times, though, criticism can also function as an unintended reinforcement. This

happens, for example, if a student experiences criticism as a reduction in isolation and therefore as in increase in

his importance in the class—a relatively desirable change. So the inappropriate behavior continues, or even

increases, contrary to the teacher's intentions. Exhibit 9 diagrams this sequence of events.

Example of Unintended Negative Reinforcement in the Classroom:

Student is isolated socially → Student publiclymisbehaves → Student gains others'

attention

Reinforcement can happen in class if an undesirable behavior, leads to a less aversive state for a student.

Social isolation can be reduced by public misbehavior, which stimulates attention that is reinforcing.

Ironically, the effort to end misbehavior ends up stimulating the misbehavior.

Exhibit 9: Attracting attention as negative reinforcement

Maintaining accurate records

Although timeliness in responding to students can sometimes happen naturally during class, there are also

situations where promptness depends on having organized key information ahead of time. Obvious examples are

the scores, marks, and grades returned to students for their work. A short quiz (such as a weekly spelling test) may

be possible to return quite soon after the quiz—sometimes you or even the students themselves can mark it during

class. More often, though, assignments and tests require longer processing times: you have to read, score, or add

comments to each paper individually. Excessive time to evaluate students' work can reduce the usefulness of a

teacher’s evaluations to students when she finally does return the work (Black, et al., 2004). During the days or

weeks waiting for a test or assignment to be returned, students are left without information about the quality or

nature of their performance; at the extreme they may even have to complete another test or do another assignment

before getting information about an earlier one. (Perhaps you yourself have experienced this particular problem!)

Delays in providing feedback about academic performance can never be eliminated entirely, but they can be

reduced by keeping accurate, well-organized records of students’ work. A number of computer programs are

available to help with this challenge; if your school does not already have one in use, then there are several

downloadable either free or at low cost from the Internet (e.g.

). Describing these is beyond

the scope of this book. For now we simply emphasize that grading systems benefit students’ learning the most when

they provide feedback as quickly and frequently as possible (McMillan, 2001), precisely the reason why accurate,

well-organized record-keeping is important to keep.

Accurate records are helpful not only for scores on tests, quizzes, or assignments, but also for developing

descriptive summaries of the nature of students’ academic skills or progress. A common way to develop a

description is the student portfolio, which is a compilation of the student’s work and on-going assessments of it

created by the teacher or in some cases by the student (Moritz & Christie, 2005; White, 2005). To know how a

student’s science project evolved from its beginning, for example, a teacher and student can keep a portfolio of lab

notes, logs, preliminary data, and the like. To know how a student’s writing skills developed, they could keep a

Educational Psychology 148 A Global Text

7. Classroom management and the learning environment

portfolio of early drafts on various writing assignments. As the work accumulates, the student can discuss it with

the teacher, and write brief reflections on its strengths thus far or on the steps needed to improve the work further.

By providing a way to respond to work as it evolves, and by including students in making the assessments,

portfolios provide relatively prompt feedback, and in any case provide it sooner than by waiting for the teacher to

review work that is complete or final.

Communicating with parents and caregivers

Since parents and caregivers in a sense “donate” their children to schools (at least figuratively speaking),

teachers are responsible for keeping them informed and involved to whatever extent is practical. Virtually all

parents understand and assume that schools are generally intended for learning. Detailed communication can

enrich parents' understanding, of how learning is addressed with their particular child’s classroom, and show them

more precisely what their particular child is doing. The better such understanding in turn encourages parents and

caregivers to support their child’s learning more confidently and “intelligently”. In this sense it contributes

indirectly to a positive learning environment in their child’s class.

There are various ways to communicate with parents, each with advantages and limitations. Here are three

common examples:

• A regular classroom newsletter: A newsletter establishes a link with parents or caregivers with

comparatively little effort on the part of the teacher. At the beginning of the year, for example, a newsletter

can tell about special materials that students will need, important dates to remember (like professional

development days when there is no school), or about curriculum plans for the next few weeks. But

newsletters also have limitations. They can seem impersonal, and they may get lost on the way home and

never reach parents or caregivers. They can also be impractical for teachers with multiple classes, as in high

school or in specialist subjects (like music or physical education), where each class follows a different

program or curriculum.

• Telephone calls: The main advantage of phoning is its immediacy and individuality. Teacher and parent or

caregiver can talk about a particular student, behavior, or concern, and do it now. By the same token,

however, phone calls are not an efficient way for informing parents about events or activities that affect

everyone in common. The individuality of phoning may explain why teachers often use this method when a

student has a problem that is urgent or unusual—as when he has failed a test, missed classes, or

misbehaved seriously. Rightly or wrongly, a student’s successes tend not to prompt phone calls to the

student’s home (though in fairness students may be more likely to tell parents about their successes

themselves, making it less essential for the teacher to do so).

• Parent-teacher conferences: Most schools schedule periodic times—often a day or evening per term—when

teachers meet briefly with parents or caregivers who wish to meet. Under good conditions, the conferences

have the individuality of phone calls, but also the richness of communication possible only in face-to-face

meetings. Since conferences are available to all parents, they need not focus on behavior or academic

problems, but often simply help to build rapport and understanding between parents or caregivers and the

teacher. Sometimes too, particularly at younger grade levels, teachers involve students in leading their own

conferences; the students display and explain their own work using a portfolio or other archive of

149

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

accumulated materials (Benson & Barnett, 2005; Stiggins & Chappuis, 2005). In spite of all of these

advantages, though, parent-teacher conferences have limitations. Some parents cannot get to conferences

because of work schedules, child care, or transportation problems. Others may feel intimated by any school-

sponsored event because they speak limited English or because they remember painful experiences from

their own school days.

Even if you make several efforts to communicate, some parents may remain out of contact. In these cases it is

important to remember that the parents may not be indifferent to their child or to the value of education. Other

possibilities exist, as some of our comments above imply: parents may have difficulties with child care, for example,

have inconvenient work schedules, or feel self-conscious about their own communication skills (Stevens &

Tollafield, 2003). Even so, there are ways to encourage parents who may be shy, hesitant, or busy. One is to think

about how they can assist the school even from home—for example, by making materials to be used in class or (if

they are comfortable using English) phoning other parents about class events. A second way is to have a specific

task for the parents in mind—one with clear structure, such as photocopying materials to be used by students later.

A third is to remember to encourage, support, and respect the parents’ presence and contributions when they do

show up at school functions. Keep in mind that parents are experts about their own particular children, and without

them, you would have no students to teach!

Responding to student misbehavior

So far we have focused on preventing behaviors that are inappropriate or annoying. The advice has all been pro-

active or forward-looking: plan classroom space thoughtfully, create reasonable procedures and rules, pace lessons

and activities appropriately, and communicate the importance of learning clearly. Although we consider these ideas

important, it would be naïve to imply they are enough to prevent all behavior problems. For various reasons,

students sometimes still do things that disrupt other students or interrupt the flow of activities. At such moments

the challenge is not about long-term planning but about making appropriate, but prompt responses. Misbehaviors

left alone can be contagious, a process educators sometimes call the ripple effect (Kounin, 1970). Chatting

between two students, for example, can gradually spread to six students; rudeness by one can eventually become

rudeness by several; and so on. Because of this tendency, delaying a response to inappropriate behavior can make

the job of getting students back on track harder than responding to it as immediately as possible.

There are many ways to respond to inappropriate behaviors, of course, and they vary in how much they focus on

the immediate behavior compared to longer-term features or patterns of a student’s behavior. There are so many

ways to respond, in fact, that we can describe only a sample of the possibilities here. None are effective all of the

time, though all do work at least some of the time. We start with a response that may not seem on the surface like a

remedy at all—simply ignoring misbehaviors.

Ignoring misbehaviors

A lot of misbehaviors are not important or frequent enough to deserve any response at all. They are likely to

disappear (or extinguish, in behaviorist terms) simply if left alone. If a student who is usually quiet during class

happens to whisper to a neighbor once in awhile, it is probably less disruptive and just as effective to ignore the

infraction than to respond to it. Some misbehaviors may not be worth a response even if they are frequent, as long

as they do not seem to bother others. Suppose, for example, that a certain student has a habit of choosing quiet

Educational Psychology 150 A Global Text

7. Classroom management and the learning environment

seat-work times to sharpen her pencil. She is continually out of her seat to go to the sharpener. Yet this behavior is

not really noticed by others. Is it then really a problem, however unnecessary or ill-timed it may be? In both

examples ignoring the behavior may be wise because there is little danger of the behavior disrupting other students

or of becoming more frequent. Interrupting your activities—or the students’—might cause more disruption than

simply ignoring the problem.

That said, there can still be problems in deciding whether a particular misbehavior is truly minor, infrequent, or

unnoticed by others. Unlike in our example above, students may whisper to each other more than “rarely” but less

than “often”: in that case, when do you decide that the whispering is in fact too frequent and needs a more active

response from you? Or the student who sharpens her pencil, mentioned above, may not bother most others, but she

may nonetheless bother a few. In that case how many bothered classmates are “too many”? Five, three, just one,

or…? In these ambiguous cases, you may need more active ways of dealing with an inappropriate behavior, like the

ones described in the next sections.

Gesturing nonverbally

Sometimes it works to communicate using gestures, eye contact, or “body language” that involve little or no

speaking. Nonverbal cues are often appropriate if a misbehavior is just a bit too serious or frequent to ignore, but

not serious or frequent enough to merit taking the time deliberately to speak to or talk with the student. If two

students are chatting off-task for a relatively extended time, for example, sometimes a glance in their direction, a

frown, or even just moving closer to the students is enough of a reminder to get them back on task. Even if these

responses prove not to be enough, they may help to keep the off-task behavior from spreading to other students.

A risk of relying on nonverbal cues, however, is that some students may not understand their meaning, or may

even fail to notice them. If the two chatting students mentioned above are engrossed in their talking, for example,

they may not see you glance or frown at them. Or they might notice but not interpret your cue as a reminder to get

back on task. Misinterpretation of nonverbal gestures and cues is more likely with young children, who are still

learning the subtleties of adults’ nonverbal “language” (Guerrero & Floyd, 2005; Heimann, et al., 2006). It is also

more likely with students who speak limited English or whose cultural background differs significantly from your

own. These students may have learned different nonverbal gestures from your own as part of their participation in

their original culture (Marsh, Elfenbein, & Ambady, 2003).

Natural and logical consequences

Consequences are the outcomes or results of an action. When managing a classroom, two kinds of consequences

are especially effective for influencing students' behavior: natural consequences and logical consequences. As the

term implies, natural consequences happen “naturally”, without deliberate intention by anyone. If a student is

late for class, for example, a natural consequence is that he misses information or material that needed to do an

assignment. Logical consequences are ones that happen because of the responses of or decisions by others, but

that also have an obvious or “logical” relationship to the original action. If one student steals another’s lunch, for

example, a logical consequence might be for the thief to reimburse the victim for the cost of the lunch. Natural and

logical consequences are often woven together and thus hard to distinguish: if one student picks a fight with

another student, a natural consequence might be injury not only to the victim, but also to the aggressor (an

151

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

inherent byproduct of fighting), but a logical consequence might be to lose friends (the response of others to

fighting). In practice both may occur.

In general research has found that both natural and logical consequences can be effective for minimizing

undesirable behaviors, provided they are applied in appropriate situations (Weinstein, Tomlinson-Clarke, &

Curran, 2004). Consider a student who runs impulsively down school hallways. The student is likely to have “traffic

accidents”, and thus (hopefully) to see that running is not safe and to reduce the frequency of running. Or consider

a student who chronically talks during class instead of working on an assigned task. The student may have to make

up the assignment later, possibly as homework. Because the behavior and the consequence are connected logically,

the student is relatively likely to see the drawback of choosing to talk, and to reduce how much he or she talks on

subsequent occasions. In either case, whether natural or logical, the key features that make consequences work are

(a) that they are appropriate to the misbehavior and (b) that the student understands the connection between the

consequences and the original behavior.

Notice, though, that natural and logical consequences do not always work; if they did, there would be no further

need for management strategies! One limitation is that misbehaviors can sometimes be so serious that no natural or

logical consequence seems sufficient or appropriate. Suppose, for example, that one student deliberately breaks

another student’s eyeglasses. There may be a natural consequence for the victim (he or she will not be able to see

easily), but not for the student who broke the glasses. There may also be no consequences for the aggressor that are

both logical and fully satisfactory: the aggressor student will not be able to repair the broken glasses himself, and

may not be able to pay for new glasses either.

Another limitation of natural and logical consequences is that their success depends on the motives of the

misbehaving student. If the student is seeking attention or acceptance by others, then consequences often work

well. Bullying in order to impress others, for example, is more likely to lose friends than to win them—so bullying

motivated in this way is self-limiting. If a student is seeking power over others, on the other hand, then the

consequences of bullying may not reduce the behavior. Bullying in order to control others’ actions by definition

actually achieves its own goal, and its “natural” result (losing friends) would be irrelevant. Of course, a bully might

also act from a combination of motives, so that natural and logical consequences limit bullying behavior, but only

partially.

A third problem with natural and logical consequences is that they can easily be confused with deliberate

punishment (Kohn, 2006). The difference is important. Consequences are focused on repairing damage and

restoring relationships, and in this sense they focus on the future. Punishments highlight a mistake or wrongdoing

and in this sense focus on the past. Consequences tend to be more solution focused. Punishments tend to highlight

the person who committed the action, and they often shame or humiliate the wrong doer. (Table 17 summarizes

these and other differences.)

Table 17: Differences between consequences and punishments

Focused on future solutions Focused on past mistakes

Focused on individual’s actions Focused on character of student or child

Focused on repairing mistakes Focused on establishing blame

Educational Psychology 152 A Global Text

7. Classroom management and the learning environment

Focused on restoring positive relationships Focused on isolating wrong-doer

Tend to reduce emotional pain and conflict Tend to impose emotional pain or conflict

Classroom examples of the differences between consequences and punishment are plentiful. If a student fails to

listen to the teacher's instructions, then a consequence is that he or she misses important information, but a

punishment may be that the teacher criticizes or reprimands the student. If a student speaks rudely to the teacher, a

consequence may be that the teacher does not respond to the comment, or simply reminds the student to speak

courteously. A punishment may be that the teacher scolds the student in the presence of others , or even imposes a

detention (“Stay after school for 15 minutes”).

Conflict resolution and problem solving

When a student misbehaves persistently and disruptively, you will need strategies that are more active and

assertive than the ones discussed so far, and that focus on conflict resolution—the reduction of disagreements

that persist over time. Conflict resolution strategies that educators and teachers tend to use usually have two parts

(Jones, 2004). First, they involve ways of identifying what “the” problem is precisely. Second, they remind the

student of classroom expectations and rules with simple clarity and assertiveness, but without apology or

harshness. When used together, the two strategies not only reduce conflicts between a teacher and an individual

student, but also provide a model for other students to follow when they have disagreements of their own. The next

sections discuss the nature of assertion and clarification for conflict resolution in more detail.

Step 1: clarifying and focusing: problem ownership

Classrooms can be emotional places even though their primary purpose is to promote thinking rather than

expression of feelings. The emotions can be quite desirable: they can give teachers and students “passion” for

learning and a sense of care among members of the class. But feelings can also cause trouble if students misbehave:

at those moments negative feelings—annoyance, anger, discomfort—can interfere with understanding exactly what

is wrong and how to set things right again. Gaining a bit of distance from the negative feelings is exactly what those

moments need, especially on the part of the teacher, the person with (presumably) the greatest maturity.

In a widely cited approach to conflict resolution called Teacher Effectiveness Training, the educator Thomas

Gordon describes this challenge as an issue of problem ownership, or deciding whose problem a behavior or

conflict it really is (Gordon, 2003). The “owner” of the problem is the primary person who is troubled or bothered

by it. The owner can be the student committing the behavior, the teacher, or another student who merely happens

to see the behavior. Since the owner of a problem needs to take primary responsibility for solving it, identifying

ownership makes a difference in how to deal with the behavior or problem effectively.

Suppose, for example, that a student named David makes a remark that the teacher finds offensive (like “Sean is

fat”). Is this remark the student's problem or the teacher's? If David made the comment privately to the teacher and

is unlikely to repeat it, then maybe it is only the teacher's problem. If he is likely to repeat it to other students or to

Sean himself, however, then maybe the problem is really David's. On the other hand, suppose that a different

student, Sarah, complains repeatedly that classmates refuse to let her into group projects. This is less likely to be

153

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

the teacher’s problem rather than Sarah's: her difficulty may affect her ability to do her own work, but not really

affect the teacher or classmates directly. As you might suspect, too, a problem may sometimes affect several people

at once. David, who criticized Sean, may discover that he offended not only the teacher, but also classmates, who

therefore avoid working with him. At that point the whole class begins to share in some aspect of “the” problem: not

only is David prevented from working with others comfortably, but also classmates and the teacher begin dealing

with bad feelings about David.

Step 2: active, empathetic listening

Diagnosing accurately who really has a problem with a behavior—who “owns” it—is helped by a number of

strategies. One is active listening—attending carefully to all aspects of what a student says and attempting to

understand or empathize as fully as possible, even if you do not agree with what is being said (Cooper & Simonds,

2003). Active listening involves asking questions in order continually to check your understanding. It also involves

encouraging the student to elaborate on his or her remarks, and paraphrasing and summarizing what the student

says in order to check your perceptions of what is said. It is important not to move too fast toward solving the

problem with advice, instructions, or scolding, even if these are responses that you might, as a teacher, feel

responsible for making. Responding too soon with solutions can shut down communication prematurely, and leave

you with inaccurate impressions of the source or nature of the problem.

Step 3: assertive discipline and I-messages

Once you have listened well to the student’s point of view, it helps to frame your responses and comments in

terms of how the student’s behavior affects you in particular, especially in your role as the teacher. The comments

should have several features:

• They should be assertive—neither passive and apologetic, nor unnecessarily hostile and aggressive (Cantor,

1996). State the problem as matter-of-factly as possible: “Joe, you are talking while I’m explaining

something”, instead of either “Joe, do you think you could be quiet now?” or “Joe, be quiet!”

• The comments should emphasize I-messages (Gordon, 1981), which are comments that focus on how the

problem behavior is affecting the teacher’s ability to teach, as well as how the behavior makes the teacher

feel. They are distinct from you-messages, which focus on evaluating the mistake or problem which the

student has created. An I-message might be, “Your talking is making it hard for me to remember what I’m

trying to say.” A you-message might be, “Your talking is rude.”

• The comments should encourage the student to think about the effects of his or her actions on others—a

strategy that in effect encourages the student to consider the ethical implications of the actions (Gibbs,

2003). Instead of simply saying: “When you cut in line ahead of the other kids, that was not fair to them”,

you can try saying, “How do you think the other kids feel when you cut in line ahead of them?”

Step 4: negotiation

The first three steps describe ways of interacting that are desirable, but also fairly specific in scope and limited in

duration. But in themselves, they may not be enough when conflict persists over time and develops a number of

complications or confusing features. A student may persist in being late for class, for example, in spite of efforts by

the teacher to modify this behavior. Or two students may repeatedly speak rudely to each other, even though the

teacher has mediated this conflict in the past. Or a student may fail to complete homework, time after time. Because

Educational Psychology 154 A Global Text

7. Classroom management and the learning environment

these problems develop over time, and because they may involve repeated disagreements, they can eventually

become stressful for the teacher, the student, and any classmates who may be affected. Their persistence can tempt

a teacher simply to dictate a resolution—a decision that can leave everyone feeling defeated, including the teacher.

Often in these situations it is better to negotiate a solution, which means systematically discussing options and

compromising on one if possible. Although negotiation always requires time and effort, it is often less time or effort

than continuing to cope with the original problem, and the results can be beneficial to everyone. A number of

experts on conflict resolution have suggested strategies for negotiating with students about persistent problems

(Davidson & Wood, 2004). The suggestions vary in detail, but usually include some combination of the steps we

have already discussed above, along with a few others:

• Decide as accurately as possible what the problem is. Usually this step involves a lot of the active listening

described above.

• Brainstorm possible solutions, and then consider their effectiveness. Remember to include students in

this step; otherwise you end up simply imposing a solution on others, which is not what negotiation is

supposed to achieve.

• If possible, choose a solution by consensus. Complete agreement on the choice may not be possible, but

strive for it as best you can. Remember that taking a vote may be a democratic, acceptable way to settle

differences in some situations, but if feelings are running high, voting does not work as well. In that case

voting may simply allow the majority to impose its will on the minority, leaving the underlying conflict

unresolved.

• Pay attention to how well the solution works after it is underway. For many reasons, things may not work

out the way you or students hope or expect. You may need to renegotiate the solution at a later time.

Keeping management issues in perspective

There are two primary messages from this chapter. One is that management issues are important, complex, and

deserving of serious attention. The other is that strategies exist that can reduce, if not eliminate, management

problems when and if they occur. We have explained some of those strategies—including some intended to prevent

problems and others intended to remedy problems.

But there is an underlying assumption about management that this chapter emphasized fully: that good

classroom management is not an end in itself, but a means for creating a classroom where learning happens and

students are motivated. Amidst the stresses of handling a problem behavior, there is a risk of losing sight of this

idea. Telling a student to be quiet is never a goal in itself, for example; it is desirable only because (or when) it

allows all students to hear the teacher’s instructions or classmates’ spoken comments, or because it allows students

to concentrate on their work. There may actually be moments when students' keeping quiet is not appropriate, such

as during a “free choice” time in an elementary classroom or during a group work task in a middle school

classroom. As teachers, we need to keep this perspective firmly in mind. Classroom management should serve

students’ learning, and not the other way around. The next chapter is based on this idea, because it discusses ways

not just to set the stage for learning, as this chapter has done, but ways to plan directly for students' learning.

155

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Chapter summary

Classroom management is the coordination of lessons and activities to make learning as productive as possible.

It is important because classrooms are complex and somewhat unpredictable, because students respond to

teachers’ actions in diverse ways, and because society requires that students attend school. There are two major

features of management: preventing problems before they occur and responding to them after they occur. Many

management problems can be prevented by attending to how classroom space is used, by establishing daily

procedures, routines, and rules, by pacing and structuring activities appropriately, and by communicating the

importance of learning and of positive behavior to students and parents. There are several ways of dealing with a

management problem after it occurs, and the choice depends on the nature of the problem. A teacher can simply

ignore a misbehavior, gesture or cue students nonverbally, rely on natural and logical consequences, or engage

conflict resolution strategies. Whatever tactics the teacher uses, it is important to keep in mind their ultimate

purpose: to make learning possible and effective.

On the Internet

This is part of a larger website for teachers

containing resources of all kinds. This section—about classroom management—has several articles with very “nuts

and bolts” tips about management. You may also find their page of resources for substitute teachers useful.

Another website for teachers with lots of resources of all kinds. A section called

“Power Tools” has dozens of brief articles about various aspects of classroom management.

Key termsActive listeningClassroom management

Conflict resolutionI-messages

Learning environmentLogical consequences

Natural consequencesNegotiation

OverlappingPortfolio

Problem ownershipProcedures

Ripple effectRules

Withitness

References

Benson, B. & Barnett, S. (2005). Student-led conferencing using showcase portfolios. Thousand Oaks, CA:

Corwin Press.

Black, P., Harrison, C., Lee C., Marshall, B., & Wiliam, D. (2004). Working inside the black box: Assessment

for learning in the classroom. Phi Delta Kappan, 86(1), 8-21.

Bothmer, S. (2003). Creating the peaceable classroom. Tuscon, AZ: Zephyr Press.

Britt, T. (2005). Effects of identity-relevance and task difficulty on task motivation, stress, and performance.

Motivation and Emotion, 29(3), 189-202.

Brophy, J. (2004). Motivating students to learn, 2nd edition. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Brookfield, S. (2006). The skillful teacher: On technique, trust, and responsiveness in the classroom, 2nd

edition. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Educational Psychology 156 A Global Text

7. Classroom management and the learning environment

Brown, D. (2004). Urban teachers’ professed classroom management strategies: Reflections of culturally

responsive teaching. Urban Education, 39(3), 266-289.

Chesebro, J. (2003). Effects of teacher clarity and nonverbal immediacy on student learning, receiver

apprehension, and affect. Communication Education, 52(2), 135-147.

Cooper, P. & Simonds, C. (2003). Communication for the classroom teacher, 7th edition. Boston: Allyn &

Bacon.

Davidson, J. & Wood, C. (2004). A conflict resolution model. Theory into Practice, 43(1), 6-13.

Emmer, E. & Stough, L. (2001). Classroom management: A critical part of educational psychology, with

implications for teacher education. Educational Psychologist, 36(2), 103-112.

Gibbs, J. (2003). Moral development and reality: Beyond the theories of Kohlberg and Hoffman. Thousand

Oaks, CA: Sage.

Good, T. & Brophy, J. (2002). Looking in classrooms, 9th edition. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.

Gordon, T. (2003). Teacher effectiveness training. New York: Three Rivers Press.

Guerrero, L. & Floyd, K. (2005). Nonverbal communication in close relationships. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Heimann , M. Strid, K., Smith , L., Tjus , T., Ulvund , S. & Meltzoff, A. (2006). Exploring the relation between

memory, gestural communication, and the emergence of language in infancy: a longitudinal study. Infant

and Child Development, 15(3), 233-249.

Jones, T. (2004). Conflict resolution education: The field, the findings, and the future. Conflict Resolution

Quarterly, 22(1-2), 233-267.

Jones, V. & Jones, L. (2006). Comprehensive classroom management: Creating communities of support

and solving problems, 6th edition. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.

Kohn, A. (2006). Beyond discipline: From compliance to community. Reston, VA: Association for

Supervision and Curriculum Development.

Kounin, J. (1970). Discipline and group management in classrooms. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

Marks, L. (2003). Instructional management tips for teachers of students with autism-spectrum disorder.

Teaching Exceptional Children, 35(4), 50-54.

Marsh, A., Elfenbein, H. & Ambady, N. (2003). Nonverbal “accents”: cultural differences in facial expressions

of emotion. Psychological Science, 14(3), 373-376.

Marzano, R. & Marzano, J. (2004). The key to classroom management. Educational Leadership, 62, pp. 2-7.

McCafferty, S., Jacobs, G., & Iddings, S. (Eds.). (2006). Cooperative learning and second language

teaching. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Moritz, J. & Christie, A. (2005). It’s elementary: Using elementary portfolios with young students. In C.

Crawford (Ed.), Proceedings of the Society for Information Technology and Teacher Education

157

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

International Conference 2005 (pp. 144-151). Chesapeake, VA: Association for the Advancement of

Computing in Education.

Nations, S. & Boyett, S. (2002). So much stuff, so little space: Creating and managing the learner-centered

classroom. Gainesville, FL: Maupin House.

Reynolds, A. (1992). What is competent beginning teaching? Review of Educational Research, 62(1), 1-35.

Stevens, B. & Tollafield, A. (2003). Creating comfortable and productive parent/teacher conferences. Phi

Delta Kappan, 84(7), 521-525.

Stiggins, R. & Chappuis, J. (2005). Using student-involved classroom assessment to close achievement gaps.

Theory into Practice 44(1), 11-18.

Thorson, S. (2003). Listening to students: Reflections on secondary classroom management. Boston: Allyn

& Bacon.

Turiel, E. (2006). The development of morality. In W. Damon, R. Lerner, & N. Eisenberg (Eds.), Handbook

of child psychology, vol. 3, pp. 789-857. New York: Wiley.

Van Meerionboer, J., Kirschner, P., & Kester, L. (2003). Taking the cognitive load off a learner’s mind:

Instructional design for complex learning. Educational Psychologist, 38(1), 5-13.

White, C. (2005). Student portfolios: An alternative way of encouraging and evaluating student learning. In

M. Achacoso & N. Svinicki (Eds.), Alternative Strategies for Evaluating Student Learning (pp. 37-42).

San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Weinstein, C.,Tomlinson-Clarke, S., & Curran, M. (2004). Toward a conception of culturally responsive

classroom management. Journal of Teacher Education, 55(1), 25-38.

Educational Psychology 158 A Global Text

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

8. The nature of classroom communication

“Be sincere; be brief; be seated.”

(Franklin Delano Roosevelt)

Franklin Roosevelt was a former president of the United States, and he advised being brief and sincere when

communicating. In advising to be seated, he was being somewhat more indirect; perhaps he was suggesting that

conversation and dialog would be improved by reducing the power differences between individuals. If so, he was

giving good advice, though perhaps it was also a bit misleading in its simplicity. As teachers, we face almost

continual talk at school, supplemented by ample amounts of nonverbal communication—gestures, facial

expressions, and other “body language”. Often the talk involves many people at once, or even an entire class, and

individuals have to take turns speaking while also listening to others having their turns, or sometimes ignoring the

others if a conversation does not concern them. As the teacher, therefore, you find yourself playing an assortment of

roles when communicating in classrooms: Master of Ceremonies, referee—and of course source of new knowledge.

Your challenge is to sort the roles out so that you are playing the right ones in the right combinations at the right

times. As you learn to do this, interestingly, much of your communication with students will indeed acquire the

qualities recommended by Franklin Roosevelt. Often, you will indeed be more sincere and brief, and you will find

that minimizing power differences between you and students is a good idea.

In this chapter we look at how you might begin to move toward these goals. We describe briefly several major

features of classroom communication that distinguish it from communication in other familiar situations. Then we

explain several techniques, both verbal and nonverbal, that contribute to effective communication, and describe

how these manifest themselves in several common activity settings, which we call structures of participation. As

you will see, how an activity is organized—its structure of participation—has a major effect on how students

communicate with each other and with the teacher.

Communication in classrooms vs communication elsewhere

Classroom events are often so complex that just talking with students can become confusing. It helps to think of

the challenge as a problem in communication—or as one expert put it, of “who says what to whom, and with

what effect” (Lasswell, 1964). In classrooms, things often do not happen at an even pace or in a logical order, or

with just the teacher and one student interacting while others listen or wait patiently. While such moments do

occur, events may sometimes instead be more like a kaleidoscope of overlapping interactions, disruptions, and

decision—even when activities are generally going well. One student finishes a task while another is still only half-

way done. A third student looks like she is reading, but she may really be dreaming. You begin to bring her back on

task by speaking to her, only to be interrupted by a fourth student with a question about an assignment. While you

answer the fourth student, a fifth walks in with a message from the office requiring a response; so the bored (third)

Educational Psychology 159 A Global Text

8. The nature of classroom communication

student is overlooked awhile longer. Meanwhile, the first student—the one who finished the current task—now

begins telling a joke to a sixth student, just to pass the time. You wonder, “Should I speak now to the bored, quiet

reader or to the joke-telling student? Or should I move on with the lesson?” While you are wondering this, a

seventh student raises his hand with a question, and so on.

One way to manage situations like these is to understand and become comfortable with the key features of

communication that are characteristic of classrooms. One set of features has to do with the functions or purposes of

communication, especially the balance among talk related to content, to procedures, and to controlling behavior.

Another feature has to do with the nature of nonverbal communication—how it supplements and sometimes even

contradicts what is said verbally. A third feature has to do with the unwritten expectations held by students and

teachers about how to participate in particular kinds of class activities—what we will later call the structure of

participation.

Functions of talk: content, procedures, and behavior control

Classrooms are different from many other group situations in that communication serves a unique combination

of three purposes at once: content, procedures, or behavior control (Wells, 2006). Content talk focuses on what is

being learned; it happens when a teacher or student states or asks about an idea or concept, for example, or when

someone explains or elaborates on some bit of new knowledge (Burns & Myhill, 2004). Usually content talk relates

in some obvious way to the curriculum or to current learning objectives, as when a teacher tells a high school

history class, “As the text explains, there were several major causes of the American Civil War.” But content talk can

also digress from the current learning objectives; a first-grade student might unexpectedly bring a caterpillar to

school and ask about how it transforms into a butterfly.

Procedural talk, as its name implies, is about administrative rules or routines needed to accomplish tasks in a

classroom. It happens, for example, when the teacher says, “When you are done with your spelling books, put them

in the bins at the side of the room”, or when a student asks, “Do you want us to print our names at the top of page?”

Procedural talk provides information that students need to coordinate their activities in what can be a relatively

crowded space—the classroom—and under conditions in which time may be relatively short or tightly scheduled. It

generally keeps activities organized and flowing smoothly. Procedural talk is not primarily about removing or

correcting unwanted behavior, although certain administrative procedures might sometimes annoy a particular

student, or students might sometimes forget to follow a procedure. Instead it is intended to provide the guidance

that students need to coordinate with each other and with the teacher.

Control talk is about preventing or correcting misbehaviors when they occur, particularly when the

misbehaviors are not because of ignorance of procedures. It happens, for example, when a teacher says, “Jill, you

were talking when you should have been listening”, or “Jason, you need to work on your math instead of doodling.”

Most control talk originates with the teacher, but students sometimes engage in it with each other, if not with the

teacher. One student may look at a nearby classmate who is whispering out of turn and quietly say, “Shhh!” in an

attempt to silence the behavior. Or a student may respond to being teased by a classmate by saying simply, “Stop

it!” Whether originating from the teacher or a student, control talk may not always be fully effective. But its purpose

is, by definition, to influence or control inappropriate behavior. Since control talk is obviously important for

managing class effectively, we discussed it at length in Chapter 7.

160

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

What can make classroom discourse confusing is that two of its functions—content and procedures—often

become combined with the third, control talk, in the same remark or interaction. A teacher may ask a content-

related question, for example, as a form of control talk. She may, for example, ask, “Jeremy, what did you think of

the film we just saw?” The question is apparently about content, but the teacher may also be trying to end Jeremy’s

daydreaming and to get him back on task—an example of control talk. Or a teacher may state a rule: “When one

person is talking, others need to be listening.” The rule is procedural in that it helps to coordinate classroom

dialogue, but it may also control inattentive behavior. Double functions like these can sometimes confuse students

because of their ambiguity, and lead to misunderstandings between certain students and teachers. A student may

hear only the content or procedural function of a teacher’s comment, and miss an implied request or command to

change inappropriate behavior (Collins & Michaels, 2006). But double functions can also help lessons to flow

smoothly by minimizing the disruption of attending to a minor behavior problem and by allowing more continuous

attention to content or procedures.

Verbal, nonverbal, and unintended communication

Another way to understand classroom communication is to distinguish verbal from nonverbal communication,

and intended both unintended forms of communication. As the name suggests, verbal communication is a

message or information expressed in words, either orally or in writing. Classrooms obviously have lots of verbal

communication; it happens every time a teacher explains a bit of content, asks a question, or writes information or

instructions on the chalkboard. Non-verbal communications are gestures or behaviors that convey

information, often simultaneously with spoken words (Guerrero, 2006). It happens, for example, when a teacher

looks directly at students to emphasize a point or to assert her authority, or when the teacher raises her eyebrows to

convey disapproval or disagreement. Nonverbal behaviors are just as plentiful as verbal communications, and while

they usually add to a current verbal message, they sometimes can also contradict it. A teacher can state verbally,

“This math lesson will be fun”, and a nonverbal twinkle in the eye can send the confirm message nonverbally. But a

simultaneous nonverbal sigh or slouch may send the opposite message—that the lesson will not, in fact be fun, in

spite of the teacher’s verbal claim.

Whether verbal or nonverbal, however, classroom communications often convey more meaning than is

intended. Unintended communications are the excess meanings of utterances; they are the messages received

by students without the teacher’s awareness or desire. A teacher may say, “This section of the text won’t be on the

test, but read it anyway for background.” But a student may instead hear the message, “Do not read this section of

the text.” What is heard is not what the teacher intended to be heard.

Like many public settings that involve a diversity of people, classrooms tend to rely heavily on explicit, verbal

communication, while at the same time recognizing and allowing nonverbal communications to occur (Neill, 1991).

This priority accounts for the characteristically businesslike style of teacher talk—a style that we discuss in detail in

the next chapter. A major reason for relying on an explicit, businesslike verbal style is that diversity among

individuals increases the chances of their misinterpreting each other. Because of differences in background, the

partners may differ in how they expect to structure conversation as well as other kinds of dialog.

Misunderstandings may result—sometimes without the partners being able to pinpoint the cause. Later in this

chapter we suggest how to minimize these problems.

Educational Psychology 161 A Global Text

8. The nature of classroom communication

Effective verbal communication

Communicating effectively requires using all forms of classroom talk in combinations appropriate for particular

utterances and interactions. In various places earlier in this book, we have suggested ways of doing so, though in

those places we usually did not frame the discussion around the term communication as such.

Effective content talk

In Chapter 8, for example, we suggested ways of talking about content so that it is most likely to be understood

clearly, but in that chapter we described these as instructional strategies. In explaining ideas, for example, whether

briefly or as a extended lecture, we pointed out that it helps to offer, in advance, organizing ideas, to relate new

content to prior knowledge, and to organize and elaborate on new information. In the same chapter, we also

suggested strategies about content talk intended for students, so that students understand their own thinking as

well as possible. We especially highlighted two ways of learning: inquiry learning and cooperative learning. Table

18summarizes instructional strategies both for students and for teachers, and indicates how they contribute to

effective verbal communication about content.

Table 18: Strategies for supporting content talk

Content talk by teachers

Strategy Definition How it helps communication

Using advance organizers Statements or ideas that give a

concise overview of new material

Orients students’ attention to

new ideas about to be learned;

assists in understanding and

remembering new material

Relating new material to prior

knowledge

Explicit connections of new ideas

to students’ existing knowledge

Facilitates discussion of new

material by making it more

meaningful to students

Elaborating and extending new

information

Explanations of new ideas in full,

complete terms

Avoids ambiguities and

misunderstandings about new ideas

or concepts

Organizing new information Providing and following a clear

structure when explaining new

material

Assists in understanding and

remembering new material

Content talk by students

Inquiry learning Students pursue problems that

they help to formulate for

themselves

To formulate and and investigate

a problem, students need to express

clearly what they wish to find out.

Cooperative learning Students work in small groups to To work together, students need

162

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

solve a common problem or task to explain ideas and questions to

fellow students clearly.

Table 19: Major strategies of effective procedural and control talk

These strategies are also discussed in Chapter 7 as features of classroom management, rather than of

communication. Note, too, that the difference between procedural and content talk is arbitrary to some extent; in

many situations one kind of talk serves the needs of the other kind.

Strategy for procedural talk Strategy for control talk

Creating and discussing procedures for daily routines Creating and discussing classroom rules of

appropriate behavior

Announcing transitions between activities Clarifying problem ownership

Providing clear instructions and guidance for

activities

Listening actively and empathetically

Reminding students periodically of procedures for

completing a task

Using I-messages

Effective procedural and control talk

In addition to communicating about content, teachers need to communicate procedures and expectations about

appropriate classroom behavior. In Chapter 7 we described quite a few ways to communicate with students about

these matters, though, in that chapter we did not refer to them as methods of communication, but as methods of

classroom management, of creating a positive learning environment, and of resolving conflicts in the class. Table 19

summarizes several of the major strategies described in that chapter.) By framing communication in these ways, we

called attention to their importance as forms of communication. As we pointed out, procedural talk and control talk

matter are used in teaching simply because clear procedures and appropriate classroom behavior are necessary

students are to learn.

Effective nonverbal communication

In spite of their importance, words are not the only way that teachers and students communicate. Gestures and

behaviors convey information as well, often supporting a teacher’s words, but sometimes also contradicting them.

Students and teachers express themselves nonverbally in all conversations, so freely and automatically in fact that

this form of communication can easily be overlooked.

Eye contact

One important nonverbal behavior is eye contact, which is the extent and timing of when a speaker looks

directly at the eyes of the listener. In conversations between friends of equal status, for example, most native

speakers of English tend to look directly at the speaker when listening, but to avert their gaze when speaking

(Kleinke, 1986). Re-engaging eye contact, in fact, often signals that a speaker is about to finish a turn and is inviting

a response from the listener.

Educational Psychology 163 A Global Text

8. The nature of classroom communication

But conversations follow different rules if they involve someone of greater authority talking with someone of

lesser authority, such as between a teacher and a student. In that case, the person in authority signals greater status

by gazing directly at the listener almost continuously, whether listening or speaking. This alternate pattern can

sometimes prove awkward if either party is not expecting it. For students unused to continuous eye contact, it can

feel like the teacher is staring excessively, intrusively, or inappropriately; an ironic effect can be for the student to

feel more self-conscious rather than more engaged, as intended. For similar reasons, inexperienced or first-time

teachers can also feel uncomfortable with gazing at students continuously. Nevertheless research about the effects

of eye contact suggests that it may help anyone, whether a student or teacher, to remember what they are seeing

and hearing (Mason, Hood, & Macrae, 2004).

Communication problems result less from eye contact as such than from differences in expectations about eye

contact. If students’ expectations differ very much from the teacher’s, one party may misinterpret the other party’s

motivations. Among some non-white ethnic groups, for example, eye contact follows a pattern that reverses the

conventional white, English-language pattern: they tend to look more intently at a partner when talking, and avert

gaze when listening (Razack, 1998). The alternative pattern works perfectly well as long as both parties expect it

and use it. As you might imagine, though, there are problems if the two partners use opposite patterns of eye

contact. In that case one person may interpret a direct gaze as an invitation to start talking, when really it is an

invitation to stop talking. Eventually the conversational partner may find himself interrupting too much, or simply

talking too long at a turn. The converse can also happen: if the first person looks away, the partner may take the

gesture as inviting the partner to keep listening, when really the first person is inviting the partner to start talking.

Awkward gaps between comments may result. In either case, if the conversational partners are a teacher and

student, rapport may deteriorate gradually. In the first case, the teacher may even conclude, wrongly, that the

student is socially inept because the student interrupts so much. In the second case, the teacher may conclude—also

wrongly—that the student is very shy or even lacking in language skill.

To avoid such misunderstandings, a teacher needs to note and remember students’ preferred gaze patterns at

times when students are free to look wherever and at whomever they please. Traditional seats-in-a-row desk

arrangements do not work well for this purpose; as you might suppose, and as research confirms, sitting in rows

makes students more likely to look either at the teacher or to look at nothing in particular (Rosenfeld, Lambert, &

Black, 1985; Razack, 1998). Almost any other seating arrangement, such as sitting in clusters or in a circle,

encourages freer patterns of eye contact. More comfortable eye contact, in turn, makes for verbal communication

that is more comfortable and productive.

Wait time

Another important nonverbal behavior is wait time, which is the pause between conversational turns. Wait

time marks when a conversational turn begins or ends. If a teacher asks a question, for example, the wait time both

allows and prompts students to formulate an appropriate response. Studies on classroom interaction generally

show that wait times in most classes are remarkably short—less than one second (Good & Brophy, 2002).

Unfortunately wait times this short can actually interfere with most students’ thinking; in one second, most

students either cannot decide what to say or can only recall a simple, automatic fact (Tobin, 1987). Increasing wait

times to several seconds has several desirable effects: students give longer, more elaborate responses, they express

more complex ideas, and a wider range of students participate in discussion. For many teachers, however, learning

164

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

to increase wait time this much takes conscious effort, and may feel uncomfortable at first. (A trick, if you are trying

to wait longer, is to count silently to five before calling on anyone.) After a few weeks of practice, discomfort with

longer wait times usually subsides, and the academic benefits of waiting become more evident.

As with eye contact, preferred wait times vary both among individuals and among groups of students, and the

differences in expected wait times can sometimes lead to awkward conversations. Though there are many

exceptions, girls tend to prefer longer wait times than boys—perhaps contributing to an impression that girls are

unnecessarily shy or that boys are self-centered or impulsive. Students from some ethnic and cultural groups tend

to prefer a much longer wait time than is typically available in a classroom, especially when English is the student’s

second language (Toth, 2004). When a teacher converses with a member of such a group, therefore, what feels to

the student like a respectful pause may seem like hesitation or resistance to the teacher. Yet other cultural groups

actually prefer overlapping comments—a sort of negative wait time. In these situations, one conversational partner

will begin at exactly the same instant as the previous speaker, or even before the speaker has finished (Chami-

Sather & Kretschmer, 2005). The negative wait time is meant to signal lively interest in the conversation. A teacher

who is used to a one-second gap between comments, however, may regard overlapping comments as rude

interruptions, and may also have trouble getting chances to speak.

Even though longer wait times are often preferable, they do not always work well with certain individuals or

groups. For teachers, the most widely useful advice is to match wait time to the students’ preferences as closely as

possible, regardless of whether these are slower or faster than what the teacher normally prefers. To the extent that

a teacher and students can match each other’s pace, they will communicate more comfortably and fully, and a larger

proportion of students will participate in discussions and activities. As with eye contact, observing students’

preferred wait times is easier in situations that give students some degree of freedom about when and how to

participate, such as open-ended discussions or informal conversations throughout the day.

Social distance

When two people interact, the physical space or distance between them—their social distance—often indicates

something about how intimate or personal their relationship is (Noller, 2006). Social distance also affects how

people describe others and their actions; someone who habitually is more distant physically is apt to be described in

more general, abstract terms than someone who often approaches more closely (Fujita, et al., 2006). In white

American society, a distance of approximately half a meter to a meter is what most people prefer when talking face-

to-face with a personal friend. The closer end of this range is more common if the individuals turn sideways to each

other, as when riding on an elevator; but usually the closest distances are reserved for truly intimate friendships,

such as between spouses. If the relationship is more businesslike, individuals are more likely to situate themselves

in the range of approximately one meter to a three meters. This is a common distance, for example, for a teacher

talking with a student or talking with a small group of students. For still more formal interactions, individuals tend

to allow more than three meters; this distance is typical, for example, when a teacher speaks to an entire class.

Just as with eye contact and wait time, however, individuals differ in the distances they prefer for these different

levels of intimacy, and complications happen if two people expect different distances for the same kind of

relationship. A student who prefers a shorter social distance than her partner can seem pushy or overly familiar to

the partner. The latter, in turn, can seem aloof or unfriendly—literally “distant”. The sources of these effects are

Educational Psychology 165 A Global Text

8. The nature of classroom communication

easy to overlook since by definition the partners never discuss social distance verbally, but they are real. The best

remedy, again, is for teachers to observe students’ naturally occurring preferences as closely as possible, and to

respect them as much as possible: students who need to be closer should be allowed to be closer, at least within

reasonable limits, and those who need to be more distant should be allowed to be more distant.

Structures of participation: effects on communication

Many class activities take on patterns that guide communication in ways that class members learn to expect,

often without even being reminded. Each pattern is a participation structure, a set of rights and responsibilities

expected from students and teacher during an activity. Sometimes the teacher announces or explains the rights and

responsibilities explicitly, though often they are just implied by the actions of class members, and individual

students learn them simply by watching others. A lecture, for example, has a particular participation structure:

students are responsible for listening, for raising a hand to speak, and for keeping comments brief and relevant if

called on. The teacher, on the other hand, has the right to talk at length, but also the responsibility to keep the talk

relevant and comprehensible.

In principle, a host of participation structures are possible, but just a handful account for most class activities

(Cazden, 2001). Here are some of the most common:

• Lecturing—the teacher talks and students listen. Maybe students take notes, but maybe not.

• Questions and answers—the teacher asks a series of questions, calling on one student at a time to answer

each of them. Students raise their hands to be recognized and give answers that are brief and “correct”. In

earlier times this participation structure was sometimes called recitation.

• Discussion—the teacher briefly describes a topic or problem and invites students to comment on it.

Students say something relevant about the topic, but also are supposed to respond to previous speakers if

possible.

• Group work—the teacher assigns a general task, and a small group of students work out the details of

implementing it. The teacher may check on the group’s progress before they finish, but not necessarily.

Each of these structures influences how communication among teachers and students tends to occur; in fact

each is itself sort of an implied message about how, when, and with whom to interact. To see how this influence

works, look in the next sections at how the participation structures affected classroom communication for one of us

authors (Kelvin Seifert) as he taught one particular topic—children’s play—over a twenty-year period. The topic was

part of a university-level course for future teachers. During this time, Kelvin’s goals about the topic remained the

same: to stimulate students’ thinking about the nature and purposes of play. But over time he tried several different

structures of participation, and students’ ways of communicating changed as a result.

Lecture

The first time Kelvin taught about children’s play, he lectured about it. He used this structure of participation

not because he believed on principle that it was the best, but because it was convenient and used widely by his

fellow university teachers. An excerpt from Kelvin’s lecture notes is shown in Table 20, and gives a sense of what he

covered at that time.

166

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

In some ways the lecture proved effective: Kelvin covered the material efficiently (in about 20 minutes), related

the topic to other ones in the course, defined and explained all key terms clearly, and did his best to relate the

material to what he thought were students’ own interests. These were all marks of good lecturing (Christensen,

2006). Students were mostly quiet during the lecture, but since only about one-third of them took notes, Kelvin had

to assume that the rest had committed the material to memory while listening. The students quietness bothered

him a little, but as a newcomer to university teaching, Kelvin was relieved simply to get through the class without

embarrassment or active resistance from the students.

But there were also some negative signs. In spite of their courtesy, few students lingered after class to talk about

children’s play or to ask questions. Worse yet, few students chose children’s play as a term paper topic, even though

it might have made a highly interesting and enjoyable one. On the final exam few seemed able to relate concepts

about play to their own experiences as teachers or leaders of recreational activities.

There was an even more subtle problem. The lecture about play focused overtly on a topic (play) that praised

action, intrinsic motivation, and self-choice. But by presenting these ideas as a lecture, Kelvin also implied an

opposite message unintentionally: that learning is something done passively, and that it follows an intellectual path

set only by the teacher. Even the physical layout of the classroom sent this message—desks faced forward, as if to

remind students to look only at the person lecturing. These are features of lecturing, as Kelvin later discovered, that

are widely criticized in educational research (McKeachie & Svinicki, 2005; Benedict & Hoag, 2004). To some

students the lecture format might even have implied that learning is equivalent to daydreaming, since both

activities require sitting quietly and showing little expression. An obvious solution might have been to invite

students to comment from time to time during the lecture, relating the topic to experiences and knowledge of their

own. But during Kelvin’s first year of teaching about play, he did little of this. The lecture medium, ironically,

contradicted the lecture message, or at least it assumed that students would think actively about the material

without ever speaking.

Questions and answers

Because of these problems, Kelvin modified his approach after a few years of teaching to include more asking of

questions which students were invited to answer. This turned the lecture on children’s play into something more

like a series of explanations of key ideas, interrupted by asking students to express their beliefs, knowledge, or

experience about children’s play. Kelvin’s preparation notes changes in appearance as a result (see Table 21).

Asking questions and inviting brief responses was reassuring because it gave indications of whether students were

listening and understanding the material. Questions served both to motivate students to listen and to assess how

much and how well they knew the material. In this regard Kelvin was using a form of communication that was and

continues to be very popular with many teachers (Cazden, 2001).

But there were also new challenges and problems. For one thing the topic of children’s play took longer to cover

than before, since Kelvin now had to allow time for students to respond to questions. This fact forced him to leave

out a few points that he used to include. More serious, though, was his impression that students often did not listen

to each other’s responses; they only listened carefully to Kelvin, the teacher. The interactions often become simply

two-way exchanges between the teacher and one student at a time: Kelvin asked, one student responded, Kelvin

Educational Psychology 167 A Global Text

8. The nature of classroom communication

acknowledged or (sometimes) evaluated. (Mehan, 1979; Richards, 2006). Some of the exchanges could in principle

have happened just as easily without any classmates present.

In general students still had little control over the course of discussion. Kelvin wondered if he was controlling

participation too much—in fact whether the question-and-answer strategy attempted the impossible task of

controlling students’ very thought processes. By asking most of the questions himself and allowing students only

brief responses, was Kelvin trying to insure e that students thought about children’s play in the “right” way, his

way? To give students more influence in discussion, it seemed that Kelvin would have to become less concerned

about precisely what ideas about children’s play he covered.

Classroom discussion

After several more years of teaching, Kelvin quit lectures altogether, even ones interspersed with questions and

answers. He began simply leading general discussions about children’s play. The change again affected his planning

for this topic. Instead of outlining detailed content, he now just made concise notes that listed issues about

children’s play that students needed to consider (some of the notes are shown in Table 23). The shift in

participation structure led to several major changes in communication between teacher and students as well as

among students. Since students spoke more freely than before, it became easier to see whether they cared about the

topic. Now, too, more students seemed motivated to think and learn about children’s play; quite a few selected this

topic, for example, for their term projects. Needless to say, these changes were all to the good.

But there were also changes that limited the effectiveness of classroom communication, even though students

were nominally freer to speak than ever. Kelvin found, for example, that certain students spoke more than their

share of the time—almost too freely, in fact, in effect preventing more hesitant students from speaking. Sometimes,

too, it seemed as if certain students did not listen to others’ comments, but instead just passed the time waiting for

their turn to speak, their hands propped permanently in the air. Meanwhile there were still others who passed the

time apparently hoping not to speak; they were busy doodling or staring out the window. Since the precise focus of

discussion was no longer under Kelvin’s control, furthermore, discussions often did not cover all of the ideas about

children’s play that Kelvin considered important. On one occasion, for example, he meant for students to discuss

whether play is always motivated intrinsically, but instead they ended up talking about whether play can really be

used to teach every possible subject area. In itself the shift in focus was not bad, but it did make Kelvin wonder

whether he was covering the material adequately. In having these misgivings, as it happened, he was supported by

other educators who have studied the effects of class discussions on learning (McKeatchie & Svinciki, 2005).

Group work

By the time he had taught about children’s play for twenty years, Kelvin had developed enough concerns about

discussion as a communication strategy that he shifted approach again. This time he began using a form of

collaborative group work: small teams of students carrying out projects on aspects of children’s play that

interested them, making observations of children at play, reporting on their results to the class, and writing a

common report about their work. (Kelvin’s work guidelines given to the groups are shown in Table 22.) Kelvin

hoped that by giving students a common focus, communication among them would improve. Conversations would

deal with the tasks at hand, students would necessarily listen to each other, and no one could afford either to

dominate talk excessively or to fall silent.

168

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

In some ways these benefits did take place. With a bit of encouragement from Kelvin, students listened to each

other more of the time than before. They also diversified their tasks and responsibilities within each group, and

they seemed to learn from each other in the course of preparing projects. Participation in the unit about children’s

play reached an all-time high in Kelvin’s twenty years of teaching at university. Yet even still there were problems.

Some groups seemed much more productive than others, and observing them closely suggested that differences

were related to ease of communication within groups. In some groups, one or two people dominated conversations

unduly. If they listened to others at all, they seemed immediately to forget that they had done so and proceeded to

implement their own ideas. In other groups, members all worked hard, but they did not often share ideas or news

about each other’s progress; essentially they worked independently in spite of belonging to the group. Here, too,

Kelvin’s experience corroborated other, more systematic observations of communication within classroom work

groups (Slavin, 1995). When all groups were planning at the same time, furthermore, communication broke down

for a very practical reason: the volume of sound in the classroom got so high that even simple conversation became

difficult, let alone the expression of subtle or complex ideas.

Communication styles in the classroom

Teachers and students have identifiable styles of talking to each other that linguists call a register. A register is

a pattern of vocabulary, grammar, and expressions or comments that people associate with a social role. A familiar

example is the “baby-talk” register often used to speak to an infant. Its features—simple repeated words and

nonsense syllables, and exaggerated changes in pitch—mark the speaker as an adult and mark the listener as an

infant. The classroom language register works the same way; it helps indicate who the teacher is and who the

student is. Teachers and students use the register more in some situations than in others, but its use is common

enough that most people in our society have no trouble recognizing it when they hear it (Cazden, 2001). In the

following scene, for example, the speakers are labeled only with letters of the alphabet; yet figuring out who is the

teacher and who are the students is not difficult:

A: All right now, I want your eyes up here. All eyes on me, please. B, are you ready to work? We are

going to try a new kind of math problem today. It’s called long division. Does anyone know what

long division is? C, what do you think it is?

C: Division with bigger numbers?

A: Any other ideas? D?

E (not D): Division by two digits.

A: …I only call on people who raise their hands. D, can you help with the answer?

D: Division with remainders.

A: Close. Actually you’re both partly right.

In this scene Person A must surely be the teacher because he or she uses a lot of procedural and control talk, and

because he or she introduces a new curriculum topic, long division. The other Persons (B, C, D, and E) must be

students because they only respond to questions, and because they individually say relatively little compared to

Person A.

Educational Psychology 169 A Global Text

8. The nature of classroom communication

In general, effective classroom communication depends on understanding how features of the classroom talk

register like these operate during actual class times. In the following sections therefore we describe details of

classroom talk, and then follow with suggestions about how to use the register as effectively as possible. In both of

these sections we assume that the better the communication, the better the learning and thinking displayed by

students. For convenience we divide classroom talk into two parts, teacher talk and student talk.

How teachers talk

Although teacher talk varies somewhat with the tasks or purposes at hand, it also has uniformities that occur

across a range of situations. Using detailed observations of discourse in science activities, for example, Jay Lemke

identified all of the following strategies from observations of teachers’ classroom talk (1990). Each strategy

simultaneously influences the course of discussion and focuses students’ attention, and in these ways also helps

indirectly to insure appropriate classroom behavior:

• Nominating, terminating, and interrupting speakers: Teachers often choose who gets to speak. (“Jose,

what do you think about X?”). On the other hand, they often bring an end to a student’s turn at speaking or

even interrupt the student before he or she finishes. (“Thanks; we need to move on now.”)

• Marking importance or irrelevance: Teachers sometimes indicate that an idea is important (“That’s a good

idea, Lyla.”). On the other hand, they sometimes also indicate that an idea is not crucial or important

(“Your right, but that’s not quite the answer I was looking for.”), or fully relevant (“We’re talking about the

book Wuthering Heights, not the movie that you may have seen.”). Marking importance and relevance

obviously helps a teacher to reinforce key content. But the strategy can also serve to improve relationships

among students if the teacher deliberately marks or highlights an idea offered by a quiet or shy student

(O’Connor & Michaels, 1996; Cohen, et al., 2004). In that case marking importance can build both a

student’s confidence and the student’s status in the eyes of classmates.

• Signaling boundaries between activities: Teachers declare when an activity is over and a new one is

starting—an example of the procedural talk that we discussed earlier. (“We need to move on. Put away your

spelling and find your math books.”) In addition to clarifying procedures, though, signaling boundaries can

also insure appropriate classroom behavior. Ending an activity can sometimes help restore order among

students who have become overly energetic, and shifting to a new activity can sometimes restore motivation

to students who have become bored or tired.

• Asking “test” questions and evaluating students’ responses: Teachers often ask test questions—questions to

which they already know the answer. Then they evaluate the quality or correctness of the students’ answers

(Teacher: “How much is 6 x 7 ?” Student: “42.” Teacher: “That’s right.”). Test questions obviously help

teachers to assess students’ learning, but they also mark the teacher as the expert in the classroom, and

therefore as a person entitled to control the flow of discourse.

There are additional features of teacher-talk that are not unique to teachers. These primarily function to make

teachers’ comments more comprehensible, especially when spoken to a group, but they also help to mark a person

who uses them as a teacher (Cazden, 2001; Black, 2004):

170

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

• Exaggerated changes in pitch: When busy teaching, teachers tend to exaggerate changes in the pitch of

their voice—reminiscent of the “sing-song” style of adults when directing speech to infants. Exaggerated

pitch changes are especially characteristic of teachers of young students, but they happen at all grade levels.

• Careful enunciation: In class teachers tend to speak more slowly, clearly, and carefully than when

conversing with a friend. The style makes a speaker sound somewhat formal, especially when combined

with formal vocabulary and grammar, mentioned next.

• Formal vocabulary and grammar: Teachers tend to use vocabulary and grammar that is more formally

polite and correct, and that uses relatively few slang or casual expressions. (Instead of saying “Get out your

stuff”, they more likely say, “Please get out your materials.”) The formality creates a businesslike distance

between teachers and students—hopefully one conducive to getting work done, rather than one that seems

simply cold or uncaring. The touch of formality also makes teachers sound a bit more intelligent or

intellectual than in casual conversation, and in this way reinforces their authority in the classroom.

How students talk

Children and youth also use a characteristic speech register when they are in a classroom and playing the role of

students in the presence of a teacher. Their register—student talk—differs somewhat from the teacher’s because of

their obvious differences in responsibilities, levels of knowledge, and relationships with each other and with the

teacher. Student-talk and teacher-talk are similar in that both involve language strategies that guide content and

procedures, and that sometimes seek to limit the inappropriate behavior of others. Compared to teachers’, though,

students’ language strategies often pursue these goals a bit more indirectly.

• Agenda enforcement: Sometimes students interrupt a discussion to ask about or remind others, and

especially the teacher, of an agreed-on agenda. If the teacher tells students to open their text to an incorrect

page, for example, a student may raise her hand to correct the teacher—or even do so without raising a

hand. This communication strategy is one of more public, direct ways that students influence activities in

the classroom, but its power is limited, since it does not create new activities, but simply returns the class to

activities agreed on previously.

• Digression attempts: During a discussion or activity, a student asks a question or makes a statement that is

not relevant to the task at hand. While the teacher is leading students in a discussion of a story that they

read, for example, a student raises his hand and asks, “Mr X, when does recess begin?”

• Side talk: One student talks to another student, either to be sociable (“Did you see that movie last week?”)

or to get information needed for the current assigned task (“What page are we on?”). Sometimes side talk

also serves to control or limit fellow students’ behavior, and in this way functions like control-talk by

teachers (as when a student whispers, “Shhh! I’m trying to listen” or “Go ahead and ask her!”). The ability

of such talk to influence classmates’ behavior is real, but limited, since students generally do not have as

much authority as teachers.

• Calling out: A student speaks out of turn without being recognized by the teacher. The student’s comment

may or may not be relevant to the ongoing task or topic, and the teacher may or may not acknowledge or

respond to it. Whether ignored or not, however, calling out may change the direction of a discussion by

Educational Psychology 171 A Global Text

8. The nature of classroom communication

influencing fellow students’ thinking or behavior, or by triggering procedural and control talk by the

teacher. (“Jason, it’s not your turn; I only call on students who raise their hands.”)

• Answering a question with a question: Instead of answering a teacher’s “test” question directly, the

student responds with a question of her own, either for clarification or as a stalling tactic (“Do you mean

X?”). Either way, the effect is to shift the discussion or questioning to content or topics that are safer and

more familiar.

• Silence: The student says nothing in response to a speaker’s comments or to an invitation to speak. The

speaker could be either the teacher or a fellow student. The silence makes the speaker less likely to continue

the current topic, and more likely to seek a new one.

• Eye contact, gaze aversion, and posture: The student looks directly at the teacher while the teacher is

speaking, or else deliberately averts gaze. The student may also adopt any variety of postures while sitting

(sit up straight vs slouching). As we discussed earlier in this chapter, the timing of eye gaze depends partly

on cultural expectations that the student brings to school. But it may also represent a deliberate choice by

the student—a message to the teacher and to classmates. The same can be said about sitting posture. In

classroom situations, listening is conventionally indicated by looking directly at the teacher, and either

sitting up straight or leaning slightly forward. Although these behaviors can be faked, they tend to indicate,

and to be taken as, a show of interest in and acceptance of what a speaker is saying. By engaging in or

avoiding these behaviors, therefore, students can sometimes influence the length and direction of a

discussion or activity.

Using classroom talk to stimulate students’ thinking

The various features of classroom talk characterize the communication of most teachers and students, at least

when they are in a classroom and “doing school”. (Communication outside of school is a different matter: then

teachers as well as students may speak, listen, and behave quite differently!) As you might suppose, the extent and

balance among the features varies depending on grade level, curriculum area, and personalities of students or

teachers. But failing to use a classroom register at all can easily create communication problems. Suppose, for

example, that a teacher never asks informal test questions. In that case the teacher will learn much less than

otherwise about her students’ knowledge of the current material. Then also suppose that a student does not

understand teachers’ questions as test questions. That student may easily respond in ways that seem disrespectful

(Teacher: “How much is 23 x 42?” Student: “I don’t know; how much do you think it is?”) (Bloome, et al., 2005).

The classroom talk register, then, constrains how communication between teachers and students can take place,

but it also gives teachers and students a “language” for talking about teaching and learning. Given this double-

edged reality, how can teachers use the classroom talk register to good advantage? How, in particular, can teachers

communicate in ways that stimulate more and better thinking and discussion? In the next, final section of the

chapter, we offer some suggestions for answering these questions. As you will see, the suggestions often reinforce

each other. They are more like a network of ideas, not a list of priorities to be considered or followed in sequence.

Probing for learner understanding

How do you know whether a student understands what you are saying? One clue, of course, is by whether the

student is looking at and concentrating on you and your comments. But this clue is not foolproof; we have all had

172

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

moments of staring at a speaker while daydreaming, only to realize later that we have not heard anything that the

speaker said. It is sometimes important, therefore, to probe more actively how much students are actually

understanding during lessons or other activities.

Strategies for probing understanding generally involve mixing instruction with conversation (Renshaw, 2004).

In explaining a new topic, for example, you can check for understanding by asking preliminary questions

connecting the topic to students’ prior experiences and knowledge about the topic. Note that this strategy combines

qualities of both instruction and conversation, in the sense that it involves combining “test” questions, to which you

already know the answer, with real questions, to which you do not. When introducing a science lesson about density

to kindergarten children, for example, the teacher might reasonably ask both of the following:

Teacher: Which of these objects that I have do you expect will sink and which ones will float? (A test

question—the teacher already will know the answer.)

Teacher: What other things have you seen that float? Or that sink? (A real question—the teacher is

asking about their experience and does not know the answer.)

By asking both kinds of questions, the teacher scaffolds the children’s learning, or creates a zone of proximal

development, which we described in Chapter 2 as part of Vygotsky’s theory of learning. Note that this zone has two

important features, both of which contribute to children’s thinking. One is that it stimulates students’ thinking (by

asking them questions), and the other is that it creates a supportive and caring atmosphere (by honoring their

personal experiences with real questions). The resulting mix of warmth and challenge can be especially motivating

(Goldstein, 1999).

When warmth and challenge are both present in a discussion, it sometimes even becomes possible to do what

may at first seem risky: calling on individual students randomly without the students’ volunteering to speak. In a

study of “cold calling” as a technique in university class discussions, the researchers found that students did not

find the practice especially stressful or punitive, as the teachers feared they might, and that spontaneous

participation in discussion actually improved as a result (Dallimore, et al., 2006). The benefit was most likely to

happen, however, when combined with gestures of respect for students, such as warning individuals ahead of class

that they might be called on, or allowing students to formulate ideas in small groups before beginning to call on

individuals.

Helping students to articulate their ideas and thinking

The classroom talk register is well designed to help students articulate ideas and thoughts, particularly when

used in the context of discussion. In addition to the conversational probes, like the ones we described in the

previous section, there are other ways to support students in expressing their ideas fully and clearly. One way is for

the teacher to check repeatedly on her own understanding of students’ contributions as a discussion unfolds.

Consider this exchange:

Student (during a class discussion): It seems to me that we all need to learn more climate change.

Teacher: What do you mean by “learn more”? It’s a big topic; what parts of it are you thinking

about?

Educational Psychology 173 A Global Text

8. The nature of classroom communication

Still another strategy for helping students to articulate their ideas is to increase the wait time between when the

teacher asks a question and when the teacher expects a student to answer. As we pointed out earlier, wait times that

are longer than average—longer than one second, that is—give students more time to formulate ideas and therefore

to express themselves more completely and precisely (Good & Brophy, 2002). In addition, longer wait times have

the added advantage of indirection: instead of telling a student to say more, the teacher needs only to wait for the

student to say more.

In general any communication strategy will help students become more articulate if it both allows and invites

further comment and elaboration on their ideas. Taken together, the invitations closely resemble a description of

class discussion, though they can actually be used singly at any time during teaching. Consider these possible

conversational moves:

• The teacher asks the student to explain his initial idea more completely.

• The teacher rephrases a comment made by a student.

• The teacher compares the student’s idea to another, related idea, and asks the student to comment.

• The teacher asks for evidence supporting the student’s idea.

• The teacher asks the student how confident he is in his idea.

• The teacher asks another student to comment on the first student’s idea.

Promoting academic risk-taking and problem-solving

In Chapter 8 we described major features of problem solving, as well as three techniques that assist in solving

problems—problem analysis, working backwards from the beginning, and analogical thinking. While all of the

techniques are helpful, they do not work if a student will not take the risk of attempting a solution to a problem in

the first place. For various reasons students may sometimes avoid such risks, especially if he or she has sometimes

failed at a task in the past, and is therefore concerned about negative evaluations again (Hope & Oliver, 2005).

What can a teacher say or do to counteract such hesitation? There are several strategies, all of which involve

focusing attention on the process of doing an activity rather than on its outcome or evaluation.

• Where possible, call attention to the intrinsic interest or satisfaction of an activity. Consider, for example,

an elementary-level activity of writing a Japanese haiku—a poem with exactly seventeen syllables. This

activity can be satisfying in itself, regardless of how it is evaluated. Casually reminding individuals of this

fact can contribute to students’ sense of ease about writing the haiku and encourage them indirectly to do

better work.

• Minimize the importance of grades where possible. This strategy supports the one above; by giving

students less to worry about, they become freer to experience the intrinsic satisfactions of an activity. In

writing that haiku mentioned above, for example, you can try saying something like: “Don’t worry too much

about your grade; just do the best you can and you will come out well enough in the end.”

• Make sure students know that they have ample time to complete an activity. If students need to rush—or

merely just thinks they do—then they are more likely to choose the safest, most familiar responses possible.

174

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

In writing an amusing story from their early childhood, for example, middle years students may need time

to consider and choose among story possibilities. Then they may need additional time to experiment with

ways of expressing the story in writing. In this case, to make sure students know that they have such time,

try saying something like: “Writing a good story will take time, and you may have to return to it repeatedly.

So we will start working on it today, but do not expect to finish today. We’ll be coming back to it several

times in the next couple of weeks.”

• Show that you value unusual ideas and elegant solutions to problems. When a student does something out

of the ordinary, show your enthusiasm for it. A visually appealing drawing, a well-crafted essay, a different

solution to a math problem than the one you expected—all of these deserve an explicit compliment.

Expressing your interest and respect does more than support the specific achievement. It also expresses a

more general, underlying message that in your classroom, it is safe and rewarding to find and share the

unusual and elegant.

Note that these communication strategies support problem-solving and the related skills of creativity that we

discussed in Chapter 8. In describing creativity in that chapter, in particular, we called attention to the difference

and importance of divergent (open-ended) thinking. As with problem-solving, though, divergent thinking may

seem risky to some students unless they are encouraged to do so explicitly. The strategies for boosting academic

risk-taking can help to communicate this encouragement—that process matters more than product, that there will

be time enough to work, and that you, as teacher, indeed value their efforts.

Promoting a caring community

A caring community is one in which all members have a respected place, in which diversity among

individuals is expected, and in which individuals assist each other with their work or activities wherever

appropriate. Classrooms and even entire schools can be caring communities, though moving them in this direction

takes work on the part of teachers and other school staff (Noddings, 1992, 2004). The key work in promoting a

caring community involves arranging for students to work together on tasks, while at the same time communicating

the teacher’s commitment to mutual respect among students and between students and teachers. Many of the

instructional strategies discussed earlier in this book, such as cooperative learning and inquiry learning (in Chapter

8), therefore contribute to community in the classroom.

More specifically, you can, as a teacher, encourage community by doing any or all of the following:

• Tell students that you value mutual respect, and describe some of the ways that students can show respect

for each other and for school staff. Better yet, invite students themselves to describe how they might show

respect.

• Look for ways to sustain relationships among students and teachers for extended times. These ways may be

easier to find in elementary school, where a teacher and class normally remain together for an entire year,

than in middle and secondary school, where students learn from many teachers and teachers teach many

students. But still there are ways. Participating in extra-curricular activities (like sports teams or drama

club), for example, can sometimes provide settings where relationships develop for relatively long periods

of time—even more than a single school year.

Educational Psychology 175 A Global Text

8. The nature of classroom communication

• Ask for input from students about what they want to learn, how they want to learn it, and what kind of

evaluation they consider fair. Although using their ideas may feel at first as if you are giving up your

responsibility as the teacher, asking for students’ input indicates respect for students. It is likely that many

of their suggestions need clarification or revision to become workable, especially if the class must also cover

a particular curriculum during a set time. But even just the asking for input shows respect, and can

contribute to community in the classroom.

• If conflicts arise between students or between a student and teacher, encourage respectful communication

as explicitly as you can. Some communication strategies about conflict resolution were described in Chapter

7 and are helpful in this regard: identifying true problem ownership, listening actively, assertive (not

aggressive) I-messages, and negotiation.

• Find times and ways for the class to experience itself as a community. This suggestion may look a bit vague

at first glance, but in practice it is actually quite concrete. Any action builds community if it is carried out by

the group as a whole, especially if it is done regularly and repeatedly and if it truly includes every member

of the class. Such actions become rituals, not in the negative sense of empty or mindless repetitions, but in

the positive sense of confirmations by group members of their commitment to each other (Ehrenreich,

2007). In the elementary grades, an obvious example of a ritual is reciting the Pledge of Allegiance (or its

equivalent in classrooms outside the United States). But there are many other examples of classroom

routines that gradually acquire the (positive) qualities of ritual or community-affirmation, often without

deliberate intention or effort. A daily, regular time to work through homework problems together in class,

for example, may serve obvious academic purposes. But it may also gradually contribute to a classroom’s

identity as a class. With time and familiarity the group homework time may eventually come to represent

“who we are” and of “what we do here” for that class.

The bottom line: messages sent, messages reconstructed

As we have explained in this chapter, teachers and students communicate in multiple, overlapping ways.

Communications may often be expressed in words—but not necessarily and not completely. They may be organized

into lectures, questions, discussions, or group projects. They tend to be expressed in particular language registers

that we have called simply teacher talk and student talk. All things considered, communication obviously serves a

wide range of teaching and learning tasks and activities, from stimulating students’ thinking, to orchestrating

classroom routines, to managing inappropriate behaviors. It is an intrinsic part of the parts of teaching that involve

interaction among class members.

Note, though, that teaching consists of more than interaction among class members. There are times when

teachers prepare lessons or activities, for example, without talking to students or anyone else. There are also times

when they develop their own skills as teachers—for example, by reading and reflecting, or by attending professional

development seminars or workshops—which may involve communication, but not in the sense discussed in this

chapter. It is to these other parts of teaching that we turn in the next chapter.

Table 20: Year one: Kelvin’s lecture notes

Nature and Purposes of Children’s Play

176

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

1. Introduction to topic: What do we mean by play?

• excess energy

• seeking stimulation—relieve boredom

• escape from work

2. Six qualities defining play

• intrinsic motivation

• attention to the process, not the product

• non-literal behavior—make-believe

• no external rules

• self-governed

• active engagement

3. Implications for teaching

• devise activities with play-like qualities

• learn by watching children playing

Table 21: Year three: Kelvin’s question-and-answer notes

Nature and Purposes of Children’s Play

1. Introduction to topic: What do we mean by play? [First ask 1-2 students for their own answers to question.]

• excess energy [Ask: What evidence is there for this?]

• seeking stimulation—relieve boredom […or for this?]

• escape from work

2. Six qualities of children’s play [Invite students’ definitions, but keep them brief.]

• intrinsic motivation

• attention to the process, not the product

• nonliteral behavior—make-believe

• no external rules

• self-governed

• active engagement

Educational Psychology 177 A Global Text

8. The nature of classroom communication

[Can you think of examples and/or counterexamples of each quality?]

3. Implications for teaching

• devise activities with playlike qualities [What activities have you already seen as a student teacher?]

• learn by watching children playing [How could you do this? Invite suggested strategies from students.]

Table 22: Year eight: Kelvin’s discussion notes

Nature and Purposes of Children’s Play

• Discuss possible explanations for play—what do students think are its true purposes? (10 minutes?)

• Can we define play? Brainstorm defining qualities, with examples. (30 minutes)

• Important question for all defining qualities: Are there exceptions—examples of play that do not show

certain defining qualities, but are still play? (15 minutes)

• What is important about play for teaching? (10 minutes +)

• …for the welfare of children? (10 minutes +)

• Etc. (anything else brought up by students)

Table 23: Year twenty: Kelvin’s guidelines for group work

Nature and Purposes of Children’s Play

• Make sure you listen to everyone, and not just to the people you agree with the most. Part of the

challenge of this project is to include all team members.

• You do not have to be best friends with someone in order to be partners. But you do have to get the work

done.

• Remember that it takes many skills and abilities to do this project well. Among other things, you need to:

1) find and understand research and other publications about children’s play,

2) observe children skillfully when they are playing,

3) have confidence in describing what you learn to group mates,

4) write about what you learn, and 5) be tactful and respectful when listening and talking with

partners.

Chapter summary

Because communication in classrooms is more complex and unpredictable than in many other situations, it is

important for teachers to understand its unique features and functions. It is helpful to think of classroom

communication as serving a mixture of three purposes at once: content talk, procedural talk, and behavior control

178

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

talk. It is also helpful to recognize that classroom communication has elements that are not only verbal, but also

nonverbal and unintended.

To be effective in using verbal communication, teachers need to use appropriate instructional strategies related

to content, such as using advance organizers, relating new information to prior knowledge, and organizing new

information on behalf of students. It includes strategies that assist students to communicate, such as inquiry

learning and cooperative learning. To communicate well about procedures and about the behaviors expected of

students, teachers need a variety of management techniques, such as those discussed in Chapter 7 and summarized

again in Table 19. To be effective in using nonverbal communication, teachers need to use appropriate eye contact,

allow ample wait time between speaking turns, and be aware of the effects of social distance on students.

Structures of participation influence communication by facilitating particular patterns of speaking and listening,

while at the same time making other patterns less convenient or disapproved. Four common participation

structures are lectures, questions-and-answers, classroom discussions, and group work.

Key termsCaring communityClass discussions

Collaborative group workCommunication

Content talkControl talk

Eye contactLecture

Nonverbal communicationParticipation structures

Procedural talkQuestions-and-answer

RegisterSocial distance

Student talk registerTeacher talk register

Unintended communicationVerbal communication

Wait time

On the Internet

This URL offers tips for

enhancing classroom communication. It is organized around ten basic topics (e.g. “Organizing Effective

Discussions”) and focuses primarily on verbal communication. It is part of the more general website for Union

University of Jackson, Tennessee.

This website contains over 40 short papers (1-4 pages each) on a

variety of topics, including many related to enhancing communication, but also some related to classroom

organization and management in general. Some of the papers refer to college or university teaching, but many are

quite relevant to public school teaching.

This

website contains a thorough discussion of nonverbal communication—more detailed than possible in this chapter,

and with photos and drawings to illustrate key points.

This website contains many resources, among

which are articles about classroom management and communication, including nonverbal communication. It is

intended strictly for public school teachers. Once you get to the homepage, click on their “Newsletter” for the

articles.

Educational Psychology 179 A Global Text

8. The nature of classroom communication

References

Benedict, M. & Hoag, J. (2004). Seating location in large lectures: Is location related to performance?

Journal of Economics Eduction, 35(3), 215-231.

Black, L. (2004). Teacher-pupil talk in whole-class discussions and processes of social positioning in primary

classrooms. Language and Education, 18(1), 347-360.

Bloome, D., Carter, S., Christian, B., Otto, S., & Shuart-Faris, N. (2005). Discourse analysis and the study of

classroom language. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Burns, C. & Myhill, D. (2004). Interactive or inactive? A consideration of the nature of interaction in whole-

class instruction. Cambridge Journal of Education, 34(1), 35-49.

Cazden, C. (2001). Classroom discourse: The language of teaching and learning, 2nd edition. Westport, CT:

Heinemann.

Chami-Sather, G. & Kretschmer, R. (2005). Lebanese/Arabic and American children’s discourse in group-

solving situations. Language and Education, 19(1), 10-22.

Christensen, N. (2006). The nuts and bolts of running a lecture course. In A. DeNeef & C. Goodwin (Eds.),

The academic’s handbook, 3rd edition, pp. 179-186. Durham, NC: Duke University Press.

Cohen, E., Brody, C., & Sapon-Shevin, M. (2004). Teaching cooperative learning. Albany, NY: State

University of New York Press.

Collins, J. & Michaels, S. (2006). Speaking and writing: Discourse strategies and the acquisition of literacy.

In J. Cook-Gumperz (Ed.), The social construction of literacy, 2nd edition,245-263. New York: Cambridge

University Press.

Dallimore, E., Hertenstein, J., & Platt, M. (2006). Nonvoluntary class participation in graduate discussion

courses: Effects of grading and cold calling. Journal of Management Education, 30(2), 354-377.

DePaulo, B., Lindsay, J., Malone, B., Muhlenbruck, L., Charlton, K., & Cooper, H. (2003). Cues to deception.

Psychological Bulletin, 129, 74-118.

Ehrenreich, B. (2007). Dancing in the streets. New York: Henry Holt/Metropolitan Books.

Fujita, K., Henderson, M., Eng, J., Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2006). Spatial distance and mental construal

of events. Psychological Science, 17(4), 278-282.

Global Deception Research Team. (2006). A world of lies. Journal of Cross-cultural Psychology, 37(6), 60-

74.

Goldstein, L. (1999). The relational zone: The role of caring relationships in the co-construction of mind.

American Educational Research Journal, 36(3), 647-673.

Good, T. & Brophy, J. (2002). Looking in Classrooms, 9th edition. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.

Guerrero, L. (2006). Nonverbal communication in close relationships. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Hope, A. & Oliver, P. (2005). Risk, education, and culture. Burlington, VT: Ashgate Publishing Company.

180

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Kleinke, C. (1986). Gaze and eye contact: A research review. Psychological Bulletin, 100(1), 78-100.

Lasswell, H. (1964). The structure and function of communication in society. In W. Schramm (Ed.), Mass

communications. Urbana, IL: University of Illinois Press.

Lemke, J. (1990). Talking science: Language, learning, and values. Westport, CT: Greenwood Publishing

Group.

Mason, M., Hood, B., & Macrae, C. (2004). Look into my eyes: Gaze direction and person memory. Memory,

12(5), 637-643.

McCarthy, A., Lee, K., Itakura, S., & Muir, D. (2006). Cultural display rules drive eye gaze during thinking.

Journal of Cross-cultural Psychology, 37(6), 717-722.

McKeatchie, W. & Svinicki, M. (2005). Teaching tips: Strategies, research, and theory for college teachers,

12th edition. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

Mehan, H. (1979). Learning lessons: Social organization of the classroom. Cambridge, MA: Harvard

University Press.

Neill, S. (1991). Classroom nonverbal communication. New York: Routledge.

Noddings, N. (1992). The challenge to care in schools: An alternative approach to education. New York:

Teachers College Press.

Noddings, N. (2004). Happiness and education. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Noller, P. (2006). Nonverbal communication in close relationships. In V. Mansunov & M. Patterson (Eds.),

Handbook of nonverbal communication, pp. 403-420.

O’Connor, M. & Michael, S. (1996). Shifting participant frameworks: Orchestrating thinking practices in

group discussion. In D. Hicks (Ed.), Discourse, learning, and schooling (pp. 63-103). New York:

Cambridge University Press.

Razack, S. (1998). Looking White people in the eye: Gender, race, and culture in courtrooms and

classrooms. Toronto, Ontario: University of Toronto Press.

Renshaw, P. (2004). Dialogic teaching, learning, and instruction: Theoretical roots and analytic perspectives.

In J. van der Linden & P. Renshaw (Eds.), Dialogic learning: Shifting perspectives to learning,

instruction, and teaching. Norwell, MA: Kluwer Academic.

Richards, K. (2006). ‘Being the teacher’: Identity and classroom discourse. Applied Linguistics, 27(1), 51-77.

Rosenfeld, P., Lambert, N., & Black, A. (1985). Desk arrangement effects on pupil classroom behavior.

Journal of Educational Psychology, 77 (1), 101-108.

Slavin, R. (1995). Cooperative learning: Theory, research, and practice, 2nd edition. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.

Tobin, K. (1987). The role of wait time in higher cognitive functions. Review of Educational Research, 57(1),

69-95.

Educational Psychology 181 A Global Text

8. The nature of classroom communication

Toth, P. (2004). When grammar instruction undermines cohesion in L2 Spanish classroom discourse. The

Modern Language Journal, 88(1), 14-30.

Wells, G. (2006). The language experience of children at home and at school. In J. Cook-Gumperz (Ed.), The

social construction of literacy, 2nd edition, 76-109. New York: Cambridge University Press.

182

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

9. Facilitating complex thinking

A few years ago one of us (Kelvin) had the privilege of co-teaching with an experienced first grade

teacher, Carolyn Eaton. As part of a research project, Ms Eaton allowed some of her reading lessons

to be observed. Here is what Kelvin saw when Ms Eaton was having a conference with Joey. They

are reading a book “together”, except that Ms Eaton wants Joey to do as much reading as possible

himself. Joey’s comments are capitalized, and Ms Eaton’s are in lowercase.

JOEY: FIRST YOU READ—THEN ME. THIS IS WHAT YOU HAVE TO DO. I READ AFTER YOU,

OK?

Ms Eaton: OK. [Ms Eaton begins.] “In the great green room there was a telephone, a red balloon,

and a picture of…” Are you going to read, or what?

YES.

“In the great green room there was…” Are you ready yet? Ready to read?

OK. “IN THE GREAT GREEN ROOM…”

“…there was…”

“THERE WAS A…” [pauses, looking at Ms Eaton rather than at the words]

“…a telephone…”

YES, THAT’S IT, A TELEPHONE! “IN THE GREAT GREEN ROOM THERE WAS A TELEPHONE, A

RED BALLOON…”

“and a picture of…”

“AND A PICTURE OF” [pauses, staring at the wall]…A COW JUMPING?”

“a cow jumping over the moon”.

“OVER MOON” [smiles from both Joey and Ms Eaton].

Joey, what does this say? [She points to the word telephone.]

“THERE WAS A TELEPHONE”.

How about here? [She points to next page, which reads “And there were three little bears, sitting on

chairs”.]

“THERE WERE BEARS, THREE BEARS, AND THEY SAT ON CHAIRS”.

Educational Psychology 183 A Global Text

9. Facilitating complex thinking

Can you read the whole book?

SURE!

OK, then you start this time.

[Joey looks at first page, alternately at the picture and at the words.]

“IN THE GREAT GREEN ROOM THERE WAS A TELEPHONE”.

[Actual text: “In the great green room, there was a telephone”,]

“AND THERE WAS A RED BALLOON”,

[Actual text: “…and a red balloon”,]

“AND A PICTURE OF THE COW JUMPING OVER THE MOON”.

[Actual text: “…and a picture of the cow jumping over the moon”.]

“AND THERE WERE…” THREE BEARS?… “LITTLE BEARS SITTING ON CHAIRS”.

[Actual text: “And there were three little bears, sitting on chairs,…”]

Could you read this book with you eyes closed?

SURE; WANT TO SEE ME DO IT?!

Well, not right now; maybe another time. Could you read it without the pictures, just looking at the

words? That’s how I do best—when I see the words instead of the pictures.

[Joey pauses to consider this.] MAYBE, BUT NOT QUITE SO WELL.

Let’s try it. [Ms Eaton proceeds to copy the words on a large sheet for Joey to “read” later.]

As Carolyn Eaton’s behavior suggests, there are decisions to make “on the fly”, even during the very act of

teaching. Ms Eaton wonders when to challenge Joey, and when to support him. She also wonders when to pause

and ask Joey to take stock of what he has read, and when to move him on ahead—when to consolidate a student’s

learning, and when to nudge the student forward. These are questions about instructional strategies which

facilitate complex learning, either directly or indirectly. In this chapter we review as many strategies as space

allows, in order to give a sense of the major instructional options and of their effects. We concentrate especially on

two broad categories of instruction, which we call direct instruction and student-centered instruction. As we hope

that you will see, each approach to teaching is useful for certain purposes. We begin, though, by looking at the ways

students think, or at least how teachers would like students to think. What does it mean for students to think

critically (astutely or logically)? Or to think creatively? Or to be skillful problem solvers? Forms of thinking lead to

choices among instructional strategies.

Forms of thinking associated with classroom learning

Although instructional strategies differ in their details, they each encourage particular forms of learning and

thinking. The forms have distinctive educational purposes, even though they sometimes overlap, in the sense that

one form may contribute to success with another form. Consider three somewhat complex forms of thinking that

are commonly pursued in classroom learning: (1) critical thinking, (2) creative thinking, and (3) problem-solving.

184

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Critical thinking

Critical thinking requires skill at analyzing the reliability and validity of information, as well as the attitude or

disposition to do so. The skill and attitude may be displayed with regard to a particular subject matter or topic, but

in principle it can occur in any realm of knowledge (Halpern, 2003; Williams, Oliver, & Stockade, 2004). A critical

thinker does not necessarily have a negative attitude in the everyday sense of constantly criticizing someone or

something. Instead, he or she can be thought of as astute: the critical thinker asks key questions, evaluates the

evidence for ideas, reasons for problems both logically and objectively, and expresses ideas and conclusions clearly

and precisely. Last (but not least), the critical thinker can apply these habits of mind in more than one realm of life

or knowledge.

With such a broad definition, it is not surprising that educators have suggested a variety of specific cognitive

skills as contributing to critical thinking. In one study, for example, the researcher found how critical thinking can

be reflected in regard to a published article was stimulated by annotation—writing questions and comments in the

margins of the article (Liu, 2006). In this study, students were initially instructed in ways of annotating reading

materials. Later, when the students completed additional readings for assignments, it was found that some students

in fact used their annotation skills much more than others—some simply underlined passages, for example, with a

highlighting pen. When essays written about the readings were later analyzed, the ones written by the annotators

were found to be more well reasoned—more critically astute—than the essays written by the other students.

In another study, on the other hand, a researcher found that critical thinking can also involve oral discussion of

personal issues or dilemmas (Hawkins, 2006). In this study, students were asked to verbally describe a recent,

personal incident that disturbed them. Classmates then discussed the incident together in order to identify the

precise reasons why the incident was disturbing, as well as the assumptions that the student made in describing the

incident. The original student—the one who had first told the story—then used the results of the group discussion to

frame a topic for a research essay. In one story of a troubling incident, a student told of a time when a store clerk

has snubbed or rejected the student during a recent shopping errand. Through discussion, classmates decided that

an assumption underlying the student’s disturbance was her suspicion that she had been a victim of racial profiling

based on her skin color. The student then used this idea as the basis for a research essay on the topic of “racial

profiling in retail stores”. The oral discussion thus stimulated critical thinking in the student and the classmates,

but it also relied on their prior critical thinking skills at the same time.

Notice that in both of these research studies, as in others like them, what made the thinking “critical” was

students’ use of metacognition—strategies for thinking about thinking and for monitoring the success and quality

of one’s own thinking. This concept was discussed in Chapter 2 as a feature of constructivist views about learning.

There we pointed out that when students acquire experience in building their own knowledge, they also become

skilled both at knowing how they learn, and at knowing whether they have learned something well. These are two

defining qualities of metacognition, but they are part of critical thinking as well. In fostering critical thinking, a

teacher is really fostering a student’s ability to construct or control his or her own thinking and to avoid being

controlled by ideas unreflectively.

How best to teach critical thinking remains a matter of debate. One issue is whether to infuse critical skills into

existing courses or to teach them through separate, free-standing units or courses. The first approach has the

Educational Psychology 185 A Global Text

9. Facilitating complex thinking

potential advantage of integrating critical thinking into students’ entire educations. But it risks diluting students’

understanding and use of critical thinking simply because critical thinking takes on a different form in each

learning context. Its details and appearance vary among courses and teachers. The free-standing approach has the

opposite qualities: it stands a better chance of being understood clearly and coherently, but at the cost of obscuring

how it is related to other courses, tasks, and activities. This dilemma is the issue—again—of transfer, discussed in

Chapter 2. Unfortunately, research to compare the different strategies for teaching critical thinking does not settle

the matter. The research suggests simply that either infusion or free-standing approaches can work as long as it is

implemented thoroughly and teachers are committed to the value of critical thinking (Halpern, 2003).

A related issue about teaching critical thinking is about deciding who needs to learn critical thinking skills the

most. Should it be all students, or only some of them? Teaching all students seems the more democratic alternative

and thus appropriate for educators. Surveys have found, however, that teachers sometimes favor teaching of critical

thinking only to high-advantage students—the ones who already achieve well, who come from relatively high-

income families, or (for high school students) who take courses intended for university entrance (Warburton &

Torff, 2005). Presumably the rationale for this bias is that high-advantage students can benefit and/or understand

and use critical thinking better than other students. Yet, there is little research evidence to support this idea, even if

it were not ethically questionable. The study by Hawkins (2006) described above, for example, is that critical

thinking was fostered even with students considered low-advantage.

Creative thinking

Creativity is the ability to make or do something new that is also useful or valued by others (Gardner, 1993).

The “something” can be an object (like an essay or painting), a skill (like playing an instrument), or an action (like

using a familiar tool in a new way). To be creative, the object, skill, or action cannot simply be bizarre or strange; it

cannot be new without also being useful or valued, and not simply be the result of accident. If a person types letters

at random that form a poem by chance, the result may be beautiful, but it would not be creative by the definition

above. Viewed this way, creativity includes a wide range of human experience that many people, if not everyone,

have had at some time or other (Kaufman & Baer, 2006). The experience is not restricted to a few geniuses, nor

exclusive to specific fields or activities like art or the composing of music.

Especially important for teachers are two facts. The first is that an important form of creativity is creative

thinking, the generation of ideas that are new as well as useful, productive, and appropriate. The second is that

creative thinking can be stimulated by teachers’ efforts. Teachers can, for example, encourage students’ divergent

thinking—ideas that are open-ended and that lead in many directions (Torrance, 1992; Kim, 2006). Divergent

thinking is stimulated by open-ended questions—questions with many possible answers, such as the following:

• How many uses can you think of for a cup?

• Draw a picture that somehow incorporates all of these words: cat, fire engine, and banana.

• What is the most unusual use you can think of for a shoe?

Note that answering these questions creatively depends partly on having already acquired knowledge about the

objects to which the questions refer. In this sense divergent thinking depends partly on its converse, convergent

thinking, which is focused, logical reasoning about ideas and experiences that lead to specific answers. Up to a

186

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

point, then, developing students’ convergent thinking—as schoolwork often does by emphasizing mastery of

content—facilitates students’ divergent thinking indirectly, and hence also their creativity (Sternberg, 2003; Runco,

2004; Cropley, 2006). But carried to extremes, excessive emphasis on convergent thinking may discourage

creativity.

Whether in school or out, creativity seems to flourish best when the creative activity is its own intrinsic reward,

and a person is relatively unconcerned with what others think of the results. Whatever the activity—composing a

song, writing an essay, organizing a party, or whatever—it is more likely to be creative if the creator focuses on and

enjoys the activity in itself, and thinks relatively little about how others may evaluate the activity (Brophy, 2004).

Unfortunately, encouraging students to ignore others' responses can sometimes pose a challenge for teachers. Not

only is it the teachers' job to evaluate students’ learning of particular ideas or skills, but also they have to do so

within restricted time limits of a course or a school year. In spite of these constraints, though, creativity still can be

encouraged in classrooms at least some of the time (Claxton, Edwards, & Scale-Constantinou, 2006). Suppose, for

example, that students have to be assessed on their understanding and use of particular vocabulary. Testing their

understanding may limit creative thinking; students will understandably focus their energies on learning “right”

answers for the tests. But assessment does not have to happen constantly. There can also be times to encourage

experimentation with vocabulary through writing poems, making word games, or in other thought-provoking ways.

These activities are all potentially creative. To some extent, therefore, learning content and experimenting or

playing with content can both find a place—in fact one of these activities can often support the other. We return to

this point later in this chapter, when we discuss student-centered strategies of instruction, such as cooperative

learning and play as a learning medium.

Problem-solving

Somewhat less open-ended than creative thinking is problem solving, the analysis and solution of tasks or

situations that are complex or ambiguous and that pose difficulties or obstacles of some kind (Mayer & Wittrock,

2006). Problem solving is needed, for example, when a physician analyzes a chest X-ray: a photograph of the chest

is far from clear and requires skill, experience, and resourcefulness to decide which foggy-looking blobs to ignore,

and which to interpret as real physical structures (and therefore real medical concerns). Problem solving is also

needed when a grocery store manager has to decide how to improve the sales of a product: should she put it on sale

at a lower price, or increase publicity for it, or both? Will these actions actually increase sales enough to pay for

their costs?

Problem solving in the classroom

Problem solving happens in classrooms when teachers present tasks or challenges that are deliberately complex

and for which finding a solution is not straightforward or obvious. The responses of students to such problems, as

well as the strategies for assisting them, show the key features of problem solving. Consider this example, and

students’ responses to it. We have numbered and named the paragraphs to make it easier to comment about them

individually:

Scene #1: a problem to be solved

A teacher gave these instructions: “Can you connect all of the dots below using only four straight

lines?” She drew the following display on the chalkboard:

Educational Psychology 187 A Global Text

9. Facilitating complex thinking

Exhibit 10: The teacher gave these

instructions: “Can you connect these dots with

only four lines

The problem itself and the procedure for solving it seemed very clear: simply experiment with

different arrangements of four lines. But two volunteers tried doing it at the board, but were

unsuccessful. Several others worked at it at their seats, but also without success.

Scene #2: coaxing students to re-frame the problem

When no one seemed to be getting it, the teacher asked, “Think about how you’ve set up the problem

in your mind—about what you believe the problem is about. For instance, have you made any

assumptions about how long the lines ought to be? Don’t stay stuck on one approach if it’s not

working!”

Scene #3: Alicia abandons a fixed response

After the teacher said this, Alicia indeed continued to think about how she saw the problem. “The

lines need to be no longer than the distance across the square,” she said to herself. So she tried

several more solutions, but none of them worked either.

The teacher walked by Alicia’s desk and saw what Alicia was doing. She repeated her earlier

comment: “Have you assumed anything about how long the lines ought to be?”

Alicia stared at the teacher blankly, but then smiled and said, “Hmm! You didn’t actually say that

the lines could be no longer than the matrix! Why not make them longer?” So she experimented again

using oversized lines and soon discovered a solution:

Exhibit 11: Alicia's solution

188

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Scene #4: Willem’s and Rachel's alternative strategies

Meanwhile, Willem worked on the problem. As it happened, Willem loved puzzles of all kinds, and

had ample experience with them. He had not, however, seen this particular problem. “It must be a

trick,” he said to himself, because he knew from experience that problems posed in this way often

were not what they first appeared to be. He mused to himself: “Think outside the box, they always

tell you…” And that was just the hint he needed: he drew lines outside the box by making them

longer than the matrix and soon came up with this solution:

Exhibit 12: Willem's and

Rachel's solution

When Rachel went to work, she took one look at the problem and knew the answer immediately: she

had seen this problem before, though she could not remember where. She had also seen other

drawing-related puzzles, and knew that their solution always depended on making the lines longer,

shorter, or differently angled than first expected. After staring at the dots briefly, she drew a solution

faster than Alicia or even Willem. Her solution looked exactly like Willem's.

This story illustrates two common features of problem solving: the effect of degree of structure or constraint on

problem solving, and the effect of mental obstacles to solving problems. The next sections discuss each of these

features, and then looks at common techniques for solving problems.

The effect of constraints: well-structured versus ill-structured problems

Problems vary in how much information they provide for solving a problem, as well as in how many rules or

procedures are needed for a solution. A well-structured problem provides much of the information needed and

can in principle be solved using relatively few clearly understood rules. Classic examples are the word problems

often taught in math lessons or classes: everything you need to know is contained within the stated problem and the

solution procedures are relatively clear and precise. An ill-structured problem has the converse qualities: the

information is not necessarily within the problem, solution procedures are potentially quite numerous, and a

multiple solutions are likely (Voss, 2006). Extreme examples are problems like “How can the world achieve lasting

peace?” or “How can teachers insure that students learn?”

By these definitions, the nine-dot problem is relatively well-structured—though not completely. Most of the

information needed for a solution is provided in Scene #1: there are nine dots shown and instructions given to draw

four lines. But not all necessary information was given: students needed to consider lines that were longer than

Educational Psychology 189 A Global Text

9. Facilitating complex thinking

implied in the original statement of the problem. Students had to “think outside the box”, as Willem said—in this

case, literally.

When a problem is well-structured, so are its solution procedures likely to be as well. A well-defined procedure

for solving a particular kind of problem is often called an algorithm; examples are the procedures for multiplying

or dividing two numbers or the instructions for using a computer (Leiserson, et al., 2001). Algorithms are only

effective when a problem is very well-structured and there is no question about whether the algorithm is an

appropriate choice for the problem. In that situation it pretty much guarantees a correct solution. They do not work

well, however, with ill-structured problems, where they are ambiguities and questions about how to proceed or even

about precisely what the problem is about. In those cases it is more effective to use heuristics, which are general

strategies—“rules of thumb”, so to speak—that do not always work, but often do, or that provide at least partial

solutions. When beginning research for a term paper, for example, a useful heuristic is to scan the library catalogue

for titles that look relevant. There is no guarantee that this strategy will yield the books most needed for the paper,

but the strategy works enough of the time to make it worth trying.

In the nine-dot problem, most students began in Scene #1 with a simple algorithm that can be stated like this:

“Draw one line, then draw another, and another, and another”. Unfortunately this simple procedure did not

produce a solution, so they had to find other strategies for a solution. Three alternatives are described in Scenes #3

(for Alicia) and 4 (for Willem and Rachel). Of these, Willem’s response resembled a heuristic the most: he knew

from experience that a good general strategy that often worked for such problems was to suspect a deception or

trick in how the problem was originally stated. So he set out to question what the teacher had meant by the word

line, and came up with an acceptable solution as a result.

Common obstacles to solving problems

The example also illustrates two common problems that sometimes happen during problem solving. One of

these is functional fixedness: a tendency to regard the functions of objects and ideas as fixed (German & Barrett,

2005). Over time, we get so used to one particular purpose for an object that we overlook other uses. We may think

of a dictionary, for example, as necessarily something to verify spellings and definitions, but it also can function as a

gift, a doorstop, or a footstool. For students working on the nine-dot matrix described in the last section, the notion

of “drawing” a line was also initially fixed; they assumed it to be connecting dots but not extending lines beyond the

dots. Functional fixedness sometimes is also called response set, the tendency for a person to frame or think

about each problem in a series in the same way as the previous problem, even when doing so is not appropriate to

later problems. In the example of the nine-dot matrix described above, students often tried one solution after

another, but each solution was constrained by a set response not to extend any line beyond the matrix.

Functional fixedness and the response set are obstacles in problem representation, the way that a person

understands and organizes information provided in a problem. If information is misunderstood or used

inappropriately, then mistakes are likely—if indeed the problem can be solved at all. With the nine-dot matrix

problem, for example, construing the instruction to draw four lines as meaning “draw four lines entirely within the

matrix” means that the problem simply could not be solved. For another, consider this problem: “The number of

water lilies on a lake doubles each day. Each water lily covers exactly one square foot. If it takes 100 days for the

lilies to cover the lake exactly, how many days does it take for the lilies to cover exactly half of the lake?” If you

190

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

think that the size of the lilies affects the solution to this problem, you have not represented the problem correctly.

Information about lily size is not relevant to the solution, and only serves to distract from the truly crucial

information, the fact that the lilies double their coverage each day. (The answer, incidentally, is that the lake is half

covered in 99 days; can you think why?)

Strategies to assist problem solving

Just as there are cognitive obstacles to problem solving, there are also general strategies that help the process be

successful, regardless of the specific content of a problem (Thagard, 2005). One helpful strategy is problem

analysis—identifying the parts of the problem and working on each part separately. Analysis is especially useful

when a problem is ill-structured. Consider this problem, for example: “Devise a plan to improve bicycle

transportation in the city.” Solving this problem is easier if you identify its parts or component subproblems, such

as (1) installing bicycle lanes on busy streets, (2) educating cyclists and motorists to ride safely, (3) fixing potholes

on streets used by cyclists, and (4) revising traffic laws that interfere with cycling. Each separate subproblem is

more manageable than the original, general problem. The solution of each subproblem contributes the solution of

the whole, though of course is not equivalent to a whole solution.

Another helpful strategy is working backward from a final solution to the originally stated problem. This

approach is especially helpful when a problem is well-structured but also has elements that are distracting or

misleading when approached in a forward, normal direction. The water lily problem described above is a good

example: starting with the day when all the lake is covered (Day 100), ask what day would it therefore be half

covered (by the terms of the problem, it would have to be the day before, or Day 99). Working backward in this case

encourages reframing the extra information in the problem (i. e. the size of each water lily) as merely distracting,

not as crucial to a solution.

A third helpful strategy is analogical thinking—using knowledge or experiences with similar features or

structures to help solve the problem at hand (Bassok, 2003). In devising a plan to improve bicycling in the city, for

example, an analogy of cars with bicycles is helpful in thinking of solutions: improving conditions for both vehicles

requires many of the same measures (improving the roadways, educating drivers). Even solving simpler, more basic

problems is helped by considering analogies. A first grade student can partially decode unfamiliar printed words by

analogy to words he or she has learned already. If the child cannot yet read the word screen, for example, he can

note that part of this word looks similar to words he may already know, such as seen or green, and from this

observation derive a clue about how to read the word screen. Teachers can assist this process, as you might expect,

by suggesting reasonable, helpful analogies for students to consider.

Broad instructional strategies that stimulate complex thinking

Because the forms of thinking just described—critical thinking, creativity and problem solving—are broad and

important educationally, it is not surprising that educators have identified strategies to encourage their

development. Some of the possibilities are shown in Table 24 and group several instructional strategies along two

dimensions: how much the strategy is student-centered and how much a strategy depends on group interaction. It

should be emphasized that the two-way classification in Table 24 is not very precise, but it gives a useful framework

for understanding the options available for planning and implementing instruction. The more important of the two

dimensions in the table is the first one—the extent to which an instructional strategy is either directed by the

Educational Psychology 191 A Global Text

9. Facilitating complex thinking

teacher or initiated by students. We take a closer look at this dimension in the next part of this chapter, followed by

discussion of group-oriented teaching strategies.

Table 24: Major instructional strategies grouped by level of teacher direction and student focus

Directed by student(s) more

Emphasizes groups

somewhat more

Cooperative learning

Inquiry

Discovery learning

Self-reflection

Independent study

Concept maps

Lectures

Direct instruction

Madeline Hunter's

“Effective Teaching”

Mastery learning

Textbook readings

Advance organizers

Outlining

Recalling, relating, and

elaborating

Emphasizes

individuals somewhat

more

Directed by teacher more

Definitions of Terms in Table 8.1

Lecture Telling or explaining previously organized information—usually to a group

Assigned reading Reading, usually individually, of previously organized information

Advance organizers Brief overview, either verbally or graphically, of material about to be covered in a

lecture or text

Outlining Writing important points of a lecture or reading, usually in a hierarchical format

Taking notes Writing important points of a lecture or reading, often organized according to the

learning needs of an individual student

Concept maps Graphic depiction of relationships among a set of concepts, terms, or ideas;

usually organized by the student, but not always

Madeline Hunter’s

“Effective Teaching”

A set of strategies that emphasizes clear presentation of goals, the explanation

and modeling of tasks to students and careful monitoring of students’ progress

toward the goals

192

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Teacher-directed instruction

As the name implies, teacher-directed instruction includes any strategies initiated and guided primarily by the

teacher. A classic example is exposition or lecturing (simply telling or explaining important information to

students) combined with assigning reading from texts. But teacher-directed instruction also includes strategies that

involve more active response from students, such as encouraging students to elaborate on new knowledge or to

explain how new information relates to prior knowledge. Whatever their form, teacher-directed instructional

methods normally include the organizing of information on behalf of students, even if teachers also expect students

to organize it further on their own. Sometimes, therefore, teacher-directed methods are thought of as transmitting

knowledge from teacher to student as clearly and efficiently as possible, even if they also require mental work on

the part of the student.

Lectures and readings

Lectures and readings are traditional staples of educators, particularly with older students (including university

students). At their best, they pre-organize information so that (at least in theory) the student only has to remember

what was said in the lecture or written in the text in order to begin understanding it (Exley & Dennick, 2004). Their

limitation is the ambiguity of the responses they require: listening and reading are by nature quiet and stationary,

and do not in themselves indicate whether a student is comprehending or even attending to the material. Educators

sometimes complain that “students are too passive” during lectures or when reading. But physical quietness is

intrinsic to these activities, not to the students who do them. A book just sits still, after all, unless a student makes

an effort to read it, and a lecture may not be heard unless a student makes the effort to listen to it.

Advance organizers

In spite of these problems, there are strategies for making lectures and readings effective. A teacher can be

especially careful about organizing information for students, and she can turn part of the mental work over to

students themselves. An example of the first approach is the use of advance organizers—brief overviews or

introductions to new material before the material itself is presented (Ausubel, 1978). Textbook authors (including

ourselves) often try deliberately to insert periodic advance organizers to introduce new sections or chapters in the

text. When used in a lecture, advance organizers are usually statements in the form of brief introductory remarks,

though sometimes diagrams showing relationships among key ideas can also serve the same purpose (Robinson, et

al., 2003). Whatever their form, advance organizers partially organize the material on behalf of the students, so that

they know where to put it all, so to speak, as they learn them in more detail.

Recalling and relating prior knowledge

Another strategy for improving teacher-directed instruction is to encourage students to relate the new material

to prior familiar knowledge. When one of us (Kelvin) first learned a foreign language (in his case French), for

example, he often noticed similarities between French and English vocabulary. A French word for picture, for

example, was image, spelled exactly as it is in English. The French word for splendid was splendide, spelled almost

the same as in English, though not quite. Relating the French vocabulary to English vocabulary helped in learning

and remembering the French.

As children and youth become more experienced in their academics, they tend to relate new information to

previously learned information more frequently and automatically (Goodwin, 1999; Oakhill, Hartt, & Samols,

Educational Psychology 193 A Global Text

9. Facilitating complex thinking

2005). But teachers can also facilitate students’ use of this strategy. When presenting new concepts or ideas, the

teacher can relate them to previously learned ideas deliberately—essentially modeling a memory strategy that

students learn to use for themselves. In a science class, for example, she can say, “This is another example of…,

which we studied before”; in social studies she can say, “Remember what we found out last time about the growth of

the railroads? We saw that…”

If students are relatively young or are struggling academically, it is especially important to remind them of their

prior knowledge. Teachers can periodically ask questions like “What do you already know about this topic?” or

“How will your new knowledge about this topic change what you know already?” Whatever the age of students,

connecting new with prior knowledge is easier with help from someone more knowledgeable, such as the teacher.

When learning algorithms for multiplication, for example, students may not at first see how multiplication is

related to addition processes which they probably learned previously (Burns, 2001). But if a teacher takes time to

explain the relationship and to give students time to explore it, then the new skill of multiplication may be learned

more easily.

Elaborating information

Elaborating new information means asking questions about the new material, inferring ideas and relationships

among the new concepts. Such strategies are closely related to the strategy of recalling prior knowledge as discussed

above: elaboration enriches the new information and connects it to other knowledge. In this sense elaboration

makes the new learning more meaningful and less arbitrary.

A teacher can help students use elaboration by modeling this behavior. The teacher can interrupt his or her

explanation of an idea, for example, by asking how it relates to other ideas, or by speculating about where the new

concept or idea may lead. He or she can also encourage students to do the same, and even give students questions

to guide their thinking. When giving examples of a concept, for example, a teacher can hold back from offering all of

the examples, and instead ask students to think of additional examples themselves. The same tactic can work with

assigned readings; if the reading includes examples, the teacher can instruct students to find or make up additional

examples of their own.

Organizing new information

There are many ways to organize new information that are especially well-suited to teacher-directed instruction.

A common way is simply to ask students to outline information read in a text or heard in a lecture. Outlining

works especially well when the information is already organized somewhat hierarchically into a series of main

topics, each with supporting subtopics or subpoints. Outlining is basically a form of the more general strategy of

taking notes, or writing down key ideas and terms from a reading or lecture. Research studies find that that the

precise style or content of notes is less important that the quantity of notes taken: more detail is usually better than

less (Ward & Tatsukawa, 2003). Written notes insure that a student thinks about the material not only while

writing it down, but also when reading the notes later. These benefits are especially helpful when students are

relatively inexperienced at school learning in general (as in the earlier grade levels), or relatively inexperienced

about a specific topic or content in particular. Not surprisingly, such students may also need more guidance than

usual about what and how to write notes. It can be helpful for the teacher to provide a note-taking guide, like the

ones shown in Exhibit 11.

194

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Notes on Science Experiment

1. Purpose of the experiment (in one sentence):

2. Equipment needed (list each item and define

any special terms):

1)

2)

3)

4)

3. Procedure used (be specific!):

4.

4. Results (include each measurement, rounded to

the nearest integer):

Observation #1

Observation #2

Observation #3

Observation #4

Average

measurement, #1-4:

Guide to Notes About Tale of Two Cities:

1. Main characters (list and describe in just a few

words):

a)

b)

c)

d)

2. Setting of the story (time and place):

3. Unfamiliar vocabulary in the story (list and

define):

a)

b)

c)

d)

4. Plot (write down only the main events):

a)

b)

c)

d)

5. Theme (or underlying “message”) of the story:

Exhibit 13: Two note taking guides

In learning expository material, another helpful strategy—one that is more visually oriented—is to make

concept maps, or diagrams of the connections among concepts or ideas. Exhibit 10 shows concept maps made by

two individuals that graphically depict how a key idea, child development, relates to learning and education. One of

Educational Psychology 195 A Global Text

9. Facilitating complex thinking

the maps was drawn by a classroom teacher and the other by a university professor of psychology (Seifert, 1991).

They suggest possible differences in how the two individuals think about children and their development. Not

surprisingly, the teacher gave more prominence to practical concerns (for example, classroom learning and child

abuse), and the professor gave more prominence to theoretical ones (for example, Erik Erikson and Piaget). The

differences suggest that these two people may have something different in mind when they use the same term, child

development. The differences have the potential to create misunderstandings between them (Seifert, 1999; Super &

Harkness, 2003). By the same token, the two maps also suggest what each person might need to learn in order to

achieve better understanding of the other person’s thinking and ideas.

Exhibit 14: Maps of personal definitions of “child development”

196

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Mastery learning

This term refers to an instructional approach in which all students learn material to an identically high level,

even if some students require more time than others to do so (Gentile, 2004). In mastery learning, the teacher

directs learning, though sometimes only in the sense of finding, writing, and orchestrating specific modules or units

for students to learn. In one typical mastery learning program, the teacher introduces a few new concepts or topics

through a brief lecture or teacher-led demonstration. Then she gives an ungraded assignment or test immediately

in order to assess how well students have learned the material, and which ones still need help. The students who

have already learned the unit are given enrichment activities. Those needing more help are provided individual

tutoring or additional self-guiding materials that clarify the initial content; they work until they have in fact

mastered the content (hence the name mastery learning). At that point students take another test or do another

assignment to show that they have in fact learned the material to the expected high standard. When the system is

working well, all students end up with high scores or grades, although usually some take longer to do so than

others.

As you might suspect, mastery learning poses two challenges. The first is ethical: is it really fair to give

enrichment only to faster students and remediation only to slower students? This practice could deteriorate into

continually providing the fast with an interesting education, while continually providing the slow only with boring,

repetitious material. In using the approach, therefore, it is important to make all materials interesting, whether

enrichment or remedial. It is also important to make sure that the basic learning goals of each unit are truly

important—even crucial—for everyone to learn, so that even slower individuals spend their time well.

The other challenge of mastery learning is more practical: the approach makes strong demands for detailed,

highly organized curriculum. If the approach is to work, the teacher must either locate such a curriculum, write one

herself, or assemble a suitable mixture of published and self-authored materials. However the curriculum is

created, the end result has to be a program filled with small units of study as well as ample enrichment and

remedial materials. Sometimes providing these practical requirements can be challenging. But not always: some

subjects (like mathematics) lend themselves to detailed, sequential organization especially well. In many cases, too,

commercial publishers have produced curricula already organized for use in mastery learning programs (Fox,

2004).

Direct instruction

Although the term direct instruction is sometimes a synonym for teacher-directed instruction, more often it

refers to a version of mastery learning that is highly scripted, meaning that it not only organizes the curriculum into

small modules or units as described above, but also dictates how teachers should teach and sometimes even the

words they should speak (Adams & Engelmann, 1996; Magliaro, Lockee, & Burton, 2005). Direct instruction

programs are usually based on a mix of ideas from behaviorism and cognitive theories of learning. In keeping with

behaviorism, the teacher is supposed to praise students immediately and explicitly when they give a correct answer.

In keeping with cognitive theory, she is supposed to state learning objectives in advance of teaching them

(providing a sort of mini-advance organizer), provide frequent reviews of materials, and check deliberately on how

well students are learning. Direct instruction usually also introduces material in small, logical steps, and calls for

plenty of time for students to practice.

Educational Psychology 197 A Global Text

9. Facilitating complex thinking

Direct instruction programs share one of the challenges of other mastery learning approaches: because they hold

all students to the same high standard of achievement, they must deal with differences in how long students require

to reach the standard. But direct instruction has an additional challenge, in that they often rely on small-group

interaction more heavily than other mastery learning programs, and use self-guiding materials less. This difference

has the benefit that direct instruction works especially well with younger students (especially kindergarten through

third grade), who may have limited skills at working alone for extended periods. The challenge is that reliance on

small-group interaction can make it impractical to use direct instruction with an entire class or for an entire school

day. In spite of these limits, however, research has found direct instruction to be very effective in teaching basic

skills such as early reading and arithmetic (Adams & Engelmann, 1996).

Madeline Hunter’s effective teaching model

A number of direct instruction strategies have been combined by Madeline Hunter into a single, relatively

comprehensive approach that she calls mastery teaching (not to be confused with the related term mastery

learning) or the effective teaching model (M. Hunter, 1982; R. Hunter, 2004). Important features of the model

are summarized in Table 25. As you can see, the features span all phases of contact with students—before, during,

and after lessons.

Table 25: Madeline Hunter's “Effective Teaching Model”

Prepare students to learn.

• Make good use of time at the beginning of a lesson or activity, when attention is best

• Direct students' attention to what lies ahead in a lesson—for example, by offering “advance organizers”

• Explain lesson objectives explicitly

Present information clearly and explicitly.

• Set a basic structure to the lesson and stay with it throughout

• Use familiar terms and examples

• Be concise

Check for understanding and give guided practice.

• Ask questions that everyone responds to—for example, “Raise your hand if you think the answer is X”

• Invite choral responses—for example, “Is this a correct answer or not?”

• Sample individuals' understanding—for example, “Barry, what's your example of X?”

Provide for independent practice.

• Work through the first few exercises or problems together

• Keep independent practice periods brief and intersperse with discussions that offer feedback

Source: R. Hunter, 2004

198

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

What happens even before a lesson begins? Like many forms of teacher-directed instruction, the effective

teaching model requires curricula and learning goals that are tightly organized and divisible into small parts, ideas,

or skills. In teaching about photosynthesis, for example, the teacher (or at least her curriculum) needs to identify

the basic elements that contribute to this process, and how they relate to each other. With photosynthesis, the

elements include the sun, plants, animals, chlorophyll, oxygen produced by plants and consumed by animals, and

carbon dioxide that produced by animals and consumed by plants. The roles of these elements need to be identified

and expressed at a level appropriate for the students. With advanced science students, oxygen, chlorophyll, and

carbon dioxide may be expressed as part of complex chemical reactions; with first-grade students, though, they may

be expressed simply as parts of a process akin to breathing or respiration.

Once this analysis of the curriculum has been done, the Hunter's effective teaching model requires making the

most of the lesson time by creating an anticipatory set, which is an activity that focuses or orients the attention of

students to the upcoming content. Creating an anticipatory set may consist, for example, of posing one or more

questions about students’ everyday knowledge or knowledge of prior lessons. In teaching about differences between

fruits and vegetables, the teacher could start by asking: “If you are making a salad strictly of fruit, which of these

would be OK to use: apple, tomato, cucumber, or orange?” As the lesson proceeds, information needs to be offered

in short, logical pieces, using language as familiar as possible to the students. Examples should be plentiful and

varied: if the purpose is to define and distinguish fruits and vegetables, for example, then features defining each

group should be presented singularly or at most just a few at a time, with clear-cut examples presented of each

feature. Sometimes models or analogies also help to explain examples. A teacher can say: “Think of a fruit as a sort

of ‘decoration’ on the plant, because if you pick it, the plant will go on living.” But models can also mislead students

if they are not used thoughtfully, since they may contain features that differ from the original concepts. In likening

a fruit to a decoration, for example, students may overlook the essential role of fruit in plant reproduction, or think

that lettuce qualifies as a fruit, since picking a few lettuce leaves does not usually kill a lettuce plant.

Throughout a lesson, the teacher repeatedly checks for understanding by asking questions that call for

active thinking on the part of students. One way is to require all students to respond somehow, either with an actual

choral response (speaking in unison together), another way with a non-verbal signal like raising hands to indicate

answers to questions. In teaching about fruits and vegetables, for example, a teacher can ask, “Here’s a list of fruits

and vegetables. As I point to each one, raise your hand if it’s a fruit, but not if it’s a vegetable.” Or she can ask:

“Here’s a list of fruits and vegetables. Say together what each on is as I point to it; you say ‘fruit’ or ‘vegetable’,

whichever applies.” Even though some students may hide their ignorance by letting more knowledgeable

classmates do the responding, the general level or quality of response can still give a rough idea of how well

students are understanding. These checks can be supplemented, of course, with questions addressed to individuals,

or with questions to which individuals must respond briefly in writing. A teacher can ask everyone, “Give me an

example of one fruit and one vegetable”, and then call on individuals to answer. She can also say: “I want everyone

to make a list with two columns, one listing all the fruits you can think of and the other listing all the vegetables you

can think of.”

As a lesson draws to a close, the teacher arranges for students to have further independent practice. The

point of the practice is not to explore new material or ideas, but to consolidate or strengthen the recent learning. At

the end of a lesson about long division, for example, the teacher can make a transition to independent practice by

Educational Psychology 199 A Global Text

9. Facilitating complex thinking

providing a set of additional problems similar to the ones she explained during the lesson. After working one or two

with students, she can turn the rest of the task over to the students to practice on their own. But note that even

though the practice is supposedly “independent”, students’ understanding still has be checked frequently. A long set

of practice problems therefore needs to be broken up into small subsets of problems, and written or oral feedback

offered periodically.

What are the limits of teacher-directed instruction?

Whatever the grade level, most subjects taught in schools have at least some features, skills, or topics that

benefit from direct instruction. Even subjects usually considered “creative” can benefit from a direct approach at

times: to draw, sing, or write a poem, for example, requires skills that may be easier to learn if presented

sequentially in small units with frequent feedback from a teacher. Research supports the usefulness of teacher-

directed instruction for a variety of educational contexts when it is designed well and implemented as intended

(Rosenshine & Mesister,1995; Good & Brophy, 2004). Teachers themselves also tend to support the approach in

principle (Demant & Yates, 2003).

But there are limits to its usefulness. Some are the practical ones are pointed out above. Teacher-directed

instruction, whatever the form, requires well-organized units of instruction in advance of when students are to

learn. Such units may not always be available, and it may not be realistic to expect busy teachers to devise their

own. Other limits of direct instruction have more to do with the very nature of learning. Some critics argue that

organizing material on behalf of the students encourages students to be passive—an ironic and undesirable result if

true (Kohn, 2000, 2006). According to this criticism, the mere fact that a curriculum or unit of study is constructed

by a teacher (or other authority) makes some students think that they should not bother seeking information

actively on their own, but wait for it to arrive of its own accord. In support of this argument, critics point to the fact

that direct instruction approaches sometimes contradict their own premises by requiring students to do a bit of

cognitive organizational work of their own. This happens, for example, when a mastery learning program provides

enrichment material to faster students to work on independently; in that case the teacher may be involved in the

enrichment activities only minimally.

Criticisms like these have led to additional instructional approaches that rely more fully on students to seek and

organize their own learning. In the next section we discuss some of these options. As you will see, student-centered

models of learning do solve certain problems of teacher-directed instruction, but they also have problems of their

own.

Student-centered models of learning

Student-centered models of learning shift some of the responsibility for directing and organizing learning from

the teacher to the student. Being student-centered does not mean, however, that a teacher gives up organizational

and leadership responsibilities completely. It only means a relative shift in the teacher’s role, toward one with more

emphasis on guiding students’ self-chosen directions. As we explained earlier in this chapter, teacher-directed

strategies do not take over responsibility for students’ learning completely; no matter how much a teacher

structures or directs learning, the students still have responsibility for working and expending effort to comprehend

new material. By the same token, student-centered models of learning do not mean handing over all organizational

200

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

work of instruction to students. The teacher is still the most knowledgeable member of the class, and still has both

the opportunity and the responsibility to guide learning in directions that are productive.

As you might suspect, therefore, teacher-directed and student-centered approaches to instruction may overlap

in practice. You can see the overlap clearly, for example, in two instructional strategies commonly thought of as

student-centered, independent study and self-reflection. In independent study, as the name implies, a student

works alone a good deal of the time, consulting with a teacher only occasionally. Independent study may be

student-centered in the sense that the student may be learning a topic or skill—an exotic foreign language, for

example—that is personally interesting. But the opposite may also be true: the student may be learning a topic or

skill that a teacher or an official school curriculum has directed the student to learn—a basic subject for which the

student is missing a credit, for example. Either way, though, the student will probably need guidance, support, and

help from a teacher. In this sense even independent study always contain elements of teacher-direction.

Similarly, self-reflection refers to thinking about beliefs and experiences in order to clarify their personal

meaning and importance. In school it can be practiced in a number of ways: for example by keeping diaries or logs

of learning or reading, or by retelling stories of important experiences or incidents in a student’s life, or by creating

concept maps like the ones described earlier in this chapter. Whatever form it takes, self-reflection by definition

happens inside a single student’s mind, and in this sense is always directed by the student. Yet most research on

self-reflection finds that self-reflection only works well when it involves and generates responses and interaction

with other students or with a teacher (Seifert, 1999; Kuit, Reay, & Freeman, 2001). To be fully self-reflective,

students need to have access to more than their existing base of knowledge and ideas—more than what they know

already. In one study about students’ self-reflections of cultural and racial prejudices (Gay & Kirkland, 2003), for

example, the researchers found that students tended to reflect on these problems in relatively shallow ways if they

worked on their own. It was not particularly effective to write about prejudice in a journal that no one read except

themselves, or to describe beliefs in a class discussion in which neither the teacher nor classmates commented or

challenged the beliefs. Much more effective in both cases was for the teacher to respond thoughtfully to students’

reflective comments. In this sense the use of self-reflection, like independent study, required elements of teacher-

direction to be successful.

How might a teacher emphasize students’ responsibility for directing and organizing their own learning? The

alternatives are numerous, as they are for teacher-directed strategies, so we can only sample some of them here. We

concentrate on ones that are relatively well known and used most widely, and especially on two: inquiry learning

and cooperative learning.

Inquiry learning

Inquiry learning stands the usual advice about expository (lecture-style) teaching on its head: instead of

presenting well-organized knowledge to students, the teacher (or sometimes fellow students) pose thoughtful

questions intended to stimulate discussion and investigation by students. The approach has been described, used,

and discussed by educators literally for decades, though sometimes under other names, including inquiry method

(Postman & Weingartner, 1969), discovery learning (Bruner, 1960/2006), or progressive education (Dewey, 1933;

Martin, 2003). For convenience, we will stay with the term inquiry learning.

Educational Psychology 201 A Global Text

9. Facilitating complex thinking

The questions that begin a cycle of inquiry learning may be posed either by the teacher or by students

themselves. Their content depends not only on the general subject area being studied, but also on the interests

which students themselves have expressed. In elementary-level science, for example, a question might be “Why do

leaves fall off trees when winter comes?” In high school social studies classes, it might be “Why do nations get into

conflict?” The teacher avoids answering such questions directly, even if asked to do so. Instead she encourages

students to investigate the questions themselves, for example by elaborating on students’ ideas and by asking

further questions based on students’ initial comments. Since students’ comments can not be predicted precisely, the

approach is by nature flexible. The initial questioning helps students to create and clarify questions which they

consider worthy of further investigation. Discussing questions about leaves falling off trees, for example, can

prompt students to observe trees in the autumn or to locate books and references that discuss or explain the biology

of tress and leaves.

But inquiry is not limited to particular grade levels or topics. If initial questions in a high school social studies

class have been about why nations get into conflict, for example, the resulting discussions can lead to investigating

the history of past wars and the history of peace-keeping efforts around the world. Whether the topic is high school

social studies or elementary school biology, the specific direction of investigations is influenced heavily by students,

but with assistance from the teacher to insure that the students’ initiatives are productive. When all goes well, the

inquiry and resulting investigations benefit students in two ways. The first is that students (perhaps obviously)

learn new knowledge from their investigations. The second is that students practice a constructive, motivating way

of learning, one applicable to a variety of problems and tasks, both in school and out.

Cooperative learning

Even though inquiry-oriented discussion and investigation benefits when it involves the teacher, it can also be

useful for students to work together somewhat independently, relying on a teacher’s guidance only indirectly.

Working with peers is a major feature of cooperative learning (sometimes also called collaborative learning). In

this approach, students work on a task in groups and often are rewarded either partially or completely for the

success of the group as a whole. Aspects of cooperative learning have been part of education for a long time; some

form of cooperation has always been necessary to participate on school sports teams, for example, or to produce a

student-run school newspaper. What is a bit newer is using cooperative or collaborative activities systematically to

facilitate the learning of a range of educational goals central to the academic curriculum (Prince, 2004).

Even though teachers usually value cooperation in students, circumstances at school can sometimes reduce

students’ incentives to show it. The traditional practice of assessing students individually, for example, can set the

stage for competition over grades, and cultural and other forms of diversity can sometimes inhibit individuals from

helping each other spontaneously. Strategies exist, however, for reducing such barriers so that students truly

benefit from each other’s presence, and are more likely to feel like sharing their skills and knowledge. Here, for

example, are several key features that make cooperative learning work well (Johnson & Johnson, 1998; Smith, et

al., 2005):

• Students need time and a place to talk and work together. This may sound obvious, but it can be

overlooked if time in class becomes crowded with other tasks and activities, or with interruptions related to

202

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

school (like assemblies) but not to the classroom. It is never enough simply to tell students to work

together, only to leave them wondering how or when they are to do so.

• Students need skills at working together. As an adult, you may feel relatively able to work with a variety of

partners on a group task. The same assumption cannot be made, however, about younger individuals,

whether teenagers or children. Some students may get along with a variety of partners, but others may not.

Many will benefit from advice and coaching about how to focus on the tasks at hand, rather than on the

personalities of their partners.

• Assessment of activities should hold both the group and the individuals accountable for success. If a final

mark for a project goes only to the group as a whole, then freeloading is possible: some members may not

do their share of the work and may be rewarded more than they deserve. Others may be rewarded less than

they deserve. If, on the other hand, a final grade for a group project goes only to each member’s individual

contribution to a group project, then overspecialization can occur: individuals have no real incentive to

work together, and cooperative may deteriorate into a set of smaller individual projects (Slavin, 1994).

• Students need to believe in the value and necessity of cooperation. Collaboration will not occur if students

privately assume that their partners have little to contribute to their personal success. Social prejudices

from the wider society—like racial bias or gender sexism, for example—can creep into the operations of

cooperative groups, causing some members to be ignored unfairly while others are overvalued. Teachers

can help reduce these problems in two ways: first by pointing out and explaining that a diversity of talents

is necessary for success on a group project, and second by pointing out to the group how undervalued

individuals are contributing to the overall project (Cohen, Brody, & Sapon-Shevin, 2004).

As these comments imply, cooperative learning does not happen automatically, and requires monitoring and

support by the teacher. Some activities may not lend themselves to cooperative work, particularly if every member

of the group is doing essentially the same task. Giving everyone in a group the same set of arithmetic problems to

work on collaboratively, for example, is a formula for cooperative failure: either the most skilled students do the

work for others (freeloading) or else members simply divide up the problems among themselves in order to reduce

their overall work (overspecialization). A better choice for a cooperative task is one that clearly requires a diversity

of skills, what some educators call a rich group work task (Cohen, Brody, & Sapon-Shevin, 2004). Preparing a

presentation about medieval castles, for example, might require (a) writing skill to create a report, (b) dramatic skill

to put on a skit and (c) artistic talent to create a poster. Although a few students may have all of these skills, more

are likely to have only one, and they are therefore likely to need and want their fellow group members’

participation.

Examples of cooperative and collaborative learning

Although this description may make the requirements for cooperative learning sound somewhat precise, there

are actually a variety of ways to implement it in practice. Error: Reference source not found summarizes several of

them. As you can see, the strategies vary in the number of how many students they involve, the prior organization

or planning provided by the teacher, and the amount of class time they normally require.

Table 26: Strategies for encouraging cooperative learning

Educational Psychology 203 A Global Text

9. Facilitating complex thinking

Strategy Type of groups

involved:

What the teacher

does:

What the students

do:

Think-pair-share

(Lyman, 1981)

Pairs of students,

sometimes linked to one

other pair

Teacher poses initial

problem or question.

First, students think

individually of the answer;

second, they share their

thinking with partner;

third, the partnership

shares their thinking with

another partnership.

Jigsaw classroom,

version #1

(Aronson, et al., 2001)

5-6 students per group,

and 5-6 groups overall

Teacher assigns

students to groups and

assigns one aspect of a

complex problem to each

group.

Students in each group

work together to become

experts in their particular

aspect of the problem;

later the expert groups

disband, and form new

groups containing one

student from each of the

former expert groups.

Jigsaw classroom,

version #2

(Slavin, 1994)

4-5 students per group,

and 4-5 groups overall

Teacher assigns

students to groups and

assigns each group to

study or learn about the

same entire complex

problem.

Students initially work

in groups to learn about

the entire problem; later

the groups disband and

reform as expert groups,

with each group focusing

on a selected aspect of the

general problem; still later

the expert groups disband

and the original general

groups reform to learn

what the expert students

can now add to their

general understanding.

STAD (Student-Teams-

Achievement Divisions)

(Slavin, 1994)

4-5 students per team

(or group)

Teacher presents a

lesson or unit to the entire

class, and later tests them

on it; grades individuals

based partly on

Students work together

to insure that team mates

improve their performance

as much as possible.

Students take tests as

204

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

individuals’ and the team’s

improvement, not just on

absolute level of

performance.

individuals.

Project-Based Learning

(Katz, 2000)

Various numbers of

students, depending on the

complexity of the project,

up to and including the

entire class

Teacher or students

pose a question or problem

of interest to other

students; teacher assists

students to clarify their

interests and to make

plans to investigate the

question further.

Students work together

for extended periods to

investigate the original

question or problem;

project leads eventually to

a presentation, written

report, or other product.

Instructional strategies: an abundance of choices

Looking broadly at this chapter, you can see that choices among instructional strategies are numerous indeed,

and that deciding among them depends on the forms of thinking that you want to encourage, the extent to which

ideas or skills need to be organized by you to be understood by students, and the extent to which students need to

take responsibility for directing their own learning. Although you may have personal preferences among possible

instructional strategies, the choice will also be guided by the uniqueness of each situation of teaching—with its

particular students, grade-level, content, and purposes. If you need to develop students’ problem solving skills, for

example, there are strategies that are especially well suited for this purpose; we described some (see, “Problem

solving strategies” in this chapter). If you need to organize complex information so that students do not become

confused by it, there are effective ways of doing so. If you want the students to take as much initiative as possible in

organizing their own learning, this too can be done.

Yet having this knowledge is still not enough to teach well. What is still needed are ideas or principles for

deciding what to teach. In this chapter we have still not addressed an obvious question: How do I find or devise

goals for my teaching and for my students’ learning? And assuming that I can determine the goals, where can I find

resources that help students to meet them?

Chapter summary

Teaching involves numerous instructional strategies, which are decisions and actions designed to facilitate

learning. The choice of strategies depends partly on the forms of thinking intended for students—whether the goal

is for students to think critically, for example, or to think creatively, or to solve problems. A fundamental decision in

choosing instructional strategies is how much to emphasize teacher-directed instruction, as compared to student-

centered models of learning. Teacher-directed strategies of instruction include lectures and readings (expository

teaching), mastery learning, scripted or direct instruction, and complex teacher-directed approaches such as

Madeline Hunter’s effective teaching model. Student-centered models of learning include independent study,

student self-reflection, inquiry learning, and various forms of cooperative or collaborative learning. Although for

Educational Psychology 205 A Global Text

9. Facilitating complex thinking

some students, curriculum content and learning goals may lend themselves toward one particular type of

instruction, teaching is often a matter of combining different strategies appropriately and creatively.

On the Internet

This web page lists over 900 instructional strategies—

about ten times as many as in this chapter! The strategies are arranged alphabetically and range from simple to

complex. For many strategies there are links to other web pages with more complete explanations and advice for

use. This is a good page if you have heard of a strategy but want to find out its definition quickly.

Like the web page above, this one also describes

instructional strategies. It includes fewer (about 200), but they are discussed in more detail and organized

according to major categories or types of strategies—a good feature if you have a general idea of what sort of

strategy you are looking for, but are not sure of precisely which one.

Key termsAdvance organizers

Algorithms Analogical thinking

Collaborative learningConcept map

Convergent thinkingCooperative learning

Creative thinkingCritical thinking

Divergent thinkingEffective teaching model

Freeloading Functional fixedness

Heuristics Ill-structured problem

Independent study

Instructional strategiesLectures

Mastery learningOverspecialization

Problem analysisProblem representation

Problem-solvingResponse set

Self-reflectionStudent-centered models of learning

Teacher-directed instructionTransfer

Well-structured problemWorking backward

References

Aronson, E. (2001). In the jigsaw classroom. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

Benson, B. & Barnett, S. (2005). Student-led conferencing using showcase portfolios. Thousand Oaks, CA:

Corwin Press.

Black, P., Harrison, C., Lee C., Marshall, B., & William, D. (2004). Working inside the black box: Assessment

for learning in the classroom. Phi Delta Kappan, 86(1), 8-21.

Bothmer, S. (2003). Creating the peaceable classroom. Tuscon, AZ: Zephyr Press.

Britt, T. (2005). Effects of identity-relevance and task difficulty on task motivation, stress, and performance.

Motivation and Emotion, 29(3), 189-202.

Brophy, J. (2004). Motivating students to learn, 2nd edition. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Brown, D. (2004). Urban teachers’ professed classroom management strategies: Reflections of culturally

responsive teaching. Urban Education, 39(3), 266-289.

206

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Brookfield, S. (2006). The skillful teacher: On technique, trust, and responsiveness in the classroom, 2nd

edition. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Chesebro, J. (2003). Effects of teacher clarity and nonverbal immediacy on student learning, receiver

apprehension, and affect. Communication Education, 52(2), 135-147.

Cooper, P. & Simonds, C. (2003). Communication for the classroom teacher, 7th edition. Boston: Allyn &

Bacon.

Cronbach, L. & Snow, R. (1977). Aptitudes and instructional methods: A handbook for research on

interaction. New York: Irvington.

Crutsinger, C., Knight, D., & Kinley. (2005). Learning style preferences: Implications for Web-based

instruction. Clothing and Textiles Research Journal, 23(4), 266-276.

Davidson, J. & Wood, C. (2004). A conflict resolution model. Theory into Practice, 43(1), 6-13.

Emmer, E. & Stough, L. (2001). Classroom management: A critical part of educational psychology, with

implications for teacher education. Educational Psychologist, 36(2), 103-112.

Gibbs, J. (2003). Moral development and reality: Beyond the theories of Kohlberg and Hoffman. Thousand

Oaks, CA: Sage.

Good, T. & Brophy, J. (2002). Looking in classrooms, 9th edition. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.

Gordon, T. (2003). Teacher effectiveness training. New York: Three Rivers Press.

Guerrero, L. & Floyd, K. (2005). Nonverbal communication in close relationships. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Hawkins, J. (2006). Accessing multicultural issues through critical thinking, critical inquiry, and the student

research process. Urban Education, 41(2), 169-141.

Heimann , M. Strid, K., Smith , L., Tjus , T., Ulvund , S. & Meltzoff, A. (2006). Exploring the relation between

memory, gestural communication, and the emergence of language in infancy: a longitudinal study. Infant

and Child Development, 15(3), 233-249.

Hunter, R. (2004). Madeline Hunter's Mastery Teaching, Revised Edition. Thousand Oaks, CA: Corwin

Press.

Jones, T. (2004). Conflict resolution education: The field, the findings, and the future. Conflict Resolution

Quarterly, 22(1-2), 233-267.

Jones, V. & Jones, L. (2006). Comprehensive classroom management: Creating communities of support

and solving problems, 6th edition. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.

Katz, L. (2000). Engaging children's minds: The project approach. Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishers.

Kohn, A. (2006). Beyond discipline: From compliance to community. Reston, VA: Association for

Supervision and Curriculum Development.

Kounin, J. (1970). Discipline and group management in classrooms. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

Educational Psychology 207 A Global Text

9. Facilitating complex thinking

Lyman, F. T. (1981). The responsive classroom discussion: The inclusion of all students. In A. Anderson

(Ed.), Mainstreaming Digest (pp. 109-113). College Park: University of Maryland Press.

Marks, L. (2003). Instructional management tips for teachers of students with autism-spectrum disorder.

Teaching Exceptional Children, 35(4), 50-54.

Marsh, A., Elfenbein, H. & Ambady, N. (2003). Nonverbal “accents”: cultural differences in facial expressions

of emotion. Psychological Science, 14(3), 373-376.

Marzano, R. & Marzano, J. (2004). The key to classroom management. Educational Leadership, 62, pp. 2-7.

McCafferty, S., Jacobs, G., & Iddings, S. (Eds.). (2006). Cooperative learning and second language

teaching. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Moritz, J. & Christie, A. (2005). It’s elementary: Using elementary portfolios with young students. In C.

Crawford (Ed.), Proceedings of the Society for Information Technology and Teacher Education

International Conference 2005 (pp. 144-151). Chesapeake, VA: Association for the Advancement of

Computing in Education.

Nations, S. & Boyett, S. (2002). So much stuff, so little space: Creating and managing the learner-centered

classroom. Gainesville, FL: Maupin House.

Peterson, T. (2004). So you’re thinking of trying problem-based learning?: Three critical success factors for

implementation. Journal of Management Education, 28(5), 630-647.

Reynolds, A. (1992). What is competent beginning teaching? Review of Educational Research, 62(1), 1-35.

Slavin. R. (1994). Cooperative learning, 2nd edition. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.

Snow, R. (1989). Aptitude-treatment interaction as a framework for research on individual differences in

learning. In P. Ackerman, R. Sternberg, & R. Glaser (Eds.), Learning and individual differences, pp. 13-

60. New York: W. H. Freeman.

Sternberg, R. & Grigorenko, E. (2004). Successful intelligence in the classroom. Theory into Practice, 43(4),

274-280.

Stevens, B. & Tollafield, A. (2003). Creating comfortable and productive parent/teacher conferences. Phi

Delta Kappan, 84(7), 521-525.

Stiggins, R. & Chappuis, J. (2005). Using student-involved classroom assessment to close achievement gaps.

Theory into Practice 44(1), 11-18.

Thorson, S. (2003). Listening to students: Reflections on secondary classroom management. Boston: Allyn

& Bacon.

Turiel, E. (2006). The development of morality. In W. Damon, R. Lerner, & N. Eisenberg (Eds.), Handbook

of child psychology, vol. 3, pp. 789-857. New York: Wiley.

Van Meerionboer, J., Kirschner, P., & Kester, L. (2003). Taking the cognitive load off a learner’s mind:

Instructional design for complex learning. Educational Psychologist, 38(1), 5-13.

208

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

White, C. (2005). Student portfolios: An alternative way of encouraging and evaluating student learning. In

M. Achacoso & N. Svinicki (Eds.), Alternative Strategies for Evaluating Student Learning (pp. 37-42).

San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Weinstein, C.,Tomlinson-Clarke, S., & Curran, M. (2004). Toward a conception of culturally responsive

classroom management. Journal of Teacher Education, 55(1), 25-38.

Educational Psychology 209 A Global Text

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

10. Planning instruction“If you don’t know where you’re going, you could end up someplace else.”

(Casey Stengel)

Casey Stengel, a much-admired baseball coach, was talking about baseball when he made this

remark. But he could easily have been speaking of teaching as well. Almost by definition, education

has purposes, goals, and objectives, and a central task of teaching is to know what these are and to

transform the most general goals into specific objectives and tasks for students. Otherwise, as Casey

Stengel said, students may end up “someplace else” that neither they, nor the teacher, nor anyone

else intends. A lot of the clarification and specification of goals needs to happen before a cycle of

instruction actually begins, but the benefits of planning happen throughout all phases of teaching. If

students know precisely what they are supposed to learn, they can focus their attention and effort

more effectively. If the teacher knows precisely what students are supposed to learn, then the teacher

can make better use of class time and choose and design assessments of their learning that are more

fair and valid. In the long run everyone benefits.

This chapter is therefore about instructional planning, the systematic selection of educational goals and

objectives and their design for use in the classroom. We will divide this idea into four parts, and discuss them one at

a time. First is the problem of selecting general goals to teach; where can a teacher find these, and what do they look

like? Second is the problem of transforming goals into specific objectives, or statements concrete enough to guide

daily activity in class; what will students actually do or say into order to learn what a teacher wants them to learn?

Third is the problem of balancing and relating goals and objectives to each other; since we may want students to

learn numerous goals, how can we combine or integrate them so that the overall classroom program does not

become fragmented or biased? Fourth is the challenge of relating instructional goals to students’ prior experiences

and knowledge. We have discussed this challenge before from the perspective of learning theory (in Chapter 2), but

in this chapter we look at it from the more practical perspective of curriculum planning.

Selecting general learning goals

At the most general or abstract level, the goals of education include important philosophical ideas like

“developing individuals to their fullest potential” and “preparing students to be productive members of society”.

Few teachers would disagree with these ideas in principle, though they might disagree about their wording or about

their relative importance. As a practical matter, however, teachers might have trouble translating such generalities

into specific lesson plans or activities for the next day’s class. What does it mean, concretely, to “develop an

individual to his or her fullest potential”? Does it mean, for example, that a language arts teacher should ask

students to write an essay about their personal interests, or does it mean that the teacher should help students learn

to write as well as possible on any topic, even ones that are not of immediate interest? What exactly should a

teacher do, from day to day, to “prepare students to be productive members of society” as well? Answers to

Educational Psychology 210 A Global Text

10. Planning instruction

questions like these are needed to plan instruction effectively. But the answers are not obvious simply by examining

statements of general educational goals.

National and state learning standards

Some (but not all) of the work of transforming such general purposes into more precise teaching goals and even

more precise objectives has been performed by broad US organizations that represent educators and other experts

about particular subjects or types of teaching (Riley, 2002). The groups have proposed national standards,

which are summaries of what students can reasonably be expected to learn at particular grade levels and in

particular subjects areas. In the United States, in addition, all state governments create state standards that serve

much the same purpose: they express what students in the state should (and hopefully can) learn at all grade levels

and in all subjects. Examples of organizations that provide national standards are listed in Table 27, and examples

of state standards are listed in Table 28 for one particular state, Ohio, in the area of language arts.

Table 27: Organizations with statements of US educational standards

Subject Organization

English and Language Arts Council of Teachers of English

American Council on the Teaching of Foreign

Languages

Mathematics National Council of Teachers of Mathematics

Physical Education and Health National Association for Sport and Physical

Education

American Cancer Society

Science National Academies of Science

American Association for the Advancement of

Science

Social Studies National Council for the Social Studies

Center for Civic Education

National Council on Economic Education

National Geographic Society

National Center for History in the Schools

Technology International Society for Technology in Education

Other Specialized Standards Statements:

211

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

American Indian Content Standards Center for Educational Technology in Indian

America

Ethical Standards for School Counselors American School Counselors Association

Information Literacy Standards American Association of School Librarians

Business Education National Business Education Association

Parent Education and Involvement Parent-Teacher Association (PTA)

Source: , accessed December 5, 2006. Summaries of all of

these standards, as well as access to the relevant web pages of the corresponding organizations, can be found at

this website. Because standards are revised continually, and because of the dynamic nature of websites, the

information may differ slightly from the above when you actually access it.

Table 28: Examples of state curriculum standards about language arts

Grade-level: Classroom example:

Kindergarten-Grade 3: Read accurately high-

frequency sight words.

Play a game: “How many words can you see around

the classroom that you can read already?”

Grade 4-7: Infer word meaning through

identification and analysis of analogies and other word

relationships.

Have students keep a journal of unfamiliar words

which they encounter and of what they think the words

mean.

Grade 8-10: Recognize the importance and function

of figurative language.

Have students write a brief essay explaining the

meaning of a common figure of speech, and speculating

on why it became common usage.

Grade 11-12: Verify meanings of words by the

author’s use of definition, restatement, example,

comparison, contrast and cause and effect.

Have students analyze an essay that includes

unfamiliar terms using clues in the essay to determine

their meaning.

Source for standards: Ohio Department of Education, 2003, p. 30-31

Because they focus on grade levels and subject areas, general statements of educational standards tend to be a

bit more specific than the broader philosophical goals we discussed above. As a rule of thumb, too, state standards

tend to be more comprehensive than national standards, both in coverage of grade levels and of subjects. The

difference reflects the broad responsibility of states in the United States for all aspects of public education; national

organizations, in contrast, usually assume responsibility only for a particular subject area or particular group of

students. Either type of standards provides a first step, however, toward transforming the grandest purposes of

schooling (like developing the individual or preparing for society) into practical classroom activities. But they

provide a first step only. Most statements of standards do not make numerous or detailed suggestions of actual

Educational Psychology 212 A Global Text

10. Planning instruction

activities or tasks for students, though some might include brief classroom examples—enough to clarify the

meaning of a standard, but not enough to plan an actual classroom program for extended periods of time. For these

latter purposes, teachers rely on more the detailed documents, the ones often called curriculum frameworks and

curriculum guides.

Curriculum frameworks and curriculum guides

The terms curriculum framework and curriculum guide sometimes are used almost interchangeably, but for

convenience we will use them to refer to two distinct kinds of documents. The more general of the two is

curriculum framework, which is a document that explains how content standards can or should be organized

for a particular subject and at various grade levels. Sometimes this information is referred to as the scope and

sequence for a curriculum. A curriculum framework document is like a standards statement in that it does not

usually provide a lot of detailed suggestions for daily teaching. It differs from a standards statement, though, in that

it analyzes each general standard in a curriculum into more specific skills that students need to learn, often a dozen

or more per standard. The language or terminology of a framework statement also tends to be somewhat more

concrete than a standards statement, in the sense that it is more likely to name behaviors of students—things that a

teacher might see them do or hear them say. Sometimes, but not always, it may suggest ways for assessing whether

students have in fact acquired each skill listed in the document. Table 29 shows a page from a curriculum

framework published by the California State Board of Education (Curriculum Development and Supplemental

Materials Committee, 1999). In this case the framework explains the state standards for learning to read, and the

excerpt in Table 29 illustrates how one particular standard, that “students speak and write with command of

English conventions appropriate to this grade level”, is broken into nine more specific skills. Note that the excerpt

names observable behaviors of students (what they do or say); we will discuss this feature again, more fully, in the

next part of this chapter, because it is helpful in classroom planning. In spite of this feature, though, the framework

document does not lay out detailed activity plans that a teacher could use on a daily basis. (Though even so, it is

over 300 pages long!)

Table 29: An excerpt from reading/language arts framework for California public schools

Comments:

More general standards statement

More specific or concrete framework statements →

(stated as relatively specific skills or behaviors)

Written and oral English language

conventions, third grade

Students write and speak with a command of

standard English conventions appropriate to this grade

level.

Sentence Structure

1.1 Understand and be able to use complete and

correct declarative, interrogative, imperative, and

exclamatory sentences in writing and speaking.

Grammar

213

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

1.2 Identify subjects and verbs that are in agreement

and identify and use pronouns, adjectives, compound

words, and articles correctly in writing and speaking.

1.3 Identify and use past, present, and future verb

tenses properly in writing and speaking.

1.4 Identify and use subjects and verbs correctly in

speaking and writing simple sentences.

Punctuation

1.5 Punctuate dates, city and state, and titles of

books correctly.

1.6 Use commas in dates, locations, and addresses

and for items in a series.

Capitalization

1.7 Capitalize geographical names, holidays,

historical periods, and special events correctly.

Spelling

1.8 Spell correctly one-syllable words that have

blends, contractions, compounds, orthographic patters,

and common homophones.

1.9 Arrange words in alphabetical order.

Teachers’ need for detailed activity suggestions is more likely to be met by a curriculum guide, a document

devoted to graphic descriptions of activities that foster or encourage the specific skills explained in a curriculum

framework document. The descriptions may mention or list curriculum goals served by an activity, but they are also

likely to specify materials that a teacher needs, time requirements, requirements for grouping students, drawings or

diagrams of key equipment or materials, and sometimes even suggestions for what to say to students at different

points during the activity. In these ways the descriptions may resemble lesson plans.

Since classroom activities often support more than one specific skill, activities in a curriculum guide may be

organized differently than they might be in a framework document. Instead of highlighting only one standard at a

time, as the framework document might, activities may be grouped more loosely—for example, according to the

dominant purpose or goal of an activity (“Activities that encourage the practice of math facts”) or according to a

dominant piece of equipment or material (“Ten activities with tin cans”). Table 30 shows a description of a

Educational Psychology 214 A Global Text

10. Planning instruction

kindergarten-level activity about “autumn leaves” that might appear in a curriculum guide. Note that the activity

meets several educational objectives at once—tracing shapes, knowledge of leaves and of colors, descriptive

language skill. Each of these skills may reflect a different curriculum standard.

Table 30: Sample curriculum guide activity

Curriculum guides provide graphic descriptions of activities that can be used fairly directly in the classroom.

Although they are relevant to standards and framework statements, they often are not organized around standards

and objectives as such.

Activity: Autumn Leaves

Level: Kindergarten

Themes and Curriculum Connections: trees, autumn, color naming, color comparisons, size comparisons,

functions of leaves, growth, the life cycle. See also Standards #xx-yy.

Best time to do it: Fall (October), or whenever leaves are available

Materials needed: (1) small paper (6 x 6 inches); (2) access to leaves; (3) white glue; (4) felt pens or colored

pencils

What to do: Give one piece of the small paper to each child. Invite children to color the sheet so that the entire

sheet is decorated. Invite children to choose one leaf. Place leaf under the colored (decorated) paper and trace the

shape of the leaf lightly in pencil. Then invite children to cut out the colored paper in the shape that has been

traced of the leaf.

Cautions: (1) Some children may need individual help with tracing or cutting. (2) Try to use leaves that are still

somewhat pliable, because some very old leaves (dried out) may crumble when traced.

Things to talk about: Are some leaves bigger than others? Do they change shape as they grow, or only their

size? How do leaves benefit trees? How many different colors can real leaves be?

Formulating learning objectives

Given curriculum frameworks and guides like the ones just described, how do you choose and formulate actual

learning objectives? Basically there are two approaches: either start by selecting content or topics that what you

want students to know (the cognitive approach) or start with what you want students to do (the behavioral

approach). In effect the cognitive approach moves from the general to the specific, and the behavioral approach

does the opposite. Each approach has advocates, as well as inherent strengths and problems. In practice, teachers

often combine or alternate between them in order to give students some of the advantages of each.

From general to specific: selecting content topics

The cognitive approach assumes that teachers normally have a number of long-term, general goals for students,

and it begins with those goals. It also assumes that each student work toward long-term, general goals along

215

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

different pathways and using different styles of learning. Because of these assumptions, it is necessary to name

indicators, which are examples of specific behaviors by which students might show success at reaching a general

learning goal. But it is neither desirable nor possible for a list of indicators to be complete—only for it to be

representative (Gronlund, 2004). Consider this example from teaching middle-school biology. For this subject you

might have a general goal like the following, with accompanying indicators:

Goal:

The student will understand the nature and purpose of photosynthesis.

Indicators:

1. explains the purpose of photosynthesis and steps in the process

2. diagrams steps in the chemical process

3. describes how plant photosynthesis affects the animal world

4. writes a plan for how to test leaves for presence of photosynthesis

5. makes an oral presentation and explains how the experiment was conducted

Using a strictly cognitive approach to planning, therefore, a teacher’s job has two parts. First she must identify,

find, or choose a manageable number of general goals—perhaps just a half dozen or so. (Sometimes these can be

taken or adapted from a curriculum framework document such as discussed earlier.) Then the teacher must think

of a handful of specific examples or behavioral indicators for each goal—just a half dozen or so of these as well. The

behavioral indicators clarify the meaning of the general goal, but are not meant to be the only way that students

might show success at learning. Then, at last, thoughtful planning for individual lessons or activities can begin. This

approach works especially well for learning goals that are relatively long-term—goals that take many lessons, days,

or weeks to reach. During such long periods of teaching, it is impossible to specify the exact, detailed behaviors that

every student can or should display to prove that he or she has reached a general goal. It is possible, however, to

specify general directions toward which all students should focus their learning and to explain the nature of the

goals with a sample of well-chosen indicators or examples (Popham, 2002).

The cognitive, general-to-specific approach is reasonable on the face of it, and in fact probably describes how

many teachers think about their instructional planning. But critics have argued that indicators used as examples

may not in fact clarify the general goal enough; students therefore end up unexpectedly—as Casey Stengel said at

the start of this chapter—“someplace else”. Given the general goal of understanding photosynthesis described

above, for example, how are we to know whether the five indicators that are listed really allow a teacher to grasp the

full meaning of the goal? Put differently, how else might a student show understanding of photosynthesis, and how

is a teacher to know that a student’s achievement is s a legitimate display of understanding? To some educators,

grasping the meaning of goals from indicators is not as obvious as it should be, and in any case is prone to

misunderstanding. The solution, they say, is not to start planning with general goals, but with specific behaviors

that identify students’ success.

Educational Psychology 216 A Global Text

10. Planning instruction

From specific to general: behavioral objectives

Compared to the cognitive approach, the behavioral approach to instructional planning reverses the steps in

planning. Instead of starting with general goal statements accompanied by indicator examples, it starts with the

identification of specific behaviors—concrete actions or words—that students should perform or display as a result

of instruction (Mager, 2005). Collectively, the specific behaviors may describe a more general educational goal, but

unlike the indicators used in the cognitive approach, they are not a mere sampling of the possible specific outcomes.

Instead they represent all the intended specific outcomes. Consider this sampling of behavioral objectives:

Objectives: Learning to use in-line roller blade skates (beginning level)

1. Student ties boots on correctly.

2. Student puts on safety gear correctly, including helmet, knee and elbow pads.

3. Student skates 15 meters on level ground without falling.

4. Student stops on demand within a three meter distance, without falling.

The objectives listed are not merely a representative sample of how students can demonstrate success with

roller-blading. Instead they are behaviors that every student should acquire in order to meet the goal of using roller

blades as a beginner. There simply are no other ways to display learning of this goal; getting 100 per cent on a

written test about roller blading, for example, would not qualify as success with this goal, though it might show

success at some other goal, such as verbal knowledge about roller blading. Even adding other skating behaviors

(like “Student skates backwards” or “Student skates in circles”) might not qualify as success with this particular

goal, because it could reasonably be argued that the additional skating behaviors are about skating at an advanced

level, not a beginning level.

In the most commonly used version of this approach, originated by Robert Mager (1962, 2005), a good

behavioral objective should have three features. First, it should specify a behavior that can in fact be observed. In

practice this usually means identifying something that a student does or says, not something a student thinks or

feels. Compare the following examples; the one on the left names a behavior to be performed, but the one on the

right names a thinking process that cannot, in principle, be seen:

Behavioral objective

The student will make a list of animal species that

live in the water but breathe air and a separate list of

species that live in the water but do not require air to

breathe.

Not behavioral object

The student will understand the difference between

fish and mammals that live in the water.

The second feature of a good behavioral objective is that it describes conditions of performance of the

behavior. What are the special circumstances to be provided when the student performs the objective? Consider

these two examples:

Special condition of performance is specified

Given a list of 50 species, the student will circle those

that live in water but breathe air and underline those

A special condition of performance is not

specified

After three days of instruction, the student will

217

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

that live in water but do not breathe air. identify species that live in water but breathe air, as well

as species that live in water but do not breathe air.

The objective on the left names a special condition of performance—that the student will be given a particular

kind of list to work from—which is not part of the instruction itself. The objective on the right appears to name a

condition—“three days of instruction”. But the condition really describes what the teacher will do (she will

instruct), not something specific to students’ performance.

The third feature of a good behavioral objective is that it specifies a minimum level or degree of acceptable

performance. Consider these two examples:

Specifies minimum level

Given a list of 50 species, the student will circle all of

those that live in water but breathe air and underline all

of those that live in water but do not breathe air. The

student will do so within fifteen minutes.

Does not specify minimum level

The student will circle names of species that live in

water but breathe air and underline those that live in

water but do not breathe air.

The objective on the left specifies a level of performance—100 per cent accuracy within 15 minutes. The objective

on the right leaves this information out (and incidentally it also omits the condition of performance mentioned on

the left).

Behavioral objectives have obvious advantages because of their clarity and precision. They seem especially well

suited for learning that by their nature they can be spelled out explicitly and fully, such as when a student is

learning to drive a car, to use safety equipment in a science laboratory, or install and run a particular computer

program. Most of these goals, as it happens, also tend to have relatively short learning cycles, meaning that they can

be learned as a result of just one lesson or activity, or of just a short series of them at most. Such goals tend not to

include the larger, more abstract goals of education. In practice, both kinds of goals— the general and the specific—

form a large part of education at all grade levels.

Finding the best in both approaches

When it comes to teaching and learning the large or major goals, then, behavioral objectives can seem unwieldy.

How, a teacher might ask, can you spell out all of the behaviors involved in a general goal like becoming a good

citizen? How could you name in advance the numerous conditions under which good citizenship might be

displayed, or the minimum acceptable level of good citizenship expected in each condition? Specifying these

features seems impractical at best, and at times even undesirable ethically or philosophically. (Would we really

want any students to become “minimum citizens”?) Because of these considerations, many teachers find it sensible

to compromise between the cognitive and behavioral approaches. Here are some features that are often part of a

compromise:

• When planning, think about BOTH long-term, general goals AND short-term, immediate objectives. A

thorough, balanced look at most school curricula shows that they are concerned with the general as well as

the specific. In teaching elementary math, for example, you may want students to learn general problem

solving strategies (a general goal), but you may also want them to learn specific math facts (a specific

objective). In teaching Shakespeare’s plays in high school, you may want students to be able to compare the

Educational Psychology 218 A Global Text

10. Planning instruction

plays critically (a general goal), but doing so may require that they learn details about the characters and

plots of the major plays (a specific objective). Since general goals usually take longer to reach than specific

objectives, instructional planning has to include both time frames.

• Plan for what students do, not what the teacher does. This idea may seem obvious, but it is easy to overlook

it when devising lesson plans. Consider that example again about teaching Shakespeare. If you want

students to learn the details about Shakespeare’s plays, it is tempting to plan objectives like “Summarize the

plot of each play to students”, or “Write and hand out to students an outline of the plays”. Unfortunately

these objectives describe only what the teacher does, and makes the assumption (often unwarranted) that

students will remember what the teacher says or puts in writing for them. A better version of the same

objective should focus on the actions of students, not of teachers—for example, “Students will write a

summary, from memory, of each of the major plays of Shakespeare”. This version focuses on what students

do instead of what the teacher does. (Of course you may still have to devise activities that help students to

reach the objective, such as providing guided practice in writing summaries of plays.)

• To insure diversity of goals and objectives when planning, consider organizing goals and objectives by using

a systematic classification scheme of educational objectives. At the beginning of this section we stated that

there is a need, when devising goals and objectives, for both the specific and the general. Actually a more

accurate statement is that there is a need for goals and objectives that refer to a variety of cognitive

processes and that have varying degrees of specificity or generality. One widely used classification scheme

that does so, for example, is one proposed 50 years ago by Benjamin Bloom (1956) and revised recently by

his associates (Anderson & Krathwohl, 2001). We describe this system, called a taxonomy of objectives, in

the next section.

Taxonomies of educational objectives

When educators have proposed taxonomies of educational objectives, they have tended to focus on one of three

areas or domains of psychological functioning: either students’ cognition (thought), students’ feelings and emotions

(affect), or students’ physical skills (psychomotor abilities). Of these three areas, they have tended to focus the most

attention on cognition. The taxonomy originated by Benjamin Bloom, for example, deals entirely with cognitive

outcomes of instruction.

Bloom’s Taxonomy:

In its original form, Bloom’s Taxonomy of educational objectives referred to forms of cognition or thinking,

which were divided into the six levels (Bloom, et al., 1956). Table 31 summarizes the levels, and offers two kinds of

examples—simple ones based on the children’s story, Goldilocks and the Three Bears, and complex ones more

typical of goals and objectives used in classrooms. The levels form a loose hierarchy from simple to complex

thinking, at least when applied to some subjects and topics. When planning for these subjects it can therefore be

helpful not only for insuring diversity among learning objectives, but also for sequencing materials. In learning

about geography, for example, it may sometimes make sense to begin with information about specific places or

societies (knowledge and comprehension), and work gradually toward comparisons and assessments among the

places or societies (analysis and synthesis).

Table 31: Bloom’s Taxonomy of objectives: cognitive domain

219

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Type or level of learning Simple example Classroom example

Knowledge: recall of

information, whether it is simple or

complex in nature

“Name three things that

Goldilocks did in the house of the

three bears.”

“List all of the planets of the solar

system.”

“State five key features of life in

the middle ages.”

Comprehension: grasping the

meaning of information, by

interpreting it or translating it from

one form to another

“Explain why Goldilocks

preferred the little bear’s chair.”

“Convert the following arithmetic

word problem to a mathematical

equation.”

“Describe how plants contribute

to the welfare of animal life.”

Application: using information

in new, concrete situations

“Predict some of the things

Goldilocks might have used if she

had entered your house.”

“Illustrate how positive

reinforcement might affect the

behavior of a pet dog.”

“Use examples from the plot to

illustrate the theme of novel.”

Analysis: breaking information

into its components to understand

its structure

“Select the part of Goldilocks and

the Three Bears where you think

Goldilocks felt most comfortable.”

“Compare the behavior of

domestic dogs with the behavior of

wolves.”

“Diagram the effects of weather

patterns on plant metabolism.”

Synthesis: putting parts of

information together into a coherent

whole

“Tell how the story would have

been different if it had been three

fishes.”

“Design an experiment to test the

effects of gravity on root growth.”

“Write an account of how

humans would be different if life had

originated on Mars instead of

Earth.”

Evaluation: judging the value of

information for a particular purpose

“Justify this statement:

Goldilocks was a bad girl.”

“Appraise the relevance of the

novel for modern life.”

“Assess the value of information

processing theory for planning

instruction.”

Educational Psychology 220 A Global Text

10. Planning instruction

Such a sequence does not work well, however, for all possible topics or subjects. To learn certain topics in

mathematics, for example, students may sometimes need to start with general ideas (like “What does it mean to

multiply?”) than with specific facts (like “How much is 4 x 6?”) (Egan, 2005). At other times, though, the reverse

sequence may be preferable. Whatever the case, a taxonomy of cognitive objectives, like Bloom’s, can help to

remind teachers to set a variety of objectives and to avoid relying excessively on just one level, such as simple recall

of factual knowledge (Notar, et al., 2004).

Bloom’s Taxonomy revised

A few years ago two of Benjamin Bloom’s original colleagues, Linda Anderson and David Krathwohl, revised his

taxonomy so as to clarify its terms and to make it more complete (Anderson & Krathwohl, 2001; Marzano, 2006).

The resulting categories are summarized and compared to the original categories in Table 32. As the chart shows,

several categories of objectives have been renamed and a second dimension added that describes the kind of

thinking or cognitive processing that may occur. The result is a much richer taxonomy than before, since every level

of the objectives can now take four different forms. Remembering, for example, can refer to four different kinds of

memory: memory for facts, for concepts, for procedures, or for metacognitive knowledge. Table 32 gives examples

of each of these kinds of memory.

Table 32: Bloom’s Taxonomy of cognitive objectives—revised

Original term from

Bloom’s Taxonomy

(1956)

Revised term

emphasizing cognitive

processing (2001)

A new dimension

added: types of

knowledge learned

(2001)

Example of

cognitive process

remembering

combined with

possible types of

knowledge

Knowledge Remembering

Comprehension Understanding

Application Applying

Analysis Analyzing

Evaluation Evaluating

Synthesis Creating

• factual knowledge

• conceptual

knowledge

• procedural

knowledge

• metacognitive

knowledge

Memory for facts:

recalling the names of

each part of a living cell

Memory for concepts:

recalling the functions of

each part of a living cell

Memory for

procedures: recalling

how to view a cell under a

microscope

Memory for

metacognition: recalling

not the names of the parts,

but a technique for

remembering the names of

221

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

the parts of a living cell

Caption: The revision to Bloom’s Taxonomy distinguishes between cognitive processes (left-hand column in

the table) and types of knowledge learned (right-hand column). The original version has terms similar to the

cognitive processing terms in the revised version. According to the revised version, any type of knowledge (from

the right-hand column) can, in principle, occur with any type of cognitive processing (left-hand column).

Taxonomies of affective objectives and psychomotor objectives

Although taxonomies related to affect, or the feelings and emotions of students, are used less commonly than

cognitive taxonomies for planning instruction, various educators have constructed them. One of the most widely

known was also published by colleagues of Benjamin Bloom and classifies affect according to how committed a

student feels toward what he is learning (Krathwohl, Bloom, & Masia, 1964/1999). Table 33 summarizes the

categories and gives brief examples. The lowest level, called receiving, simply involves willingness to experience

new knowledge or activities. Higher levels involve embracing or adopting experiences in ways that are increasingly

organized and that represent increasingly stable forms of commitment.

Table 33: Taxonomies of objectives: affective domain and psychomotor domain

Affective domain Psychomotor domain

Receiving Willingness to attend to

particular experience

Imitation Repeating a simple

action that has been

demonstrated

Responding Willingness to

participate actively in an

experience

Manipulation Practice of an action

that has been imitated but

only learned partially

Valuing Perception of

experience as worthwhile

Precision Quick, smooth

execution of an action that

has been practiced

Organization Coordination of valued

experiences into partially

coherent wholes

Articulation Execution of an action

not only with precision, but

also with modifications

appropriate to new

circumstances

Characterization by a Coordination of valued Naturalization Incorporation of an

Educational Psychology 222 A Global Text

10. Planning instruction

value complex experiences and of

organized sets of

experiences into a single

comprehensive value

hierarchy

action into the motor

repertoire, along with

experimentation with new

motor actions

Taxonomies related to abilities and skills that are physical, or psychomotor, have also been used less widely than

affective taxonomies, with the notable exception of one area of teaching where they are obviously relevant: physical

education. As you might expect, taxonomic categories of motor skills extend from simple, brief actions to complex,

extended action sequences that combine simpler, previously learned skills smoothly and automatically (Harrow,

1972; Simpson, 1972). One such classification scheme is shown in Table 33. An example of a very basic

psychomotor skill might be imitating the action of throwing a ball when modeled by someone else; an example of

the latter might be performing a 10 minute gymnastics routine which the student has devised for himself or herself.

Note, though, that many examples of psychomotor skills also exist outside the realm of physical education. In a

science course, for example, a student might need to learn to operate laboratory equipment that requires using

delicate, fine movements. In art classes, students might learn to draw, and in music they might learn to play an

instrument (both are partly motor skills). Most first graders are challenged by the motor skills of learning to write.

For students with certain physical disabilities, furthermore, motor skill development is an important priority for the

student’s entire education.

Students as a source of instructional goals

So far our discussion of instructional planning has described goals and objectives as if they are selected

primarily by educators and teachers, and not by students themselves. The assumption may be correct in many

cases, but there are problems with it. One problem is that choosing goals and objectives for students, rather than by

students, places a major burden on everyone involved in education—curriculum writers, teachers, and students.

The curriculum writers have to make sure that they specify standards, goals, and objectives that are truly important

for students to learn (what if it really does not matter, for example, whether a science student learns about the

periodic table of the elements?). Teachers have to make sure that students actually become motivated to learn the

specified goals and objectives, even if the students are not motivated initially. Students have to master pre-set goals

and objectives even if they might not have chosen them personally. Some critics of education have argued that these

requirements can be serious impediments to learning (Kohn, 2004). The problems are widespread and especially

noticeable in two forms of teaching. One is with the youngest students, who may especially lack patience with an

educational agenda set by others (Kohn, 1999; Seitz, 2006). The other is with culturally diverse classrooms, where

students and their families may hold a variety of legitimate, but unconventional expectations about what they

should learn (J. Banks & C. Banks, 2005).

In response to concerns like these, some educators advocate planning instruction around goals set or expressed

either by students themselves or by the cultures or communities with which students identify. Their suggestions

223

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

vary in detail, but can be organized into two broad categories: (1) emergent curriculum and (2) multicultural and

anti-bias curriculum.

Emergent curriculum

An emergent curriculum is one that explicitly builds on interests expressed by students, rather than goals set

by curriculum writers, curriculum documents, or teachers. As you might suspect, therefore, instructional planning

for an emergent curriculum does not have the same meaning that the term has had in the chapter up to now.

Instead, since an emergent curriculum by definition unfolds spontaneously and flexibly, students’ interests may be

predictable, but usually not very far in advance (Peterson, 2002). Suppose, for example, that a first-grade teacher

plans a unit around Halloween, and that as one of the activities for this unit she reads a book about Halloween. In

listening to the book, however, the students turn out to be less interested in its Halloween content than in the fact

that one of the illustrations in the book shows a picture of a full moon partially hidden by clouds. They begin asking

about the moon: why it is full sometimes but not other times, why it rises in different places each month, and

whether the moon really moves behind clouds or whether the clouds actually do the moving. The teacher

encourages their questions and their interest in moon astronomy. Over the next days or weeks, she arranges further

activities and experiences to encourage students’ interest: she sets aside her original plans about Halloween and

finds books about the moon and about how the solar system works. She invites a local amateur astronomer to visit

the group and talk about his observations of the moon. Several children build models of the moon out of paper

maché. Some find books describing trips of the space shuttles to the moon. Others make a large mural depicting a

moonscape. And so on; the original goals about Halloween are not so much rejected, as set aside or forgotten in

favor of something more immediately interesting and motivating.

While these activities could in principle happen because of recommendations from a curriculum document, the

key point about emergent curriculum is that they happen for a very different reason: these activities happen and the

goals emerge because the children want them. A teacher’s challenge is therefore not planning activities that match

predetermined curriculum goals or objectives, but to respond flexibly and sensitively as students’ interests become

known and explicit. Teachers’ responsiveness is facilitated by two practices that are especially prominent when a

teacher adopts an emergent approach to curriculum. The first is careful, continuous observation of students. The

teacher watches and listens, and may keep informal written records of students’ comments and activities. The

information allows her to respond more effectively to the interests they express, and at the same time it provides a

type of assessment of students’ progress—information about what the students are actually learning.

A second strategy that facilitates teachers’ success is curriculum webbing, a process of brainstorming

connections among initiatives suggested by students and ideas suggested by the teacher. In some cases webs can be

created jointly with students by brainstorming with them about where their current interests may lead. In other

cases they can be created independently by the teacher’s own reflections. In still others, when a classroom has more

than one adult responsible for it, they can be created jointly with fellow teachers or teacher assistants. The latter

approach works especially well in preschool, kindergartens, or special education classrooms, which often have more

than one adult responsible for the class (Vartuli & Rohs, 2006).

To some, emergent curriculum may seem like a formula for curriculum and management disasters. But the

approach has often proved quite successful, particularly in early childhood education and the earliest grade levels of

Educational Psychology 224 A Global Text

10. Planning instruction

elementary school (Seitz, 2006; Wurm, 2005). Something akin to emergent curriculum is quite possible, in

principle, even with older students. In Chapter 8, for example, we described a high school program in which

students began with problems and experiences that were personally relevant, and discussed the problems with

classmates to formulate research problems which they then studied more formally and systematically (Hawkins,

2006). In essence this strategy created an emergent curriculum analogous to the ones described above for young

children. What the high school students studied was not predetermined, but emerged from their own expressed

interests.

Multicultural and anti-bias education

A culture is an all-encompassing set of values, beliefs, practices and customs of a group or community—its total

way of life. Cultures may be shared widely, even by much if not all of an entire nation, or they may be shared by

relatively few, such as a small community within a large city. Sometimes the term culture is even applied to the way

of life of an individual family or of a specialized group in society; some might argue, for example, that there is a

culture of schooling shared by teachers, though not necessarily by all students.

Because culture by definition touches on all aspects of living, it is likely to affect students’ perspectives about

school, their ways of learning and their motivations to learn. The differences go beyond obvious differences in

holidays, language, or food preferences. In some cultures, for example, individuals keep good eye contact with

someone to whom they are speaking, and expect the same from others. In other cultures, such behavior is

considered intrusive or overly aggressive, and avoiding eye contact while speaking is considered more respectful. Or

another example: in some cultures it is expected that individuals will be punctual (or on time), whereas in others

punctuality is considered overly compulsive, and a more casual approach to time is the norm. Students regularly

bring differences like these to school, where they combine with expectations from teachers and other school staff,

and contribute indirectly to differences in achievement and satisfaction among students.

To be fully effective, therefore, instructional planning has to take into account the diversity in students’ cultural

backgrounds, whether the differences are observable or subtle. Planning also has to work deliberately to reduce the

social biases and prejudices that sometimes develop about cultural differences. Multicultural education and

anti-bias education are two terms referring to these purposes. Their meanings often overlap significantly,

depending on the context or on who is using the terms. Generally, though, the first term—multicultural education—

has somewhat more to do with understanding the differences among cultures. The latter term—anti-bias education

—has more to do with overcoming social prejudices and biases resulting from cultural differences. For convenience

in this chapter, we will use the single term multicultural education to refer to both understanding differences and

overcoming prejudices.

Fully effective multicultural education has several features. The most obvious and familiar one is content

integration: the curriculum uses examples and information from different cultures to illustrate various concepts

or ideas already contained in the curriculum (Vavrus, 2002). In studying holidays, for example, an elementary-

school teacher includes activities and information about Kwanzaa as well as Christmas, Hanukkah, or other

holidays happening at about the same time. In studying the US Civil War, another example, a middle-years teacher

includes material written from the perspective of African-American slaves and Southern landowners. In teaching

language arts, students learn basic vocabulary of any non-English languages spoken by some members of the class.

225

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

But there is more to multicultural education than integrating content from diverse cultures. Among other

features, it also requires an equity pedagogy, which is an effort to allow or even encourage, a variety of learning

styles—styles at which students may have become skillful because of their cultural backgrounds (Crow, 2005; C.

Banks & J. Banks, 1995). In elementary language arts, for example, there may be more than one “best” way to tell a

story. Should a student necessarily have to tell it alone and standing in front of the whole class, or might the student

tell it jointly with a friend or in a smaller group? In learning to write a story, is legitimate variety also possible

there? Should a written story necessarily begin with a topic sentence that announces what the story is about, or can

it save a statement of topic for the ending or even it leave it out altogether in order to stimulate readers to think?

The best choice is related in part to the nature and purpose of the story, of course, but partly also to differences in

cultural expectations about story telling. Choosing a story form also points toward another feature of multicultural

education, the knowledge construction process, which is the unstated, unconscious process by which a

cultural group creates knowledge or information. The popular media, for example, often portray Hispanic-

Americans in ways that are stereotypical, either subtly or blatantly (Lester & Ross, 2003). A fully multicultural

curriculum finds way to call these images to the attention of students and to engage them in thinking about how

and why the images oversimplify reality.

Yet there is even more to a fully multicultural education. In addition to content integration, equity pedagogy,

and knowledge construction, it fosters prejudice reduction, or activities, discussions and readings that identify

students’ negative evaluations of cultural groups (Jacobson, 2003; J. Banks & C. Banks, 2004). The activities and

discussions can of course take a somewhat philosophical approach—examining how students feel in general, what

experiences they remember having involving prejudice, and the like. But the activities and discussions can also take

a more indirect and subtle form, as when a teacher periodically speaks in a student’s native language as a public

sign of respect for the student. Gestures and discussions like these are especially effective if they contribute to the

fifth element of multicultural education, empowering the school and social structure, in which all teachers

and staff members find ways to convey respect for cultural differences, including even during extra-curricular and

sports activities. A sports team or a debate club should not be limited to students from one cultural background and

exclude those from another—or more subtly, accept everyone but give the more desirable roles only to individuals

with particular social backgrounds. To the extent that cultural respect and inclusion are school-wide, teaching and

learning both become easier and more successful, and instructional planning in particular becomes more relevant

to students’ needs.

Enhancing student learning through a variety of resources

Whether instructional goals originate from curriculum documents, students’ expressed interests, or a mixture of

both, students are more likely to achieve the goals if teachers draw on a wide variety of resources. As a practical

matter, this means looking for materials and experiences that supplement—or occasionally even replace—the most

traditional forms of information, such as textbooks. Precisely what resources to use depend on factors unique to

each class, school, or community, but they might include one or more of the following.

The Internet as a learning tool

The Internet has become a fixture of modern society, and it offers a huge variety of information on virtually any

topic, including any school subject and any possible grade level from kindergarten through university. At the time

of writing this book (2007), about two-thirds of all households in the United States and Canada have at least some

Educational Psychology 226 A Global Text

10. Planning instruction

sort of Internet access, and virtually 100 per cent of public and private schools have some access (Parsad & Jones,

2006). These circumstances make the Internet a potential major resource for teachers and students—a virtual

library many times larger than even the largest physical (or “bricks and mortar”) libraries in the world.

But the vastness of the Internet is not entirely a blessing. A major problem is that the sheer volume of

information available, which can sometimes make searching for a specific topic, article, or document overwhelming

and inefficient. The newer search engines (such as Google at ) can help with this

problem, though they do not solve it completely. When searching the term photosynthesis, for example, Google and

other similar search engines return over six million web pages that discuss or refer this topic in some way! If a

teacher is planning a unit about photosynthesis, or if a student is writing an essay about it, which of these web

pages will prove most helpful? Choosing among web pages is a new, somewhat specialized form of computer

literacy, one that can be learned partially by trial-and-error online, but that also benefits from assistance by a

teacher or by more experienced peers (Ragains, 2006).

Another problem with the Internet is inequity of access. Even though, as we mentioned above, virtually all

schools now have access of some sort, the access is distributed quite unevenly across communities and income

groups (Skinner, Biscope, & Poland, 2003; Parsad & Jones, 2005). For one thing, the large majority of Web pages

are posted in English, and this fact naturally poses a challenge for any students who still learning to read or write

English. For another, schools vary widely in how much Internet service they can provide. In general, well-to-do

schools and those in cities provide more access than those located in less well-off areas or in rural areas—though

there are many exceptions. A richly endowed school might have an Internet connection in every classroom as well

as multiple connections in a school library or in specialized computer rooms. Students as well as faculty would be

able to use these facilities, and one or more teachers might have special training in Internet research to help when

problems arise. At the other extreme, a school might have only a few Internet connections for the entire school, or

even just one, located in a central place like the library or the school office. Usage by students would consequently

be limited, and teachers would essentially teach themselves how to search the Internet and how to troubleshoot

technical problems when they occur.

In spite of these problems, the Internet has considerable potential for enhancing students’ learning, precisely

because of its flexibility and near universality. Some of the best recent successes involve the creation of a learning

commons (sometimes also called an information commons or teaching commons), a combination of a website

and an actual, physical place in a school or library that brings together information, students and teachers so that

both (though perhaps especially students) can learn (Haas & Robertson, 2004; Beagle, 2006). A learning commons

includes an online library catalogue and online Internet service, but it also offers other services: online information

and advice about study skills, for example, as well as access to peer tutors and support groups, either online or in

person, that can help with difficulties about writing or doing assignments. As you might suspect, using a learning

commons effectively sometimes requires reorganizing certain features of teaching and learning, chiefly toward

greater explicit collaboration among students and teachers.

227

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Using local experts and field trips

Two other ways of enhancing learning include bringing local experts to the classroom and taking the class on

field trips outside the classroom. Both of these strategies help to make learning more vivid, as well as more relevant

to the particular community and lives that students lead.

Local experts

Classroom visits by persons with key experience can often add a lot to many curriculum subjects and topics. In

one tenth grade science class studying environmental issues, for example, the teacher invited the city forester, the

person responsible for the health of trees planted in city parks and along city boulevards. The forester had special

knowledge of the stresses on trees in urban environments, and he was able to explain and give examples of

particular problems that had occurred and their solutions. In a second grade class with many Hispanic students, on

the other hand, a teacher aide was able to serve as an expert visitor by describing her memories of childhood in a

Spanish-speaking community in New Mexico. Later she also recruited an older Hispanic friend and relative to the

class to describe their experiences growing up in Central America. She also acted as their English-Spanish

interpreter. In all of these examples, the experts made the learning more real and immediate. Their presence

counteracted the tendency to equate school learning with book-based knowledge—a common hazard when basing

instructional planning primarily on curriculum documents.

Field trips

In addition to bringing the world to the classroom by inviting visitors, teachers can do the converse, they can

take the classroom to the world by leading students on field trips. Such trips are not confined to any particular

grade level. In the early grades of elementary school, for example, one common goal of the curriculum is to learn

about community helpers—the police, firefighters, store owners, and others who make a community safe and

livable. As indicated already, representatives of these groups can visit the class and tell about their work. But the

class can also visit the places which these people tell about: a police station, a fire hall, a local retail store, and the

like. Such trips offer a more complete picture of the context in which community professionals work than is

possible simply from hearing and reading about it. The benefits are possible for older students as well. In learning

about water-borne diseases as part of a biology class, for example, one middle-school class took a field trip to the

local water-treatment facility, where staff members explained where the town’s water came from and how the water

was cleaned to become drinkable at any tap.

From a teacher’s point of view, of course, there are certain risks about arranging classroom visitors or field trips.

One is that a visitor may turn out not to communicate well with children or young people—he or she may assume

too much prior knowledge, for example, or veer off the chosen topic. Another problem is that field trips often

require additional funds (for admission fees or to pay for a bus), and require support from additional adults—often

parents—to supervise students outside of school. Some of these problems are by-passed by arranging “virtual” field

trips and hearing from “virtual” visitors: using computer software or media to show students places and activities

which they cannot visit in person (Clark, 2006). Generally, though, a computer-based experience cannot compare

with a real trip or visitor in vividness, and the benefits of actual, in-person field trips or visitors often therefore

outweigh the challenges of arranging them.

Educational Psychology 228 A Global Text

10. Planning instruction

Service learning

Still another way to enhance learning is to incorporate service learning, which is activity that combines real

community service with analysis and reflection on the significance of the service (Johnson & O’Grady, 2006;

Thomsen, 2006). Picking up trash in an urban stream bed, for example, is a community service which students can

perform. To transform this service into service learning, students also need to note and reflect on the trash that

they find; talk and write about the ecological environment of the stream and of the community; and even make

recommendations for improving the local environment. To accomplish these objectives, service learning activities

should not be sporadic, nor used as a punishment—as when a teacher or principal assigns trash pick-up as an after-

school detention activity.

Under good conditions, service learning enhances instructional plans both morally and intellectually. Morally, it

places students in the role of creating good for the community, and counteracts students’ perception that being

“good” simply means complying with teachers’ or parents’ rules passively. Intellectually, service learning places

social and community issues in a vivid, lived context. The environment, economic inequality, or race relations, for

example, are no longer just ideas that people merely talk about, but problems that people actually act upon

(Dicklitch, 2005).

As you might suspect, though, making service learning successful is not automatic. For one thing, service

learning lends itself well only to certain curriculum areas (for example, community studies or social studies). For

another, some students may initially resist service learning, wondering whether it benefits them personally as

students (Jones, Gilbride-Brown, & Gasiorski, 2005). Also, some service projects may inadvertently be invented

only to benefit students, without adequate consultation or advice from community members. Bringing food

hampers to low-income families may seem like a good idea to middle-class students or instructors, but some

families may perceive this action less as a benefit than as an act of charity which they therefore resent. But none of

these problems are insurmountable. Evaluations generally find that service learning, when done well, increases

students’ sense of moral empowerment as well as their knowledge of social issues (Buchanan, Baldwin, & Rudisill,

2002). Like many other educational practices, insuring success with service learning requires doing it well.

Creating bridges among curriculum goals and students’ prior experiences

To succeed, then, instructional plans do require a variety of resources, like the ones discussed in the previous

section. But they also require more: they need to connect with students’ prior experiences and knowledge.

Sometimes the connections can develop as a result of using the Internet, taking field trips, or engaging in service

learning, particularly if students are already familiar with these activities and places. More often than not, though,

teachers need to find additional ways to connect curriculum with students’ experiences—ways that fit more

thoroughly and continuously into the daily work of a class. Fortunately, such techniques are readily at hand; they

simply require the teacher to develop a habit of looking for opportunities to use them. Among the possibilities are

four that deserve special mention: (1) modeling behavior and modeling representations of ideas, (2) activating

prior knowledge already familiar to students, (3) anticipating preconceptions held by students, and (4) providing

guided and independent practice, including its most traditional form, homework.

229

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Modeling

The term modeling can mean either a demonstration of a desired behavior or a representation of an important

theory, idea, or object. Each of these meanings can link curriculum goals with students’ prior knowledge and

experience.

Modeling as a demonstration

In the first meaning, modeling refers to performing or demonstrating a desired new behavior or skill, as when

a teacher or classmate demonstrates polite behaviors or the correct solution to a math problem. In this case we say

that the teacher or classmate models the desired behavior, either deliberately or in the course of other ongoing

activity. Students observe the modeled behavior and (hopefully) imitate it themselves. Research repeatedly shows

that modeling desired behaviors is an effective way to learn new behaviors, especially when the model is perceived

as important (like the teacher), similar to the learner (like a student’s best friend), or has a warm, positive

relationship with the learner (like the teacher or the student’s friend) (Bandura, 2002; Gibson, 2004). Modeling in

this sense is sometimes also called observational learning. It has many of the same properties as the classic

operant conditioning discussed in Chapter 2, except that reinforcement during observational learning is witnessed

in others rather than experienced by the learner directly. Watching others being reinforced is sometimes called

vicarious reinforcement. The idea is that if, for example, a student observes a classmate who behaves politely with

the teacher and then sees that classmate receive praise for the behavior (vicarious reinforcement), the student is

more likely to imitate the polite behavior that he saw. As in classic operant conditioning, furthermore, if the student

observes that politeness by classmates is ignored (extinction or no reinforcement), then the student is much less

likely to imitate the politeness. Worse yet, if the student observes that negative behaviors in others lead to positive

consequences (like attention from peers), then the student may imitate the negative behaviors (Rebellon, 2006).

Cursing and swearing, and even bullying or vandalism, can be reinforced vicariously, just as can more desired

behaviors.

Modeling—in this first sense of a demonstration—connects instructional goals to students’ experiences by

presenting real, vivid examples of behaviors or skills in a way that a student can practice directly, rather than

merely talk about. There is often little need, when imitating a model, to translate ideas or instructions from verbal

form into action. For students struggling with language and literacy, in particular, this feature can be a real

advantage.

Modeling—as simplified representation

In a second meaning of modeling, a model is a simplified representation of a phenomenon that incorporates

the important properties of the phenomenon. Models in this sense may sometimes be quite tangible, direct copies

of reality; when I was in fourth grade growing up in California, for example, we made scale models of the Spanish

missions as part of our social studies lessons about California history. But models can also be imaginary, though

still based on familiar elements. In a science curriculum, for example, the behavior of gas molecules under pressure

can be modeled by imagining the molecules as ping pong balls flying about and colliding in an empty room.

Reducing the space available to the gas by making the room smaller, causes the ping pong balls to collide more

frequently and vigorously, and thereby increases the pressure on the walls of the room. Increasing the space has the

opposite effect. Creating an actual room full of ping pong balls may be impractical, of course, but the model can still

be imagined.

Educational Psychology 230 A Global Text

10. Planning instruction

Modeling in this second sense is not about altering students’ behavior, but about increasing their understanding

of a newly learned idea, theory, or phenomenon. The model itself uses objects or events that are already familiar to

students—simple balls and their behavior when colliding—and in this way supports students’ learning of new,

unfamiliar material. Not every new concept or idea lends itself to such modeling, but many do: students can create

models of unfamiliar animals, for example, or of medieval castles, or of ecological systems. Two-dimensional

models—essentially drawings—can also be helpful: students can illustrate literature or historical events, or make

maps of their own neighborhoods. The choice of model depends largely on the specific curriculum goals which the

teacher needs to accomplish at a particular time.

Activating prior knowledge

Another way to connect curriculum goals to students’ experience is by activating prior knowledge, a term

that refers to encouraging students to recall what they know already about new material being learned. Various

formats for activating prior knowledge are possible. When introducing a unit about how biologists classify animal

and plant species, for example, a teacher can invite students to discuss how they already classify different kinds of

plants and animals. Having highlighted this informal knowledge, the teacher can then explore how the same species

are classified by biological scientists, and compare the scientists’ classification schemes to the students’ own

schemes. The activation does not have to happen orally, as in this example; a teacher can also ask students to write

down as many distinct types of animals and plants that they can think of, and then ask students to diagram or map

their relationships—essentially creating a concept map like the ones we described in Chapter 8 (Gurlitt, et al.,

2006). Whatever the strategy used, activation helps by making students’ prior knowledge or experience conscious

and therefore easier to link to new concepts or information.

Anticipating preconceptions of students

Ironically, activating students’ prior knowledge can be a mixed blessing if some of the prior knowledge is

misleading or downright wrong. Misleading or erroneous knowledge is especially common among young students,

but it can happen at any grade level. A kindergarten child may think that the sun literally “rises” in the morning,

since she often hears adults use this expression, or that the earth is flat because it obviously looks flat. But a high

school student may mistakenly believe that large objects (a boulder) fall faster than small ones (a pebble), or that a

heavy object dropped (not thrown) from a moving car window will fall straight down instead of traveling laterally

alongside the car while it falls.

Because misconceptions are quite common among students and even among adults, teachers are more effective

if they can anticipate preconceptions of students wherever possible. The task is twofold. First the teacher

must know or at least guess students’ preconceptions as much as possible in advance, so that she can design

learning activities to counteract and revise their thinking. Some preconceptions have been well-documented by

educational research and therefore can in principle be anticipated easily—though they may still sometimes take a

teacher by surprise during a busy activity or lesson (Tanner & Allen, 2005; Chiu & Lin, 2005). Exhibit 9.8 lists a few

of these common preconceptions. Others may be unique to particular students, however, and a teacher may only by

able to learn of them through experience—by listening carefully to what students say and write and by watching

what they do. A few preconceptions may be so ingrained or tied to other, more deeply held beliefs that students may

resist giving them up, either consciously or unconsciously. It may be hard, for example, for some students to give up

231

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

the idea that girls are less talented at math or science than are boys, even though research generally finds this is not

the case (Hyde & Linn, 2006).

Table 34: Several misconceptions about science

Misconception What to do

Stars and constellations appear in the same place in

the sky every night.

Ask students to observe carefully the locations of a

bright star once a week for several weeks.

The world is flat, circular like a pancake. Use a globe or ball to find countries located over the

horizon; use computer software (e.g. Global Earth) to

illustrate how a round Earth can look flat up close.

Dinosaurs disappeared at the same time that human

beings appeared and because of human activity.

Construct a timeline of major periods of Darwinian

evolution.

Rivers always flow from North to South. Identify rivers that flow South to North (e.g. the Red

River in North Dakota and Canada); talk about how

Southern locations are not necessarily “lower”.

Force is needed not only to start an object moving,

but to keep it moving.

Explain the concept of inertia; demonstrate inertia

using low-friction motion (e.g. with a hovercraft or dry-

ice puck).

Volume, weight, and size are identical concepts. Have students weigh objects of different sizes or

volumes, and compare the results.

Seasons happen because the Earth changes distance

from the sun.

Explain the tilt of Earth’s axis using a globe and light

as a model; demonstrate reduced heating of surfaces by

placing similar surfaces outdoors at different angles to

the sun’s rays.

Sources: Chi, 2005; D. Clark, 2006; Slotta & Chi, 2006; Owens, 2003.

The second task when anticipating preconceptions is to treat students’ existing knowledge and beliefs with

respect even when they do include misconceptions or errors. This may seem obvious in principle, but it needs

remembering when students persist with misconceptions in spite of a teacher’s efforts to teach alternative ideas or

concepts. Most of us—including most students—have reasons for holding our beliefs, even when the beliefs do not

agree with teachers, textbooks, or other authorities, and we appreciate having our beliefs treated with respect.

Students are no different from other people in this regard. In a high school biology class, for example, some

students may have personal reasons for not agreeing with the theory of evolution associated with Charles Darwin.

For religious reasons they may support explanations of the origins of life that give a more active, interventionist

role to God (Brumfiel, 2005). If their beliefs disagree with the teacher’s or the textbook, then the disagreement

needs to be acknowledged, but acknowledged respectfully. For some students (and perhaps some teachers),

expressing fundamental disagreement respectfully may feel awkward, but it needs to be done nonetheless.

Educational Psychology 232 A Global Text

10. Planning instruction

Guided practice, independent practice, and homework

So far, we have focused on bridging the goals or content of a curriculum to events, beliefs, and ideas from

students’ lives. In studying human growth in a health class, for example, a teacher might ask students to bring

photos of themselves as a much younger child. In this case a concept from the curriculum—human growth—then is

related to a personal event, being photographed as a youngster, that the student finds meaningful.

But teachers can also create bridges between curriculum and students’ experiences in another way, by relating

the process of learning in school with the process of learning outside of school. Much of this task involves helping

students to make the transition from supervised learning to self-regulated learning—or put differently, from

practice that is relatively guided to practice that is relatively independent.

Guided practice

When students first learn a new skill or a new set of ideas, they are especially likely to encounter problems and

make mistakes that interfere with the very process of learning. In figuring out how to use a new software program,

for example, a student may unknowingly press a wrong button that prevents further functioning of the program. In

translating sentences from Spanish into English in language class, for another example, a student might

misinterpret one particular word or grammatical feature. This one mistake may cause many sentences to be

translated incorrectly, and so on. So students initially need guided practice—opportunities to work somewhat

independently, but with a teacher or other expert close at hand prevent or fix difficulties when they occur. In

general, educational research has found that guided practice helps all learners, but especially those who are

struggling (Bryan & Burstein, 2004: Woodward, 2004). A first-grade child has difficulty in decoding printed words,

for example, benefits from guidance more than one who can decode easily. But both students benefit in the initial

stages of learning, since both may make more mistakes then. Guided practice, by its nature, sends a dual message to

students: it is important to learn new material well, but it is also important to become able to use learning without

assistance, beyond the lesson where it is learned and even beyond the classroom.

Guided practice is much like the concepts of the zone of proximal development (or ZPD) and instructional

scaffolding that we discussed in Chapter 2 in connection with Vygotsky’s theory of learning. In essence, during

guided practice the teacher creates a ZPD or scaffold (or framework) in which the student can accomplish more

with partial knowledge or skill than the student could accomplish alone. But whatever its name—guided practice, a

ZPD, or a scaffold—insuring success of guidance depends on several key elements: focusing on the task at hand,

asking questions that break the task into manageable parts, reframing or restating the task so that it becomes more

understandable, and giving frequent feedback about the student’s progress (Rogoff, 2003). Combining the elements

appropriately takes sensitivity and improvisational skill—even artfulness—but these very challenges are among the

true joys of teaching.

Independent practice

As students gain facility with a new skill or new knowledge, they tend to need less guidance and more time to

consolidate (or strengthen) their new knowledge with additional practice. Since they are less likely to encounter

mistakes or problems at this point, they begin to benefit from independent practice—opportunities to review

and repeat their knowledge at their own pace and with fewer interruptions. At this point, therefore, guided practice

may feel less like help than like an interruption, even if it is well-intentioned. A student who already knows how to

233

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

use a new computer program, for example, may be frustrated by waiting for the teacher to explain each step of the

program individually. If a student is already skillful at translating Spanish sentences into English in a language

class, it can be annoying for the teacher to “help” by pointing out minor errors that the student is likely to catch for

herself.

By definition, the purpose of independent practice is to provide more self-regulation of learning than what

comes from guided practice. It implies a different message for students than what is conveyed by guided practice, a

message that goes beyond the earlier one: that it is now time to take more complete responsibility for own learning.

When all goes well, independent practice is the eventual outcome of the zone of proximal development created

during the earlier phase of guided practice described above: the student can now do on his or her own, what

originally required assistance from someone else. Or stated differently, independent practice is a way of

encouraging self-determination about learning, in the sense that we discussed this idea in Chapter 6. In order to

work independently, a student must set his or her own direction and monitor his or her own success; by definition,

no one can do this for the student.

Homework

The chances are that you already have experienced many forms of homework in your own educational career.

The widespread practice of assigning review work to do outside of school is a way of supplementing scarce time in

class and of providing independent practice for students. Homework has generated controversy throughout most of

its history in public education, partly because it encroaches on students’ personal and family-oriented time, and

partly because research finds no consistent benefits of doing homework (Gill & Schlossman, 2004; Kohn, 2004). In

spite of these criticisms, though, parents and teachers tend to favor homework when it is used for two main

purposes. One purpose is to review and practice material that has already been introduced and practiced at school;

a sheet of arithmetic problems might be a classic example. When used for this purpose, the amount of homework is

usually minimal in the earliest grades, if any is assigned at all. One educational expert recommends only ten

minutes per day in first grade at most, and only gradual increases in amount as students get older (Cooper &

Valentine, 2001).

The second purpose for supporting homework is to convey the idea of schoolwork being the “job” of childhood

and youth. Just as on an adult job, students must complete homework tasks with minimal supervision and

sometimes even minimal training. Doing the tasks, furthermore, is a way to get ahead or further along in the work

place (for an adult) or at school (for a child). One study in which researchers interviewed children about these ideas,

in fact, found that children do indeed regard homework as work in the same way that adults think of a job (Cornu &

Xu, 2004). In the children’s minds, homework tasks were not “fun”, in spite of teachers’ frequent efforts to make

them fun. Instead they were jobs that needed doing, much like household chores. When it came to homework,

children regarded parents as the teachers’ assistants—people merely carrying out the wishes of the teacher. Like any

job, the job of doing homework varied in stressfulness; when required at an appropriate amount and level of

difficulty, and when children reported having good “bosses” (parents and teachers), the job of homework could

actually be satisfying in the way that many adults’ jobs can be satisfying when well-done.

Educational Psychology 234 A Global Text

10. Planning instruction

Planning for instruction as well as for learning

This chapter started with one premise but ended with another. It started with the idea that teachers need to

locate curriculum goals, usually from a state department of education or a publisher of a curriculum document. In

much of the chapter we described what these authorities provide for individual classroom teachers, and how their

documents can be clarified and rendered specific enough for classroom use. In the middle of the chapter, however,

the premise shifted. We began noting that instruction cannot be planned simply for students; teachers also need to

consider involving students themselves in influencing or even choosing their own goals and ways of reaching the

goals. Instructional planning, in other words, should not be just for students, but also by students, at least to some

extent. In the final parts of the chapter we described a number of ways of achieving a reasonable balance between

teachers’ and students’ influence on their learning. We suggested considering relatively strong measures, such as an

emergent or an anti-bias curriculum, but we also considered more moderate ones, like the use of the Internet, of

local experts and field trips, of service learning, and of guided and independent practice. All things considered,

then, teachers’ planning is not just about organizing teaching; it is also about facilitating learning. Its dual purpose

is evident in many features of public education, including the one we discuss in the next two chapters, the

assessment of learning.

Chapter summary

In the United States, broad educational goals for most subject areas are published by many national

professional associations and by all state departments of education. Usually the state departments of education also

publish curriculum framework or curriculum guides that offer somewhat more specific explanations of educational

goals, and how they might be taught.

Transforming the goals into specific learning objectives, however, remains a responsibility of the teacher. The

formulation can focus on curriculum topics that can analyzed into specific activities, or it can focus on specific

behaviors expected of students and assembled into general types of outcomes. Taxonomies of educational

objectives, such as the ones originated by Benjamin Bloom, are a useful tool with either approach to instructional

planning.

In addition to planning instruction on students’ behalf, many teachers organize instruction so that students

themselves can influence the choice of goals. One way to do so is through emergent curriculum; another way is

through multicultural and anti-bias curriculum.

Whatever planning strategies are used, learning is enhanced by using a wide variety of resources, including the

Internet, local experts, field trips, and service learning, among others. It is also enhanced if the teacher can build

bridges between curriculum goals and students’ experiences through judicious use of modeling, activation of prior

knowledge, anticipation of students’ preconceptions, and an appropriate blend of guided and independent practice.

Key termsAffective objectives

Anti-bias educationBloom’s taxonomy

Content integrationCurriculum framework

Curriculum guide

235

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Educational goalsEmergent curriculum

Equity pedagogyGuided practice

Independent practiceIndicators

Instructional planningLearning commons

Learning objectivesModeling as demonstration

Modeling as simplified representationMulticultural education

National standardsPsychomotor objectives

Scope and sequenceService learning

State standardsTaxonomy of educational objectives

On the Internet

These are two of many websites that explains what behavioral objectives are, and how to write them. They give

more detail than is possible in this chapter.

This page is part of the website for the

Anti-Defamation League of America, an organization dedicated to eliminating racial and ethnic bias throughout

society. This particular page explains the concept of anti-bias education, but it also has links to pages that contain

tips for teachers dealing with racial and ethnic bias.

This website contains links to educational standards

documents written by every major state department of education and a number of national and professional

associations. It covers all of the major subjects commonly taught in public schools.

References

Anderson, L. & Krathwohl, D. (2001). A taxonomy for learning, teaching, and assessing. New York:

Longman.

Bandura, A. (2002). Social cognitive theory in cultural context. Journal of Applied Psychology: An

International Review, 51, 269-290.

Banks, C. & Banks, J. (1995). Equity pedagogy: An essential component of multicultural education. Theory

into Practice, 34(3), 152-158.

Banks, J. & Banks, C. (2005). Multicultural education: Issues and perspectives, 5th edition. New York: Wiley.

Beagle, D. Bailey, R., & Tierney, B. (2004). The information commons handbook. New York: Neal-Shuman

Publishers.

Educational Psychology 236 A Global Text

10. Planning instruction

Bloom, B. (1956). Taxonomy of educational objectives. New York: David McKay Publishers.

Brumfiel, G. (2005). Intelligent design: Who has designs on your students’ minds? Nature, 434, 1062-1065.

Bryan, T. & Burstein, K. (2004). Improving homework completion and academic performance: Lessons from

special education. Theory into Practice, 43(3), 213-219.

Buchanan, A., Baldwin, S., & Rudisill, M. (2002). Service learning as scholarship in teacher education.

Educational Researcher 32(8), 28-34.

Chi, M. (2005). Commonsense conceptions of emergent processes: Why some misconceptions are robust.

Journal of the Learning Sciences, 14(2), 161-199.

Chiu, M. & Lin, J. (2005). Promoting 4th-graders’ conceptual change of their understanding of electrical

current via multiple analogies. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 42(4), 429-464.

Clark, D. (2006). Longitudinal conceptual change in students’ understanding of thermal equilibrium: An

examination of the process of conceptual restructuring. Cognition and Instruction, 24(4), 467-563.

Clark, K. (2006). Computer Based Virtual Field Trips in the K-12 Classroom. In C. Crawford et al. (Eds.),

Proceedings of Society for Information Technology and Teacher Education International Conference

2006, pp. 3974-3980. Chesapeake, VA: AACE.

Cooper, H. & Valentine, J. (2001). Using research to answer practical questions about homework.

Educational Psychology, 36(3), 143-153.

Corno, L. & Xu, J. (2004). Homework as the job of childhood. Theory into Practice, 43(3), 227-233.

Crow, C. (2005). Multicultural education: Equity pedagogy on perspectives and practices of secondary

teachers. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Waco, TX: Baylor University.

Curriculum Development and Supplemental materials Commission. (1999).

Reading/Language Arts Framework for California Public Schools. Sacramento, CA:

California Department of Education.

Derman-Sparks, L. (1994). Empowering children to create a caring culture in a world of differences.

Childhood Education, 70, 66-71.

Dicklitch, S. (2005). Human rights—human wrongs: Making political science real through service-learning.

In D. Butin (Ed.), Service-learning in higher education: Critical issues and directions, pp. xxx-yyy. New

York: Palgrave Macmillan.

Egan, K. (2005). An imaginative approach to teaching. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Gibson, S. (2004). Social learning (cognitive) theory and implications for human resources development.

Advances in Developing Human Resources, 6(2), 192-210.

Gill, B. & Schlossman, S. (2004). Villain or savior? The American discourse on homework, 1850-2003.

Theory into practice, 43(3), 174-181.

237

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Gronlund, N. (2004). Writing instructional objectives for teaching and assessment, 6th edition. Upper

Saddle River, NJ: Pearson.

Gulitt, J., Renkl, A., Motes, M., & Hauser, S. (2006). How can we use concept maps for prior knowledge

activation? Proceedings of the 7th International Conference on Learning Sciences, 217-220.

Haas, L. & Robertson, J. (Eds). (2004). The information commons. Washington, D.C.: Association of

Research Libraries.

Harrow, A. (1972). A taxonomy of the psychomotor domain. New York: David McKay.

Hawkins, J. (2006). Accessing multicultural issues through critical thinking, critical inquiry, and the student

research process. Urban Education, 41(2), 169-141.

Haywood, K. & Getchell, N. (2005). Life span motor development, 4th edition. Champaign, IL: Human

Kinetics Press.

Hyde, J. & Lynn, M. (2006). Gender similarities in mathematics and science. Science, 314(5799), 599-600.

Jacobson, T. (2003). Confronting out discomfort: Clearing the way for anti-bias in early childhood.

Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.

Johnson, B. & O’Grady, C. (Eds.). (2006). The spirit of service: Exploring faith, service, and social justice in

higher education. Bolton, MA: Anker Publishers.

Jones, S., Gilbride-Brown, J., & Gasiorski, A. (2005). Getting inside the “underside” of service-learning:

Student resistance and possibilities. In D. Butin (Ed.), Service-learning in higher education: Critical

issues and directions, pp. xxx-yyy. New York: Palgrave Macmillan.

Kohn, A. (1999). The schools our children deserve. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

Kohn, A. (2004). Challenging students, and how to have more of them. Phi Delta Kappan,86(3), 184-194.

Lester, P. & Ross, S. (2003). Images that injure: Pictorial stereotypes in the media. Westport, CT: Praeger.

Mager, R. (2005). Preparing instructional objectives, 3rd edition. Atlanta, GA: Center for Effective

Performance.

Marzano, R. (2006). Designing a new taxonomy of educational objectives. Thousand Oaks, CA: Corwin

Books.

Notar, C., Wilson, J., Yunker, B., & Zuelke, D. (2004). The table of specifications: Insuring accountability in

teacher-made tests. Journal of Instructional Psychology, 31(3).

Ohio Department of Education. (2003). Academic Content Standards. Columbus, Ohio: Author.

Owens, C. (2003). Nonsense, sense and science: Misconceptions and illustrated trade books. Journal of

children’s literature, 29(1), 55-62.

Parsad, B. & Jones, J. (2005). Internet access in U.S. public schools and classrooms: 1994-2003.

Washington, D.C.: United States Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics.

Educational Psychology 238 A Global Text

10. Planning instruction

Perkins, D. & Mebert, C. (2005). Efficacy of multicultural education for preschool children. Journal of Cross-

Cultural Psychology, 36(4), 497-512.

Peterson, E. (2002). A practical guide to early childhood curriculum: Linking thematic, emergent, and

skill-based planning to children’s outcomes, 2nd edition. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.

Popham, J. (2002). What every teacher should know about educational assessment. Upper Saddle River,

NJ: Pearson.

Ragains, P. (2006). Information literacy instruction that works. New York: Neal-Schuman Publishers.

Rebellon, C. (2006). Do adolescents engage in delinquency to attract the social attention of peers? An

extension and longitudinal test of the social reinforcement hypothesis. Journal of Research in Crime and

Delinquency, 43(4), 387-411.

Riley, R. (2002). Education reform through standards and partnerships, 1993-2000. Phi Delta Kappan,

83(9), 700-707.

Rogoff, B. (2003). Cultural nature of human development, Chapter 7, “Thinking with the tools and

institutions of culture,” pp. 236-281.

Seitz, H. (2006). The plan: building on children’s interests. Young Children, 61(2), 36-41.

Skinner, H., Biscope, S., & Poland, B. (2003). Quality of Internet access: Barriers behind Internet use

statistics, 57(5), 875-880.

Slotta, J. & Chi, M. (2006). Helping students understand challenging topics in science through ontology

training. Cognition and Instruction, 24(2), 261-289.

Tanner, K. & Allen, D. (2005). Approaches to biology teaching and learning—understanding the wrong

answers: Teaching toward conceptual change. Cell Biology Education, 4, 112-117.

Thomsen, K. (2006). Service-learning in grades K-8: Experiential learning that builds character and

motivation. Thousand Oaks, CA: Corwin.

Vartuli, S. & Rohs, J. (2006). Conceptual organizers of early childhood curriculum content. Early Childhood

Education Journal, 33(4), 231-237.

Vavrus, M. (2002). Transforming the multicultural education of teachers. New York: Teachers’ College

Press.

Woodward, J. (2004). Mathematics education in the United States: Past to present Journal of Learning

Disabilities, 37, pp. 16-31.

Wurm, J. (2005). Working in the Reggio way. St. Paul, MN: Redleaf Press.

239

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

11. Teacher-made assessment strategies

Kym teaches sixth grade students in an urban school where most of the families in the community

live below the poverty line. Each year the majority of the students in her school fail the state-wide

tests. Kym follows school district teaching guides and typically uses direct instruction in her

Language Arts and Social Studies classes. The classroom assessments are designed to mirror those

on the state-wide tests so the students become familiar with the assessment format. When Kym is in a

graduate summer course on motivation she reads an article called, “Teaching strategies that honor

and motivate inner-city African American students” (Teel, Debrin-Parecki, & Covington, 1998) and

she decides to change her instruction and assessment in fall in four ways. First, she stresses an

incremental approach to ability focusing on effort and allows students to revise their work several

times until the criteria are met. Second, she gives students choices in performance assessments (e.g.

oral presentation, art project, creative writing). Third, she encourages responsibility by asking

students to assist in classroom tasks such as setting up video equipment, handing out papers etc.

Fourth, she validates student’ cultural heritage by encouraging them to read biographies and

historical fiction from their own cultural backgrounds. Kym reports that the changes in her students’

effort and demeanor in class are dramatic: students are more enthusiastic, work harder, and

produce better products. At the end of the year twice as many of her students pass the State-wide test

than the previous year.

Afterward. Kym still teaches sixth grade in the same school district and continues to modify the

strategies described above. Even though the performance of the students she taught improved the

school was closed because, on average, the students’ performance was poor. Kym gained a Ph.D and

teaches Educational Psychology to preservice and inservice teachers in evening classes.

Kym’s story illustrates several themes related to assessment that we explore in this chapter on teacher-made

assessment strategies and in the Chapter 12 on standardized testing. First, choosing effective classroom

assessments is related to instructional practices, beliefs about motivation, and the presence of state-wide

standardized testing. Second, some teacher-made classroom assessments enhance student learning and motivation

—some do not. Third, teachers can improve their teaching through action research. This involves identifying a

problem (e.g. low motivation and achievement), learning about alternative approaches (e.g. reading the literature),

implementing the new approaches, observing the results (e.g. students’ effort and test results), and continuing to

modify the strategies based on their observations.

Best practices in assessing student learning have undergone dramatic changes in the last 20 years. When

Rosemary was a mathematics teacher in the 1970s, she did not assess students’ learning she tested them on the

Educational Psychology 240 A Global Text

11. Teacher-made assessment strategies

mathematics knowledge and skills she taught during the previous weeks. The tests varied little format and students

always did them individually with pencil and paper. Many teachers, including mathematics teachers, now use a

wide variety of methods to determine what their students have learned and also use this assessment information to

modify their instruction. In this chapter the focus is on using classroom assessments to improve student learning

and we begin with some basic concepts.

Basic concepts

Assessment is an integrated process of gaining information about students’ learning and making value

judgments about their progress (Linn & Miller, 2005). Information about students’ progress can be obtained from

a variety of sources including projects, portfolios, performances, observations, and tests. The information about

students’ learning is often assigned specific numbers or grades and this involves measurement. Measurement

answers the question, “How much?” and is used most commonly when the teacher scores a test or product and

assigns numbers (e.g. 28 /30 on the biology test; 90/100 on the science project). Evaluation is the process of

making judgments about the assessment information (Airasian, 2005). These judgments may be about individual

students (e.g. should Jacob’s course grade take into account his significant improvement over the grading period?),

the assessment method used (e.g. is the multiple choice test a useful way to obtain information about problem

solving), or one’s own teaching (e.g. most of the students this year did much better on the essay assignment than

last year so my new teaching methods seem effective).

The primary focus in this chapter is on assessment for learning, where the priority is designing and using

assessment strategies to enhance student learning and development. Assessment for learning is often formative

assessment, i.e. it takes place during the course of instruction by providing information that teachers can use to

revise their teaching and students can use to improve their learning (Black, Harrison, Lee, Marshall & Wiliam,

2004). Formative assessment includes both informal assessment involving spontaneous unsystematic

observations of students’ behaviors (e.g. during a question and answer session or while the students are working on

an assignment) and formal assessment involving pre-planned, systematic gathering of data. Assessment of

learning is formal assessment that involves assessing students in order to certify their competence and fulfill

accountability mandates and is the primary focus of the next chapter on standardized tests but is also considered in

this chapter. Assessment of learning is typically summative, that is, administered after the instruction is

completed (e.g. a final examination in an educational psychology course). Summative assessments provide

information about how well students mastered the material, whether students are ready for the next unit, and what

grades should be given (Airasian, 2005).

Assessment for learning: an overview of the process

Using assessment to advance students’ learning not just check on learning requires viewing assessment as a

process that is integral to the all phases of teaching including planning, classroom interactions and instruction,

communication with parents, and self-reflection (Stiggins, 2002). Essential steps in assessment for learning

include:

Step 1: Having clear instructional goals and communicating them to students

In the previous chapter we documented the importance of teachers thinking carefully about the purposes of each

lesson and unit. This may be hard for beginning teachers. For example, Vanessa, a middle school social studies

241

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

teacher, might say that the goal of her next unit is: “Students will learn about the Cvil War.” Clearer goals require

that Vanessa decides what it is about the US Civil War she wants her students to learn, e.g. the dates and names of

battles, the causes of the US Civil War, the differing perspectives of those living in the North and the South, or the

day-to-day experiences of soldiers fighting in the war. Vanessa cannot devise appropriate assessments of her

students’ learning about the US Civil War until she is clear about her own purposes.

For effective teaching Vanessa also needs to communicate clearly the goals and objectives to her students so they

know what is important for them to learn. No matter how thorough a teacher’s planning has been, if students do

not know what they are supposed to learn they will not learn as much. Because communication is so important to

teachers a specific chapter is devoted to this topic (Chapter 8), and so communication is not considered in any

detail in this chapter.

Step 2: Selecting appropriate assessment techniques

Selecting and administrating assessment techniques that are appropriate for the goals of instruction as well as

the developmental level of the students are crucial components of effective assessment for learning. Teachers need

to know the characteristics of a wide variety of classroom assessment techniques and how these techniques can be

adapted for various content, skills, and student characteristics. They also should understand the role reliability,

validity, and the absence of bias should play is choosing and using assessment techniques. Much of this chapter

focuses on this information.

Step 3: Using assessment to enhance motivation and confidence

Students’ motivation and confidence is influenced by the type of assessment used as well as the feedback given

about the assessment results. Consider, Samantha a college student who takes a history class in which the

professor’s lectures and text book focus on really interesting major themes. However, the assessments are all

multiple choice tests that ask about facts and Samantha, who initially enjoys the classes and readings, becomes

angry, loses confidence she can do well, and begins to spend less time on the class material. In contrast, some

instructors have observed that that many students in educational psychology classes like the one you are now taking

will work harder on assessments that are case studies rather than more traditional exams or essays. The type of

feedback provided to students is also important and we elaborate on these ideas later in this chapter.

Step 4: Adjusting instruction based on information

An essential component of assessment for learning is that the teacher uses the information gained from

assessment to adjust instruction. These adjustments occur in the middle of a lesson when a teacher may decide that

students’ responses to questions indicate sufficient understanding to introduce a new topic, or that her observations

of students’ behavior indicates that they do not understand the assignment and so need further explanation.

Adjustments also occur when the teacher reflects on the instruction after the lesson is over and is planning for the

next day. We provide examples of adjusting instruction in this chapter and consider teacher reflection in more

detail in Appendix C..

Step 5: Communicating with parents and guardians

Students’ learning and development is enhanced when teachers communicate with parents regularly about their

children’s performance. Teachers communicate with parents in a variety of ways including newsletters, telephone

Educational Psychology 242 A Global Text

11. Teacher-made assessment strategies

conversations, email, school district websites and parent-teachers conferences. Effective communication requires

that teachers can clearly explain the purpose and characteristics of the assessment as well as the meaning of

students’ performance. This requires a thorough knowledge of the types and purposes of teacher made and

standardized assessments (this chapter and Chapter 12) and well as clear communication skills (Chapter 8).

We now consider each step in the process of assessment for learning in more detail. In order to be able to select

and administer appropriate assessment techniques teachers need to know about the variety of techniques that can

be used as well as what factors ensure that the assessment techniques are high quality. We begin by considering

high quality assessments.

Selecting appropriate assessment techniques I: high quality assessments

For an assessment to be high quality it needs to have good validity and reliability as well as absence from bias.

Validity

Validity is the evaluation of the “adequacy and appropriateness of the interpretations and uses of assessment

results” for a given group of individuals (Linn & Miller, 2005, p. 68). For example, is it appropriate to conclude that

the results of a mathematics test on fractions given to recent immigrants accurately represents their understanding

of fractions? Is it appropriate for the teacher to conclude, based on her observations, that a kindergarten student,

Jasmine, has Attention Deficit Disorder because she does not follow the teachers oral instructions? Obviously in

each situation other interpretations are possible that the immigrant students have poor English skills rather than

mathematics skills, or that Jasmine may be hearing impaired.

It is important to understand that validity refers to the interpretation and uses made of the results of an

assessment procedure not of the assessment procedure itself. For example, making judgments about the results of

the same test on fractions may be valid if the students all understand English well. A teacher concluding from her

observations that the kindergarten student has Attention Deficit Disorder (ADD) may be appropriate if the student

has been screened for hearing and other disorders (although the classification of a disorder like ADD cannot be

made by one teacher). Validity involves making an overall judgment of the degree to which the interpretations and

uses of the assessment results are justified. Validity is a matter of degree (e.g. high, moderate, or low validity)

rather than all-or none (e.g. totally valid vs invalid) (Linn & Miller, 2005).

Three sources of evidence are considered when assessing validity—content, construct and predictive. Content

validity evidence is associated with the question: How well does the assessment include the content or tasks it is

supposed to? For example, suppose your educational psychology instructor devises a mid-term test and tells you

this includes chapters one to seven in the text book. Obviously, all the items in test should be based on the content

from educational psychology, not your methods or cultural foundations classes. Also, the items in the test should

cover content from all seven chapters and not just chapters three to seven—unless the instructor tells you that these

chapters have priority.

Teachers’ have to be clear about their purposes and priorities for instruction before they can begin to gather

evidence related content validity. Content validation determines the degree that assessment tasks are relevant and

representative of the tasks judged by the teacher (or test developer) to represent their goals and objectives (Linn &

Miller, 2005). It is important for teachers to think about content validation when devising assessment tasks and

one way to help do this is to devise a Table of Specifications. An example, based on Pennsylvania’s State standards

243

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

for grade 3 geography, is in . In the left hand column is the instructional content for a 20-item test the teacher has

decided to construct with two kinds of instructional objectives: identification and uses or locates. The second and

third columns identify the number of items for each content area and each instructional objective. Notice that the

teacher has decided that six items should be devoted to the sub area of geographic representations- more than any

other sub area. Devising a table of specifications helps teachers determine if some content areas or concepts are

over-sampled (i.e. there are too many items) and some concepts are under-sampled (i.e. there are too few items).

Table 35: Example of Table of Specifications: grade 3 basic geography literacy

Content Instructional objective

Identifies Uses or

locates

Total

number of

items

Per cent

of items

Identify geography tools and their uses

Geographic representations: e.g. maps,

globe, diagrams and photographs3 3 6 30%

Spatial information: sketch & thematic

maps 1 1 2 10%

Mental maps 1 1 2 10%

Identify and locate places and regions

Physical features (e.g. lakes, continents) 1 2 3 15%

Human features (countries, states, cities) 3 2 5 25%

Regions with unifying geographic

characteristics e.g. river basins1 1 2 10%

Number of items 10 10 20

Percentage of items 50% 50% 100%

Construct validity evidence is more complex than content validity evidence. Often we are interested in making

broader judgments about student’s performances than specific skills such as doing fractions. The focus may be on

constructs such as mathematical reasoning or reading comprehension. A construct is a characteristic of a person we

assume exists to help explain behavior. For example, we use the concept of test anxiety to explain why some

individuals when taking a test have difficulty concentrating, have physiological reactions such as sweating, and

perform poorly on tests but not in class assignments. Similarly mathematics reasoning and reading comprehension

are constructs as we use them to help explain performance on an assessment. Construct validation is the process of

determining the extent to which performance on an assessment can be interpreted in terms of the intended

constructs and is not influenced by factors irrelevant to the construct. For example, judgments about recent

Educational Psychology 244 A Global Text

11. Teacher-made assessment strategies

immigrants’ performance on a mathematical reasoning test administered in English will have low construct validity

if the results are influenced by English language skills that are irrelevant to mathematical problem solving.

Similarly, construct validity of end-of-semester examinations is likely to be poor for those students who are highly

anxious when taking major tests but not during regular class periods or when doing assignments. Teachers can help

increase construct validity by trying to reduce factors that influence performance but are irrelevant to the construct

being assessed. These factors include anxiety, English language skills, and reading speed (Linn & Miller 2005).

A third form of validity evidence is called criterion-related validity. Selective colleges in the USA use the

ACT or SAT among other criteria to choose who will be admitted because these standardized tests help predict

freshman grades, i.e. have high criterion-related validity. Some K-12 schools give students math or reading tests in

the fall semester in order to predict which are likely to do well on the annual state tests administered in the spring

semester and which students are unlikely to pass the tests and will need additional assistance. If the tests

administered in fall do not predict students’ performances accurately then the additional assistance may be given to

the wrong students illustrating the importance of criterion-related validity.

Reliability

Reliability refers to the consistency of the measurement (Linn & Miller 2005). Suppose Mr Garcia is teaching a

unit on food chemistry in his tenth grade class and gives an assessment at the end of the unit using test items from

the teachers’ guide. Reliability is related to questions such as: How similar would the scores of the students be if

they had taken the assessment on a Friday or Monday? Would the scores have varied if Mr Garcia had selected

different test items, or if a different teacher had graded the test? An assessment provides information about

students by using a specific measure of performance at one particular time. Unless the results from the assessment

are reasonably consistent over different occasions, different raters, or different tasks (in the same content domain)

confidence in the results will be low and so cannot be useful in improving student learning.

Obviously we cannot expect perfect consistency. Students’ memory, attention, fatigue, effort, and anxiety

fluctuate and so influence performance. Even trained raters vary somewhat when grading assessment such as

essays, a science project, or an oral presentation. Also, the wording and design of specific items influence students’

performances. However, some assessments are more reliable than others and there are several strategies teachers

can use to increase reliability

First, assessments with more tasks or items typically have higher reliability. To understand this, consider two

tests one with five items and one with 50 items. Chance factors influence the shorter test more then the longer test.

If a student does not understand one of the items in the first test the total score is very highly influenced (it would

be reduced by 20 per cent). In contrast, if there was one item in the test with 50 items that were confusing, the total

score would be influenced much less (by only 2 percent). Obviously this does not mean that assessments should be

inordinately long, but, on average, enough tasks should be included to reduce the influence of chance variations.

Second, clear directions and tasks help increase reliability. If the directions or wording of specific tasks or items are

unclear, then students have to guess what they mean undermining the accuracy of their results. Third, clear scoring

criteria are crucial in ensuring high reliability (Linn & Miller, 2005). Later in this chapter we describe strategies for

developing scoring criteria for a variety of types of assessment.

245

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Absence of bias

Bias occurs in assessment when there are components in the assessment method or administration of the

assessment that distort the performance of the student because of their personal characteristics such as gender,

ethnicity, or social class (Popham, 2005). Two types of assessment bias are important: offensiveness and unfair

penalization. An assessment is most likely to be offensive to a subgroup of students when negative stereotypes are

included in the test. For example, the assessment in a health class could include items in which all the doctors were

men and all the nurses were women. Or, a series of questions in a social studies class could portray Latinos and

Asians as immigrants rather than native born Americans. In these examples, some female, Latino or Asian students

are likely to be offended by the stereotypes and this can distract them from performing well on the assessment.

Unfair penalization occurs when items disadvantage one group not because they may be offensive but because of

differential background experiences. For example, an item for math assessment that assumes knowledge of a

particular sport may disadvantage groups not as familiar with that sport (e.g. American football for recent

immigrants). Or an assessment on team work that asks students to model their concept of a team on a symphony

orchestra is likely to be easier for those students who have attended orchestra performances—probably students

from affluent families. Unfair penalization does not occur just because some students do poorly in class. For

example, asking questions about a specific sport in a physical education class when information on that sport had

been discussed in class is not unfair penalization as long as the questions do not require knowledge beyond that

taught in class that some groups are less likely to have.

It can be difficult for new teachers teaching in multi-ethnic classrooms to devise interesting assessments that do

not penalize any groups of students. Teachers need to think seriously about the impact of students’ differing

backgrounds on the assessment they use in class. Listening carefully to what students say is important as is learning

about the backgrounds of the students.

Selecting appropriate assessment techniques II: types of teacher-made assessments

One of the challenges for beginning teachers is to select and use appropriate assessment techniques. In this

section we summarize the wide variety of types of assessments that classroom teachers use. First we discuss the

informal techniques teachers use during instruction that typically require instantaneous decisions. Then we

consider formal assessment techniques that teachers plan before instruction and allow for reflective decisions.

Teachers’ observation, questioning, and record keeping

During teaching, teachers not only have to communicate the information they planned but also continuously

monitor students’ learning and motivation in order to determine whether modifications have to be made (Airasian,

2005). Beginning teachers find this more difficult than experienced teachers because of the complex cognitive skills

required to improvise and be responsive to students needs while simultaneously keeping in mind the goals and

plans of the lesson (Borko & Livingston, 1989). The informal assessment strategies teachers most often use during

instruction are observation and questioning.

Observation

Effective teachers observe their students from the time they enter the classroom. Some teachers greet their

students at the door not only to welcome them but also to observe their mood and motivation. Are Hannah and

Naomi still not talking to each other? Does Ethan have his materials with him? Gaining information on such

Educational Psychology 246 A Global Text

11. Teacher-made assessment strategies

questions can help the teacher foster student learning more effectively (e.g. suggesting Ethan goes back to his locker

to get his materials before the bell rings or avoiding assigning Hannah and Naomi to the same group).

During instruction, teachers observe students’ behavior to gain information about students’ level of interest and

understanding of the material or activity. Observation includes looking at non-verbal behaviors as well as listening

to what the students are saying. For example, a teacher may observe that a number of students are looking out of

the window rather than watching the science demonstration, or a teacher may hear students making comments in

their group indicating they do not understand what they are supposed to be doing. Observations also help teachers

decide which student to call on next, whether to speed up or slow down the pace of the lesson, when more examples

are needed, whether to begin or end an activity, how well students are performing a physical activity, and if there

are potential behavior problems (Airasian, 2005). Many teachers find that moving around the classroom helps

them observe more effectively because they can see more students from a variety of perspectives. However, the fast

pace and complexity of most classrooms makes it difficult for teachers to gain as much information as they want.

Questioning

Teachers ask questions for many instructional reasons including keeping students’ attention on the lesson,

highlighting important points and ideas, promoting critical thinking, allowing students’ to learn from each others

answers, and providing information about students’ learning. Devising good appropriate questions and using

students’ responses to make effective instantaneous instructional decisions is very difficult. Some strategies to

improve questioning include planning and writing down the instructional questions that will be asked, allowing

sufficient wait time for students to respond, listening carefully to what students say rather than listening for what is

expected, varying the types of questions asked, making sure some of the questions are higher level, and asking

follow-up questions.

While the informal assessment based on spontaneous observation and questioning is essential for teaching there

are inherent problems with the validity, reliability and bias in this information (Airasian, 2005; Stiggins 2005). We

summarize these issues and some ways to reduce the problems in Table 35.

Table 36: Validity and reliability of observation and questioning

Problem Strategies to alleviate problem

Teachers lack of objectivity

about overall class

involvement and

understanding

Try to make sure you are not only seeing what you want to see. Teachers

typically want to feel good about their instruction so it is easy to look for positive

student interactions. Occasionally, teachers want to see negative student

reactions to confirm their beliefs about an individual student or class.

Tendency to focus on

process rather than learning

Remember to concentrate on student learning not just involvement. Most of

teachers’ observations focus on process—student attention, facial expressions

posture—rather than pupil learning. Students can be active and engaged but not

developing new skills.

Limited information and

selective sampling

Make sure you observe a variety of students—not just those who are typically

very good or very bad.

247

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Walk around the room to observe more students “up close” and view the room

from multiple perspectives.

Call on a wide variety of students—not just those with their hands up, or those

who are skilled as the subject, or those who sit in a particular place in the room.

Keep records

Fast pace of classrooms

inhibits corroborative

evidence

If you want to know if you are missing important information ask a peer to visit

your classroom and observe the students’ behaviors. Classrooms are complex and

fast paced and one teacher cannot see much of what is going on while trying to

also teach.

Cultural and individual

differences in the meaning of

verbal and non verbal

behaviors

Be cautious in the conclusions that you draw from your observations and

questions. Remember that the meaning and expectations of certain types of

questions, wait time, social distance, and role of “small talk” varies across

cultures (Chapter 4). Some students are quiet because of their personalities not

because they are uninvolved, nor keeping up with the lesson, nor depressed or

tired.

Record keeping

Keeping records of observations improves reliability and can be used to enhance understanding of one student, a

group, or the whole class' interactions. Sometimes this requires help from other teachers. For example, Alexis, a

beginning science teacher is aware of the research documenting that longer wait time enhances students’ learning

(e.g. Rowe, 2003) but is unsure of her behaviors so she asks a colleague to observe and record her wait times

during one class period. Alexis learns her wait times are very short for all students so she starts practicing silently

counting to five whenever she asks students a question.

Teachers can keep anecdotal records about students without help from peers. These records contain

descriptions of incidents of a student’s behavior, the time and place the incident takes place, and a tentative

interpretation of the incident. For example, the description of the incident might involve Joseph, a second grade

student, who fell asleep during the mathematics class on a Monday morning. A tentative interpretation could be the

student did not get enough sleep over the weekend, but alternative explanations could be the student is sick or is on

medications that make him drowsy. Obviously additional information is needed and the teacher could ask Joseph

why he is so sleepy and also observe him to see if he looks tired and sleepy over the next couple of weeks.

Anecdotal records often provide important information and are better than relying on one’s memory but they

take time to maintain and it is difficult for teachers to be objective. For example, after seeing Joseph fall asleep the

teacher may now look for any signs of Joseph’s sleepiness—ignoring the days he is not sleepy. Also, it is hard for

teachers to sample a wide enough range of data for their observations to be highly reliable.

Teachers also conduct more formal observations especially for students with special needs who have IEP’s. An

example of the importance of informal and formal observations in a preschool follows:

Educational Psychology 248 A Global Text

11. Teacher-made assessment strategies

The class of preschoolers in a suburban neighborhood of a large city has eight special needs students

and four students—the peer models—who have been selected because of their well developed

language and social skills. Some of the special needs students have been diagnosed with delayed

language, some with behavior disorders, and several with autism. The students are sitting on the

mat with the teacher who has a box with sets of three “cool” things of varying size (e.g. toy pandas)

and the students are asked to put the things in order by size, big, medium and small. Students who

are able are also requested to point to each item in turn and say “This is the big one”, “This is the

medium one” and “This is the little one”. For some students, only two choices (big and little) are

offered because that is appropriate for their developmental level. The teacher informally observes

that one of the boys is having trouble keeping his legs still so she quietly asks the aid for a weighted

pad that she places on the boy’s legs to help him keep them still. The activity continues and the aide

carefully observes students behaviors and records on IEP progress cards whether a child meets

specific objectives such as: “When given two picture or object choices, Mark will point to the

appropriate object in 80 per cent of the opportunities.” The teacher and aides keep records of the

relevant behavior of the special needs students during the half day they are in preschool. The daily

records are summarized weekly. If there are not enough observations that have been recorded for a

specific objective, the teacher and aide focus their observations more on that child, and if necessary,

try to create specific situations that relate to that objective. At end of each month the teacher

calculates whether the special needs children are meeting their IEP objectives.

Selected response items

Common formal assessment formats used by teachers are multiple choice, matching, and true/false items. In

selected response items students have to select a response provided by the teacher or test developer rather than

constructing a response in their own words or actions. Selected response items do not require that students recall

the information but rather recognize the correct answer. Tests with these items are called objective because the

results are not influenced by scorers’ judgments or interpretations and so are often machine scored. Eliminating

potential errors in scoring increases the reliability of tests but teachers who only use objective tests are liable to

reduce the validity of their assessment because objective tests are not appropriate for all learning goals (Linn &

Miller, 2005). Effective assessment for learning as well as assessment of learning must be based on aligning the

assessment technique to the learning goals and outcomes.

For example, if the goal is for students to conduct an experiment then they should be asked to do that rather that

than being asked about conducting an experiment.

Common problems

Selected response items are easy to score but are hard to devise. Teachers often do not spend enough time

constructing items and common problems include:

1. Unclear wording in the items

• True or False: Although George Washington was born into a wealthy family, his father died when he

was only 11, he worked as a youth as a surveyor of rural lands, and later stood on the balcony of Federal

Hall in New York when he took his oath of office in 1789.

249

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

2. Cues that are not related the content being examined.

• A common clue is that all the true statements on a true/false test or the corrective alternatives on a

multiple choice test are longer than the untrue statements or the incorrect alternatives.

3. Using negatives (or double negatives) the items.

• A poor item. “True or False: None of the steps made by the student was unnecessary.”

• A better item. True or False: “All of the steps were necessary.”

Students often do not notice the negative terms or find them confusing so avoiding them is generally

recommended (Linn & Miller 2005). However, since standardized tests often use negative items, teachers

sometimes deliberately include some negative items to give students practice in responding to that format.

4. Taking sentences directly from textbook or lecture notes.

Removing the words from their context often makes them ambiguous or can change the meaning. For

example, a statement from Chapter 3 taken out of context suggests all children are clumsy. “Similarly with

jumping, throwing and catching: the large majority of children can do these things, though often a bit

clumsily.” A fuller quotation makes it clearer that this sentence refers to 5-year-olds: For some fives, running

still looks a bit like a hurried walk, but usually it becomes more coordinated within a year or two. Similarly

with jumping, throwing and catching: the large majority of children can do these things, though often a bit

clumsily, by the time they start school, and most improve their skills noticeably during the early elementary

years.” If the abbreviated form was used as the stem in a true/false item it would obviously be misleading.

5. Avoid trivial questions

e.g. Jean Piaget was born in what year?

a) 1896

b) 1900

c) 1880

d) 1903

While it important to know approximately when Piaget made his seminal contributions to the understanding of

child development, the exact year of his birth (1880) is not important.

Strengths and weaknesses

All types of selected response items have a number of strengths and weaknesses. True/False items are

appropriate for measuring factual knowledge such as vocabulary, formulae, dates, proper names, and technical

terms. They are very efficient as they use a simple structure that students can easily understand, and take little time

to complete. They are also easier to construct than multiple choice and matching items. However, students have a

50 per cent probability of getting the answer correct through guessing so it can be difficult to interpret how much

students know from their test scores. Examples of common problems that arise when devising true/false items are

in Table 37.

Educational Psychology 250 A Global Text

11. Teacher-made assessment strategies

Table 37: Common errors in selected response items

Type of item Common errors Example

True False The statement is not

absolutely true—typically

because it contains a

broad generalization.

T F The President of the United States is elected to that

office.

This is usually true but the US Vice President can succeed the

President.

The item is opinion

not fact .

T F Education for K-12 students is improved though policies

that support charter schools.

Some people believe this, some do not.

Two ideas are

included in item

T F George H Bush the 40th president of the US was defeated

by William Jefferson Clinton in 1992.

The 1st idea is false; the 2nd is true making it difficult for

students to decide whether to circle T or F.

Irrelevant cues T F The President of the United States is usually elected to

that office.

True items contain the words such as usually generally;

whereas false items contain the terms such as always, all, never.

Matching Columns do not

contain homogeneous

information

Directions: On the line to the US Civil War Battle write the

year or confederate general in Column B.

Column A Column B

Ft Sumter General Stonewall Jackson

2nd Battle of Bull Run General Johnson

Ft Henry 1861

1862

Column B is a mixture of generals and dates.

Too many items in

each list

Lists should be relatively short (4 – 7) in each column. More

than 10 are too confusing.

Responses are not in

logical order

In the example with Spanish and English words should be in

a logical order (they are alphabetical). If the order is not logical,

student spend too much time searching for the correct answer.

251

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Multiple Choice Problem (i.e. the

stem) is not clearly stated

problem

New Zealand

a) Is the worlds’ smallest continent

b) Is home to the kangaroo

c) Was settled mainly by colonists from Great Britain

d) Is a dictatorship

This is really a series of true-false items. Because the correct

answer is c) a better version with the problem in the stem is

Much of New Zealand was settled by colonists from

a) Great Britain

b) Spain

c) France

d) Holland

Some of the

alternatives are not

plausible

Who is best known for their work on the development of the

morality of justice.

1. Gerald Ford

2. Vygotsky

3. Maslow

4. Kohlberg

Obviously Gerald Ford is not a plausible alternative.

Irrelevant cues 1. Correct alternative is longer

2. Incorrect alternatives are not grammatically correct

with the stem

3. Too many correct alternatives are in position “b” or

“c” making it easier for students to guess. All the options

(e.g. a, b, c, d) should be used in approximately equal

frequently (not exact as that also provides clues).

Educational Psychology 252 A Global Text

11. Teacher-made assessment strategies

Use of “All of above” 1. If all of the “above is used” then the other items must

be correct. This means that a student may read the 1st

response, mark it correct and move on. Alternatively, a

student may read the 1st two items and seeing they are

true does nor need to read the other alternatives to know

to circle “all of the above”. The teacher probably does

not want either of these options.

In matching items, two parallel columns containing terms, phrases, symbols, or numbers are presented and the

student is asked to match the items in the first column with those in the second column. Typically there are more

items in the second column to make the task more difficult and to ensure that if a student makes one error they do

not have to make another. Matching items most often are used to measure lower level knowledge such as persons

and their achievements, dates and historical events, terms and definitions, symbols and concepts, plants or animals

and classifications (Linn & Miller, 2005). An example with Spanish language words and their English equivalents is

below:

Directions: On the line to the left of the Spanish word in Column A, write the letter of the English word in

Column B that has the same meaning.

Column A Column B

___ 1. Casa A. Aunt

___ 2. Bebé B. Baby

___ 3. Gata C. Brother

___ 4. Perro D. Cat

___ 5. Hermano E. Dog

F. Father

G. House

While matching items may seem easy to devise it is hard to create homogenous lists. Other problems with

matching items and suggested remedies are in Table 37.

Multiple Choice items are the most commonly used type of objective test items because they have a number of

advantages over other objective test items. Most importantly they can be adapted to assess higher levels thinking

such as application as well as lower level factual knowledge. The first example below assesses knowledge of a

specific fact whereas the second example assesses application of knowledge.

Who is best known for their work on the development of the morality of justice?

a) Erikson

b) Vygotsky

253

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

c) Maslow

d) Kohlberg

Which one of the following best illustrates the law of diminishing returns

a) A factory doubled its labor force and increased production by 50 per cent

b) The demand for an electronic product increased faster than the supply of the product

c) The population of a country increased faster than agricultural self sufficiency

d) A machine decreased in efficacy as its parts became worn out

(Adapted from Linn and Miller 2005, p, 193).

There are several other advantages of multiple choice items. Students have to recognize the correct answer not

just know the incorrect answer as they do in true/false items. Also, the opportunity for guessing is reduced because

four or five alternatives are usually provided whereas in true/false items students only have to choose between two

choices. Also, multiple choice items do not need homogeneous material as matching items do.

However, creating good multiple choice test items is difficult and students (maybe including you) often become

frustrated when taking a test with poor multiple choice items. Three steps have to be considered when constructing

a multiple choice item: formulating a clearly stated problem, identifying plausible alternatives, and removing

irrelevant clues to the answer. Common problems in each of these steps are summarized in Table 38

Constructed response items

Formal assessment also includes constructed response items in which students are asked to recall information

and create an answer—not just recognize if the answer is correct—so guessing is reduced. Constructed response

items can be used to assess a wide variety of kinds of knowledge and two major kinds are discussed: completion or

short answer (also called short response) and extended response.

Completion and short answer

Completion and short answer items can be answered in a word, phrase, number, or symbol. These types of items

are essentially the same only varying in whether the problem is presented as a statement or a question (Linn &

Miller 2005). For example:

Completion: The first traffic light in the US was invented by…………….

Short Answer: Who invented the first traffic light in the US?

These items are often used in mathematics tests, e.g.

3 + 10 = …………..?

If x = 6, what does x(x-1) =……….

Draw the line of symmetry on the following shape

A major advantage of these items is they that they are easy to construct. However, apart from their use in

mathematics they are unsuitable for measuring complex learning outcomes and are often difficult to score.

Completion and short answer tests are sometimes called objective tests as the intent is that there is only one correct

Educational Psychology 254 A Global Text

11. Teacher-made assessment strategies

answer and so there is no variability in scoring but unless the question is phrased very carefully, there are

frequently a variety of correct answers. For example, consider the item

Where was President Lincoln born?………………..

The teacher may expect the answer “in a log cabin” but other correct answers are also “on Sinking Spring Farm”,

“in Hardin County” or “in Kentucky”. Common errors in these items are summarized in Table 38.

Table 38: Common errors in constructed response items

Type of item Common errors Example

Completion

and short answer

There is more than one

possible answer.

e.g. Where was US President Lincoln born? The answer could

be in a log cabin, in Kentucky etc.

Too many blanks are in

the completion item so it is

too difficult or doesn’t

make sense.

e.g. In ….. theory, the first stage, ….. . is when infants process

through their ……. and ….. ………

Clues are given by

length of blanks in

completion items.

e.g. Three states are contiguous to New Hampshire: . ….is to

the West, ……is to the East and ………..…. is to the South.

Extended

Response

Ambiguous questions e.g. Was the US Civil War avoidable?

Students could interpret this question in a wide variety of

ways, perhaps even stating “yes” or “no”. One student may

discuss only political causes another moral, political and

economic causes. There is no guidance in the question for

students.

Poor reliability in

grading

The teacher does not use a scoring rubric and so is

inconsistent in how he scores answers especially unexpected

responses, irrelevant information, and grammatical errors.

Perception of student

influences grading

By spring semester the teacher has developed expectations of

each student’s performance and this influences the grading

(numbers can be used instead of names).

The test consists of three constructed responses and the

teacher grades the three answers on each students’ paper before

moving to the next paper. This means that the grading of

questions 2 and 3 are influenced by the answers to question 1

(teachers should grade all the 1st question then the 2nd etc).

Choices are given on Testing experts recommend not giving choices in tests

255

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

the test and some answers

are easier than others.

because then students are not really taking the same test creating

equity problems.

Extended response

Extended response items are used in many content areas and answers may vary in length from a paragraph to

several pages. Questions that require longer responses are often called essay questions. Extended response items

have several advantages and the most important is their adaptability for measuring complex learning outcomes—

particularly integration and application. These items also require that students write and therefore provide teachers

a way to assess writing skills. A commonly cited advantage to these items is their ease in construction; however,

carefully worded items that are related to learning outcomes and assess complex learning are hard to devise (Linn &

Miller, 2005). Well-constructed items phrase the question so the task of the student is clear. Often this involves

providing hints or planning notes. In the first example below the actual question is clear not only because of the

wording but because of the format (i.e. it is placed in a box). In the second and third examples planning notes are

provided:

Example 1: Third grade mathematics:

The owner of a bookstore gave 14 books to the school. The principal will give an equal number of books to each

of three classrooms and the remaining books to the school library. How many books could the principal give to each

student and the school?

Show all your work on the space below and on the next page. Explain in words how you found the answer. Tell

why you took the steps you did to solve the problem.

From Illinois Standards Achievement Test, 2006; (http://www.isbe.state.il.us/assessment/isat.htm)

Example 2: Fifth grade science: The grass is always greener

Jose and Maria noticed three different types of soil, black soil, sand, and clay, were found in their neighborhood.

They decided to investigate the question, “How does the type of soil (black soil, sand, and clay) under grass sod

affect the height of grass?”

Plan an investigation that could answer their new question.

In your plan, be sure to include:

• Prediction of the outcome of the investigation

• Materials needed to do the investigation

• Procedure that includes:

• logical steps to do the investigation

• one variable kept the same (controlled)

• one variable changed (manipulated)

• any variables being measure and recorded

Educational Psychology 256 A Global Text

11. Teacher-made assessment strategies

• how often measurements are taken and recorded

(From Washington State 2004 assessment of student learning )

http://www.k12.wa.us/assessment/WASL/default.aspx)

Example 3: Grades 9-11 English:

Writing prompt

Some people think that schools should teach students how to cook. Other people think that cooking is something

that ought to be taught in the home. What do you think? Explain why you think as you do.

Planning notes

Choose One:

□ I think schools should teach students how to cook

□ I think cooking should l be taught in the home

I think cooking should be taught in ……………………………..because………

(school) or (the home)

(From Illinois Measure of Annual Growth in English http://www.isbe.state.il.us/assessment/image.htm )

A major disadvantage of extended response items is the difficulty in reliable scoring. Not only do various

teachers score the same response differently but also the same teacher may score the identical response differently

on various occasions (Linn & Miller 2005). A variety of steps can be taken to improve the reliability and validity of

scoring. First, teachers should begin by writing an outline of a model answer. This helps make it clear what students

are expected to include. Second, a sample of the answers should be read. This assists in determining what the

students can do and if there are any common misconceptions arising from the question. Third, teachers have to

decide what to do about irrelevant information that is included (e.g. is it ignored or are students penalized) and how

to evaluate mechanical errors such as grammar and spelling. Then, a point scoring or a scoring rubric should be

used.

In point scoring components of the answer are assigned points. For example, if students were asked:

What are the nature, symptoms, and risk factors of hyperthermia?

Point Scoring Guide:

Definition (natures) 2 pts

Symptoms (1 pt for each) 5 pts

Risk Factors (1 point for each) 5 pts

Writing 3 pts

This provides some guidance for evaluation and helps consistency but point scoring systems often lead the

teacher to focus on facts (e.g. naming risk factors) rather than higher level thinking that may undermine the validity

257

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

of the assessment if the teachers’ purposes include higher level thinking. A better approach is to use a scoring rubric

that describes the quality of the answer or performance at each level.

Scoring rubrics

Scoring rubrics can be holistic or analytical. In holistic scoring rubrics, general descriptions of performance are

made and a single overall score is obtained. An example from grade 2 language arts in Los Angeles Unified School

District classifies responses into four levels: not proficient, partially proficient, proficient and advanced is on Table

39.

Table 39: Example of holistic scoring rubric: English language arts grade 2

Assignment. Write about an interesting, fun, or exciting story you have read in class this year. Some of the

things you could write about are:

 What happened in the story (the plot or events)

 Where the events took place (the setting)

 People, animals, or things in the story ( the characters)

In your writing make sure you use facts and details from the story to describe everything clearly.

After you write about the story, explain what makes the story interesting, fun or exciting.

Scoring rubric

Advanced

Score 4

The response demonstrates well-developed reading comprehension skills.

Major story elements (plot, setting, or characters) are clearly and accurately described.

Statements about the plot, setting, or characters are arranged in a manner that makes

sense.

Ideas or judgments (why the story is interesting, fun, or exciting) are clearly supported

or explained with facts and details from the story.

Proficient

Score 3

The response demonstrates solid reading comprehension skills.

Most statements about the plot, setting, or characters are clearly described.

Most statements about the plot, setting, or characters are arranged in a manner that

makes sense.

Ideas or judgments are supported with facts and details from the story.

Partially

Proficient

The response demonstrates some reading comprehension skills

Educational Psychology 258 A Global Text

11. Teacher-made assessment strategies

Score 1

There is an attempt to describe the plot, setting, or characters

Some statements about the plot, setting, or characters are arranged in a manner that

makes sense.

Ideas or judgments may be supported with some facts and details from the story.

Not Proficient

Score 1

The response demonstrates little or no skill in reading comprehension.

The plot, setting, or characters are not described, or the description is unclear.

Statements about the plot, setting, or characters are not arranged in a manner that

makes sense.

Ideas or judgments are not stated, and facts and details from the text are not used.

Source: Adapted from English Language Arts Grade 2 Los Angeles Unified School District, 2001

(http://www.cse.ucla.edu/resources/justforteachers_set.htm)

Analytical rubrics provide descriptions of levels of student performance on a variety of characteristics. For

example, six characteristics used for assessing writing developed by the Northwest Regional Education Laboratory

(NWREL) are:

• ideas and content

• organization

• voice

• word choice

• sentence fluency

• conventions

Descriptions of high, medium, and low responses for each characteristic are available from:

http://www.nwrel.org/assessment/toolkit98/traits/index.html).

Holistic rubrics have the advantages that they can be developed more quickly than analytical rubrics. They are

also faster to use as there is only one dimension to examine. However, they do not provide students feedback about

which aspects of the response are strong and which aspects need improvement (Linn & Miller, 2005). This means

they are less useful for assessment for learning. An important use of rubrics is to use them as teaching tools and

provide them to students before the assessment so they know what knowledge and skills are expected.

Teachers can use scoring rubrics as part of instruction by giving students the rubric during instruction,

providing several responses, and analyzing these responses in terms of the rubric. For example, use of accurate

terminology is one dimension of the science rubric in Table 40. An elementary science teacher could discuss why it

259

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

is important for scientists to use accurate terminology, give examples of inaccurate and accurate terminology,

provide that component of the scoring rubric to students, distribute some examples of student responses (maybe

from former students), and then discuss how these responses would be classified according to the rubric. This

strategy of assessment for learning should be more effective if the teacher (a) emphasizes to students why using

accurate terminology is important when learning science rather than how to get a good grade on the test (we

provide more details about this in the section on motivation later in this chapter); (b) provides an exemplary

response so students can see a model; and (c) emphasizes that the goal is student improvement on this skill not

ranking students.

Table 40: Example of a scoring rubric, Science

*On the High School Assessment, the application of a concept to a practical problem or real-world situation

will be scored when it is required in the response and requested in the item stem.

Level of

understanding

Use of accurate

scientific

terminology

Use of

supporting

details

Synthesis of

information

Application

of information*

4 There is evidence in

the response that the

student has a full and

complete

understanding.

The use of accurate

scientific terminology

enhances the response.

Pertinent

and complete

supporting

details

demonstrate

an integration

of ideas.

The response

reflects a

complete

synthesis of

information.

An effective

application of the

concept to a

practical problem

or real-world

situation reveals

an insight into

scientific

principles.

3 There is evidence in

the response that the

student has a good

understanding.

The use of accurate

scientific terminology

strengthens the

response.

The

supporting

details are

generally

complete.

The response

reflects some

synthesis of

information.

The concept

has been applied

to a practical

problem or real-

world situation.

2 There is evidence in

the response that the

student has a basic

understanding.

The use of accurate

scientific terminology

may be present in the

response.

The

supporting

details are

adequate.

The response

provides little or

no synthesis of

information.

The application

of the concept to a

practical problem

or real-world

situation is

inadequate.

1 There is evidence in

the response that the

student has some

The use of accurate

scientific terminology is

not present in the

The

supporting

details are only

The response

addresses the

question.

The

application, if

attempted, is

Educational Psychology 260 A Global Text

11. Teacher-made assessment strategies

understanding. response. minimally

effective.

irrelevant.

0 The student has NO UNDERSTANDING of the question or problem. The response is completely

incorrect or irrelevant.

Performance assessments

Typically in performance assessments students complete a specific task while teachers observe the process or

procedure (e.g. data collection in an experiment) as well as the product (e.g. completed report) (Popham, 2005;

Stiggens, 2005). The tasks that students complete in performance assessments are not simple—in contrast to

selected response items—and include the following:

• playing a musical instrument

• athletic skills

• artistic creation

• conversing in a foreign language

• engaging in a debate about political issues

• conducting an experiment in science

• repairing a machine

• writing a term paper

• using interaction skills to play together

These examples all involve complex skills but illustrate that the term performance assessment is used in a

variety of ways. For example, the teacher may not observe all of the process (e.g. she sees a draft paper but the final

product is written during out-of-school hours) and essay tests are typically classified as performance assessments

(Airasian, 2000). In addition, in some performance assessments there may be no clear product (e.g. the

performance may be group interaction skills).

Two related terms, alternative assessment and authentic assessment are sometimes used instead of

performance assessment but they have different meanings (Linn & Miller, 2005). Alternative assessment refers to

tasks that are not pencil-and-paper and while many performance assessments are not pencil-and paper tasks some

are (e.g. writing a term paper, essay tests). Authentic assessment is used to describe tasks that students do that are

similar to those in the “real world”. Classroom tasks vary in level of authenticity (Popham, 2005). For example, a

Japanese language class taught in a high school in Chicago conversing in Japanese in Tokyo is highly authentic—

but only possible in a study abroad program or trip to Japan. Conversing in Japanese with native Japanese speakers

in Chicago is also highly authentic, and conversing with the teacher in Japanese during class is moderately

authentic. Much less authentic is a matching test on English and Japanese words. In a language arts class, writing a

letter (to an editor) or a memo to the principal is highly authentic as letters and memos are common work products.

261

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

However, writing a five-paragraph paper is not as authentic as such papers are not used in the world of work.

However, a five paragraph paper is a complex task and would typically be classified as a performance assessment.

Advantages and disadvantages

There are several advantages of performance assessments (Linn & Miller 2005). First, the focus is on complex

learning outcomes that often cannot be measured by other methods. Second, performance assessments typically

assess process or procedure as well as the product. For example, the teacher can observe if the students are

repairing the machine using the appropriate tools and procedures as well as whether the machine functions

properly after the repairs. Third, well designed performance assessments communicate the instructional goals and

meaningful learning clearly to students. For example, if the topic in a fifth grade art class is one-point perspective

the performance assessment could be drawing a city scene that illustrates one point perspective.

(http://www.sanford-artedventures.com). This assessment is meaningful and clearly communicates the learning

goal. This performance assessment is a good instructional activity and has good content validity—common with

well designed performance assessments (Linn & Miller 2005).

One major disadvantage with performance assessments is that they are typically very time consuming for

students and teachers. This means that fewer assessments can be gathered so if they are not carefully devised fewer

learning goals will be assessed—which can reduce content validity. State curriculum guidelines can be helpful in

determining what should be included in a performance assessment. For example, Eric, a dance teacher in a high

school in Tennessee learns that the state standards indicate that dance students at the highest level should be able

to do demonstrate consistency and clarity in performing technical skills by:

• performing complex movement combinations to music in a variety of meters and styles

• performing combinations and variations in a broad dynamic range

• demonstrating improvement in performing movement combinations through self-evaluation

• critiquing a live or taped dance production based on given criteria

(http://www.tennessee.gov/education/ci/standards/music/dance912.shtml)

Eric devises the following performance task for his eleventh grade modern dance class.

In groups of 4-6 students will perform a dance at least 5 minutes in length. The dance selected should

be multifaceted so that all the dancers can demonstrate technical skills, complex movements, and a

dynamic range (Items 1-2). Students will videotape their rehearsals and document how they

improved through self evaluation (Item 3). Each group will view and critique the final performance

of one other group in class (Item 4). Eric would need to scaffold most steps in this performance

assessment. The groups probably would need guidance in selecting a dance that allowed all the

dancers to demonstrate the appropriate skills; critiquing their own performances constructively;

working effectively as a team, and applying criteria to evaluate a dance.

Another disadvantage of performance assessments is they are hard to assess reliably which can lead to

inaccuracy and unfair evaluation. As with any constructed response assessment, scoring rubrics are very important.

Educational Psychology 262 A Global Text

11. Teacher-made assessment strategies

An example of holistic and analytic scoring rubrics designed to assess a completed product are in Table 39 and

Table 40. A rubric designed to assess the process of group interactions is in Table 41.

Table 41: Example of group interaction rubric

Score Time

management

Participation and

performance in roles

Shared involvement

0 Group did not stay

on task and so task was

not completed.

Group did not assign or

share roles.

Single individual did the task.

1 Group was off-task

the majority of the time

but task was completed.

Groups assigned roles

but members did not use

these roles.

Group totally disregarded comments and

ideas from some members.

2 Group stayed on task

most of the time.

Groups accepted and

used some but not all roles.

Group accepted some ideas but did not

give others adequate consideration.

3 Group stayed on task

throughout the activity

and managed time well.

Group accepted and used

roles and actively

participated.

Groups gave equal consideration to all

ideas.

4 Group defined their

own approach in a way

that more effectively

managed the activity.

Group defined and used

roles not mentioned to

them. Role changes took

place that maximized

individuals’ expertise.

Groups made specific efforts to involve

all group members including the reticent

members.

Source: Adapted from Group Interaction ( GI) SETUP ( 2003). Issues, Evidence and You. Ronkonkomo, NY

Lab-Aids. (http://cse.edc.org/products/assessment/middleschool/scorerub.asp))

This rubric was devised for middle grade science but could be used in other subject areas when assessing group

process. In some performance assessments several scoring rubrics should be used. In the dance performance

example above Eric should have scoring rubrics for the performance skills, the improvement based on self

evaluation, the team work, and the critique of the other group. Obviously, devising a good performance assessment

is complex and Linn and Miller (2005) recommend that teachers should:

• Create performance assessments that require students to use complex cognitive skills. Sometimes teachers

devise assessments that are interesting and that the students enjoy but do not require students to use

higher level cognitive skills that lead to significant learning. Focusing on high level skills and learning

outcomes is particularly important because performance assessments are typically so time consuming.

263

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

• Ensure that the task is clear to the students. Performance assessments typically require multiple steps so

students need to have the necessary prerequisite skills and knowledge as well as clear directions. Careful

scaffolding is important for successful performance assessments.

• Specify expectations of the performance clearly by providing students scoring rubrics during the

instruction. This not only helps students understand what it expected but it also guarantees that teachers

are clear about what they expect. Thinking this through while planning the performance assessment can be

difficult for teachers but is crucial as it typically leads to revisions of the actual assessment and directions

provided to students.

• Reduce the importance of unessential skills in completing the task. What skills are essential depends on the

purpose of the task. For example, for a science report, is the use of publishing software essential? If the

purpose of the assessment is for students to demonstrate the process of the scientific method including

writing a report, then the format of the report may not be significant. However, if the purpose includes

integrating two subject areas, science and technology, then the use of publishing software is important.

Because performance assessments take time it is tempting to include multiple skills without carefully

considering if all the skills are essential to the learning goals.

Portfolios

“A portfolio is a meaningful collection of student work that tells the story of student achievement or growth”

(Arter, Spandel, & Culham, 1995, p. 2). Portfolios are a purposeful collection of student work not just folders of all

the work a student does. Portfolios are used for a variety of purposes and developing a portfolio system can be

confusing and stressful unless the teachers are clear on their purpose. The varied purposes can be illustrated as four

dimensions (Linn & Miller 2005):

Assessment for Learning Assessment of learning

Current Accomplishments Progress

Best Work Showcase Documentation

Finished Working

When the primary purpose is assessment for learning, the emphasis is on student self-reflection and

responsibility for learning. Students not only select samples of their work they wish to include, but also reflect and

interpret their own work. Portfolios containing this information can be used to aid communication as students can

present and explain their work to their teachers and parents (Stiggins, 2005). Portfolios focusing on assessment of

learning contain students’ work samples that certify accomplishments for a classroom grade, graduation, state

requirements etc. Typically, students have less choice in the work contained in such portfolios as some consistency

Educational Psychology 264 A Global Text

11. Teacher-made assessment strategies

is needed for this type of assessment. For example, the writing portfolios that fourth and seventh graders are

required to submit in Kentucky must contain a self-reflective statement and an example of three pieces of writing

(reflective, personal experience or literary, and transactive). Students do choose which of their pieces of writing in

each type to include in the portfolio.

(http://www.kde.state.ky.us/KDE/Instructional+Resources/Curriculum+Documents+and+Resources/Student

+Performance+Standards/).

Portfolios can be designed to focus on student progress or current accomplishments. For example, audio tapes

of English language learners speaking could be collected over one year to demonstrate growth in learning. Student

progress portfolios may also contain multiple versions of a single piece of work. For example, a writing project may

contain notes on the original idea, outline, first draft, comments on the first draft by peers or teacher, second draft,

and the final finished product (Linn & Miller 2005). If the focus is on current accomplishments, only recent

completed work samples are included.

Portfolios can focus on documenting student activities or highlighting important accomplishments.

Documentation portfolios are inclusive containing all the work samples rather than focusing on one special

strength, best work, or progress. In contrast, showcase portfolios focus on best work. The best work is typically

identified by students. One aim of such portfolios is that students learn how to identify products that demonstrate

what they know and can do. Students are not expected to identify their best work in isolation but also use the

feedback from their teachers and peers.

A final distinction can be made between a finished portfolio—maybe used to for a job application—versus a

working portfolio that typically includes day-to-day work samples. Working portfolios evolve over time and are not

intended to be used for assessment of learning. The focus in a working portfolio is on developing ideas and skills so

students should be allowed to make mistakes, freely comment on their own work, and respond to teacher feedback

(Linn & Miller, 2005). Finished portfolios are designed for use with a particular audience and the products selected

may be drawn from a working portfolio. For example, in a teacher education program, the working portfolio may

contain work samples from all the courses taken. A student may develop one finished portfolio to demonstrate she

has mastered the required competencies in the teacher education program and a second finished portfolio for her

job application.

Advantages and disadvantages

Portfolios used well in classrooms have several advantages. They provide a way of documenting and evaluating

growth in a much more nuanced way than selected response tests can. Also, portfolios can be integrated easily into

instruction, i.e. used for assessment for learning. Portfolios also encourage student self-evaluation and reflection, as

well as ownership for learning (Popham, 2005). Using classroom assessment to promote student motivation is an

important component of assessment for learning which is considered in the next section.

However, there are some major disadvantages of portfolio use. First, good portfolio assessment takes an

enormous amount of teacher time and organization. The time is needed to help students understand the purpose

and structure of the portfolio, decide which work samples to collect, and to self reflect. Some of this time needs to

be conducted in one-to-one conferences. Reviewing and evaluating the portfolios out of class time is also

enormously time consuming. Teachers have to weigh if the time spent is worth the benefits of the portfolio use.

265

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Second, evaluating portfolios reliability and eliminating bias can be even more difficult than in a constructed

response assessment because the products are more varied. The experience of the state-wide use of portfolios for

assessment in writing and mathematics for fourth and eighth graders in Vermont is sobering. Teachers used the

same analytic scoring rubric when evaluating the portfolio. In the first two years of implementation samples from

schools were collected and scored by an external panel of teachers. In the first year the agreement among raters (i.e.

inter-rater reliability) was poor for mathematics and reading; in the second year the agreement among raters

improved for mathematics but not for reading. However, even with the improvement in mathematics the reliability

was too low to use the portfolios for individual student accountability (Koretz, Stecher, Klein & McCaffrey, 1994).

When reliability is low, validity is also compromised because unstable results cannot be interpreted meaningfully.

If teachers do use portfolios in their classroom, the series of steps needed for implementation are outlined in

Table 36. If the school or district has an existing portfolio system these steps may have to be modified.

Table 42: Steps in implementing a classroom portfolio program

1. Make sure students own their

portfolios.

Talk to your students about your ideas of the portfolio, the different

purposes, and the variety of work samples. If possible, have them help make

decisions about the kind of portfolio you implement.

2. Decide on the purpose. Will the focus be on growth or current accomplishments? Best work

showcase or documentation? Good portfolios can have multiple purposes but

the teacher and students need to be clear about the purpose.

3. Decide what work samples to

collect,

For example, in writing, is every writing assignment included? Are early

drafts as well as final products included?

4. Collect and store work

samples,

Decide where the work sample will be stored. For example, will each

student have a file folder in a file cabinet, or a small plastic tub on a shelf in

the classroom?

5. Select criteria to evaluate

samples,

If possible, work with students to develop scoring rubrics. This may take

considerable time as different rubrics may be needed for the variety of work

samples. If you are using existing scoring rubrics, discuss with students

possible modifications after the rubrics have been used at least once.

6. Teach and require students

conduct self evaluations of their

own work,

Help students learn to evaluate their own work using agreed upon

criteria. For younger students, the self evaluations may be simple (strengths,

weaknesses, and ways to improve); for older students a more analytic

approach is desirable including using the same scoring rubrics that the

teachers will use.

7. Schedule and conduct

portfolio conferences ,

Teacher-student conferences are time consuming but conferences are

essential for the portfolio process to significantly enhance learning. These

conferences should aid students’ self evaluation and should take place

frequently.

Educational Psychology 266 A Global Text

11. Teacher-made assessment strategies

8. Involve parents. Parents need to understand the portfolio process. Encourage parents to

review the work samples. You may wish to schedule parent, teacher-students

conferences in which students talk about their work samples.

Source: Adapted from Popham (2005)

Assessment that enhances motivation and student confidence

Studies on testing and learning conducted more than 20 years ago demonstrated that tests promote learning

and that more frequent tests are more effective than less frequent tests (Dempster & Perkins, 1993). Frequent

smaller tests encourage continuous effort rather than last minute cramming and may also reduce test anxiety

because the consequences of errors are reduced. College students report preferring more frequent testing than

infrequent testing (Bangert-Downs, Kulik, Kulik, 1991). More recent research indicates that teachers’ assessment

purpose and beliefs, the type of assessment selected, and the feedback given contributes to the assessment climate

in the classroom which influences students’ confidence and motivation. The use of self-assessment is also

important in establishing a positive assessment climate.

Teachers’ purposes and beliefs

Student motivation can be enhanced when the purpose of assessment is promoting student learning and this is

clearly communicated to students by what teachers say and do (Harlen, 2006). This approach to assessment is

associated with what the psychologist, Carol Dweck, (2000) calls an incremental view of ability or intelligence. An

incremental view assumes that ability increases whenever an individual learns more. This means that effort is

valued because effort leads to knowing more and therefore having more ability. Individuals with an incremental

view also ask for help when needed and respond well to constructive feedback as the primary goal is increased

learning and mastery. In contrast, a fixed view of ability assumes that some people have more ability than others

and nothing much can be done to change that. Individuals with a fixed view of ability often view effort in opposition

to ability (“Smart people don’t have to study”) and so do not try as hard, and are less likely to ask for help as that

indicates that they are not smart. While there are individual differences in students’ beliefs about their views of

intelligence, teachers’ beliefs and classroom practices influence students’ perceptions and behaviors.

Teachers with an incremental view of intelligence communicate to students that the goal of learning is mastering

the material and figuring things out. Assessment is used by these teachers to understand what students know so

they can decide whether to move to the next topic, re-teach the entire class, or provide remediation for a few

students. Assessment also helps students’ understand their own learning and demonstrate their competence.

Teachers with these views say things like, “We are going to practice over and over again. That’s how you get good.

And you’re going to make mistakes. That’s how you learn.” (Patrick, Anderman, Ryan, Edelin, Midgley, 2001, p.

45).

In contrast, teachers with a fixed view of ability are more likely to believe that the goal of learning is doing well

on tests especially outperforming others. These teachers are more likely to say things that imply fixed abilities e.g.

“This test will determine what your math abilities are”, or stress the importance of interpersonal competition, “We

will have speech competition and the top person will compete against all the other district schools and last year the

winner got a big award and their photo in the paper.” When teachers stress interpersonal competition some

267

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

students may be motivated but there can only a few winners so there are many more students who know they have

no chance of winning. Another problem with interpersonal competition in assessment is that the focus can become

winning rather than understanding the material.

Teachers who communicate to their students that ability is incremental and that the goal of assessment is

promoting learning rather that ranking students, or awarding prizes to those who did very well, or catching those

who did not pay attention, are likely to enhance students’ motivation.

Choosing assessments

The choice of assessment task also influences students’ motivation and confidence. First, assessments that have

clear criteria that students understand and can meet rather than assessments that pit students against each other in

interpersonal competition enhances motivation (Black, Harrison, Lee, Marshall, Wiliam, 2004). This is consistent

with the point we made in the previous section about the importance of focusing on enhancing learning for all

students rather than ranking students. Second, meaningful assessment tasks enhance student motivation. Students

often want to know why they have to do something and teachers need to provide meaningful answers. For example,

a teacher might say, “You need to be able to calculate the area of a rectangle because if you want new carpet you

need to know how much carpet is needed and how much it would cost.” Well designed performance tasks are often

more meaningful to students than selected response tests so students will work harder to prepare for them.

Third, providing choices of assessment tasks can enhance student sense of autonomy and motivation according

to self determination theory (see Chapter 6). Kym, the sixth grade teacher whose story began this chapter, reports

that giving students choices was very helpful. Another middle school social studies teacher Aaron, gives his students

a choice of performance tasks at the end of the unit on the US Bill of Rights. Students have to demonstrate specified

key ideas but can do that by making up a board game, presenting a brief play, composing a rap song etc. Aaron

reports that students work much harder on this performance assessment which allows them to use their strengths

than previously when he did not provide any choices and gave a more traditional assignment. Measurement experts

caution that a danger of giving choices is that the assessment tasks are no longer equivalent and so the reliability of

scoring is reduced so it is particularly important to use well designed scoring rubrics. Fourth, assessment tasks

should be challenging but achievable with reasonable effort (Elliott, McGregor & Thrash, 2004). This is often hard

for beginning teachers to do, who may give assessment tasks that are too easy or too hard, because they have to

learn to match their assessment to the skills of their students.

Providing feedback

When the goal is assessment for learning, providing constructive feedback that helps students know what they

do and do not understand as well as encouraging them to learn from their errors is fundamental. Effective feedback

should be given as soon as possible as the longer the delay between students’ work and feedback the longer students

will continue to have some misconceptions. Also, delays reduce the relationship between students’ performance and

the feedback as students can forget what they were thinking during the assessment. Effective feedback should also

inform students clearly what they did well and what needs modification. General comments just as “good work, A”,

or “needs improvement” do not help students understand how to improve their learning. Giving feedback to

students using well designed scoring rubrics helps clearly communicate strengths and weaknesses. Obviously

grades are often needed but teachers can minimize the focus by placing the grade after the comments or on the last

Educational Psychology 268 A Global Text

11. Teacher-made assessment strategies

page of a paper. It can also be helpful to allow students to keep their grades private making sure when returning

assignments that the grade is not prominent (e.g. not using red ink on the top page) and never asking students to

read their scores aloud in class. Some students choose to share their grades—but that should be their decision not

their teachers.

When grading, teachers often become angry at the mistakes that student make. It is easy for teachers to think

something like: “With all the effort I put into teaching, this student could not even be bothered to follow the

directions or spell check!” Many experienced teachers believe that communicating their anger is not helpful, so

rather than saying: “How dare you turn in such shoddy work”, they rephrase it as, “I am disappointed that your

work on this assignment does not meet the standards set” (Sutton, 2003). Research evidence also suggests that

comments such as “You are so smart” for a high quality performance can be counterproductive. This is surprising to

many teachers but if students are told they are smart when they produce a good product, then if they do poorly on

the next assignment the conclusion must be they are “not smart” (Dweck, 2000). More effective feedback focuses

on positive aspects of the task (not the person), as well as strategies, and effort. The focus of the feedback should

relate to the criteria set by the teacher and how improvements can be made.

When the teacher and student are from different racial/ethnic backgrounds providing feedback that enhances

motivation and confidence but also includes criticism can be particularly challenging because the students of color

have historical reasons to distrust negative comments from a white teacher. Research by Cohen Steele, Ross (1999)

indicates that “wise” feedback from teachers needs three components: positive comments, criticisms, and an

assurance that the teacher believes the student can reach higher standards. We describe this research is more detail

in “Deciding for yourself about the research” found in Appendix #2.

Self and peer assessment

In order to reach a learning goal, students need to understand the meaning of the goal, the steps necessary to

achieve a goal, and if they are making satisfactory progress towards that goal (Sadler, 1989). This involves self

assessment and recent research has demonstrated that well designed self assessment can enhance student learning

and motivation (Black & Wiliam, 2006). For self assessment to be effective, students need explicit criteria such as

those in an analytical scoring rubric. These criteria are either provided by the teacher or developed by the teacher in

collaboration with students. Because students seem to find it easier to understand criteria for assessment tasks if

they can examine other students’ work along side their own, self assessment often involves peer assessment. An

example of a strategy used by teachers involves asking students to use “traffic lights” to indicate of their confidence

in their assignment or homework. Red indicates that they were unsure of their success, orange that they were

partially unsure, and green that they were confident of their success. The students who labeled their own work as

orange and green worked in mixed groups to evaluate their own work while the teacher worked with the students

who had chosen red (Black & Wiliam, 2006).

If self and peer assessment is used, it is particularly important that the teachers establish a classroom culture for

assessment that is based on incremental views of ability and learning goals. If the classroom atmosphere focuses on

interpersonal competition, students have incentives in self and peer assessment to inflate their own evaluations

(and perhaps those of their friends) because there are limited rewards for good work.

269

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Adjusting instruction based on assessment

Using assessment information to adjust instruction is fundamental to the concept of assessment for learning.

Teachers make these adjustments “in the moment” during classroom instruction as well as during reflection and

planning periods. Teachers use the information they gain from questioning and observation to adjust their teaching

during classroom instruction. If students cannot answer a question, the teacher may need to rephrase the question,

probe understanding of prior knowledge, or change the way the current idea is being considered. It is important for

teachers to learn to identify when only one or two students need individual help because they are struggling with

the concept, and when a large proportion of the class is struggling so whole group intervention is needed.

After the class is over, effective teachers spend time analyzing how well the lessons went, what students did and

did not seem to understand, and what needs to be done the next day. Evaluation of student work also provides

important information for teachers. If many students are confused about a similar concept the teacher needs to re-

teach it and consider new ways of helping students understand the topic. If the majority of students complete the

tasks very quickly and well, the teacher might decide that the assessment was not challenging enough. Sometimes

teachers become dissatisfied with the kinds of assessments they have assigned when they are grading—perhaps

because they realize there was too much emphasis on lower level learning, that the directions were not clear

enough, or the scoring rubric needed modification. Teachers who believe that assessment data provides

information about their own teaching and that they can find ways to influence student learning have high teacher

efficacy or beliefs that they can make a difference in students’ lives. In contrast, teachers who think that student

performance is mostly due to fixed student characteristics or the homes they come from (e.g. “no wonder she did so

poorly considering what her home life is like”) have low teacher efficacy (Tschannen-Moran, Woolfolk Hoy, & Hoy,

1998).

Communication with parents and guardians

Clear communication with parents about classroom assessment is important—but often difficult for beginning

teachers. The same skills that are needed to communicate effectively with students are also needed when

communicating with parents and guardians. Teachers need to be able to explain to parents the purpose of the

assessment, why they selected this assessment technique, and what the criteria for success are. Some teachers send

home newsletters monthly or at the beginning of a major assessment task explaining the purpose and nature of the

task, any additional support that is needed (e.g. materials, library visits), and due dates. Some parents will not be

familiar with performance assessments or the use of self and peer assessment so teachers need to take time to

explain these approaches carefully.

Many school districts now communicate though websites that have mixtures of public information available to

all parents in the class (e.g. curriculum and assessment details) as well information restricted to the parents or

guardians of specific students (e.g. the attendance and grades). Teachers report this is helpful as parents have

access to their child’s performance immediately and when necessary, can talk to their child and teacher quickly.

The recommendations we provided above on the type of feedback that should be given to students also apply

when talking to parents. That is, the focus should be on students’ performance on the task, what was done well and

what needs work, rather than general comments about how “smart” or “weak” the child is. If possible, comments

should focus on strategies that the child uses well or needs to improve (e.g. reading test questions carefully,

Educational Psychology 270 A Global Text

11. Teacher-made assessment strategies

organization in a large project). When the teacher is white and the student or parents are minority, trust can be an

issue so using “wise” feedback when talking to parents may help.

Action research: studying yourself and your students

Assessment for learning emphasizes devising and conducting assessment data in order to improve teaching and

learning and so is related to action research (also called teacher research). In Chapter 1, we described action

research as studies conducted by teachers of their own students or their own work. Action research can lead to

decisions that improve a teacher’s own teaching or the teaching of colleagues. Kym, the teacher we described at the

beginning of this chapter, conducted action research in her own classroom as she identified a problem of poor

student motivation and achievement, investigated solutions during the course on motivation, tried new approaches,

and observed the resulting actions.

Cycles of planning, acting and reflecting

Action research is usually described as a cyclical process with the following stages (Mertler, 2006).

• Planning Stage. Planning has three components. First, planning involves identifying and defining a

problem. Problems sometimes start with some ill defined unease or feeling that something is wrong and it

can take time to identify the problem clearly so that it becomes a researchable question. The next step, is

reviewing the related literature and this may occur within a class or workshop that the teachers are

attending. Teachers may also explore the literature on their own or in teacher study groups. The third step

is developing a research plan. The research plan includes what kind of data will be collected (e.g. student

test scores, observation of one or more students, as well as how and when it will be collected (e.g. from files,

in collaboration with colleagues, in spring or fall semester).

• Acting sage. During this stage the teacher is collecting and analyzing data. The data collected and the

analyses do not need to be complex because action research, to be effective, has to be manageable.

• Developing an action plan. In this stage the teacher develops a plan to make changes and implements these

changes. This is the action component of action research and it is important that teachers document their

actions carefully so that they can communicate them to others.

• Communicating and reflection. An important component of all research is communicating information.

Results can be shared with colleagues in the school or district, in an action research class at the local

college, at conferences, or in journals for teachers. Action research can also involve students as active

participants and if this is the case, communication may include students and parents. Communicating with

others helps refine ideas and so typically aids in reflection. During reflection teachers/researchers ask such

questions as: “What did I learn?” “What should I have done differently?” “What should I do next?”

Questions such as these often lead to a new cycle of action research beginning with planning and then

moving to the other steps.

Ethical issues—privacy, voluntary consent

Teachers are accustomed to collecting students’ test scores, data about performances, and descriptions of

behaviors as an essential component of teaching. However, if teachers are conducting action research and they plan

to collect data that will be shared outside the school community then permission from parents (or guardians) and

271

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

students must be obtained in order to protect the privacy of students and their families. Typically permission is

obtained by an informed consent form that summarizes the research, describes the data that will be collected,

indicates that participation is voluntary, and provides a guarantee of confidentiality or anonymity (Hubbard &

Power, 2005). Many large school districts have procedures for establishing informed consent as well as person in

the central office who is responsible for the district guidelines and specific application process. If the action

research is supported in some way by a college of university (e.g. through a class) then informed consent

procedures of that institution must be followed.

One common area of confusion for teachers is the voluntary nature of student participation in research. If the

data being collected are for a research study, students can choose not to participate. This is contrary to much

regular classroom instruction where teachers tell students they have to do the work or complete the tasks.

Grading and reporting

Assigning students grades is an important component of teaching and many school districts issue progress

reports, interim reports, or mid term grades as well as final semester grades. Traditionally these reports were

printed on paper and sent home with students or mailed to students’ homes. Increasingly, school districts are using

web-based grade management systems that allow parents to access their child’s grades on each individual

assessment as well as the progress reports and final grades.

Grading can be frustrating for teachers as there are many factors to consider. In addition, report cards typically

summarize in brief format a variety of assessments and so cannot provide much information about students’

strengths and weaknesses. This means that report cards focus more on assessment of learning than assessment for

learning. There are a number of decisions that have to be made when assigning students’ grades and schools often

have detailed policies that teachers have to follow. In the next section, we consider the major questions associated

with grading.

How are various assignments and assessments weighted?

Students typically complete a variety of assignments during a grading period such as homework, quizzes,

performance assessments, etc. Teachers have to decide—preferably before the grading period begins—how each

assignment will be weighted. For example, a sixth grade math teacher may decide to weight the grades in the

following manner:

Weekly quizzes 35 per cent

Homework 15 per cent

Performance Assessment 30 per cent

Class participation 20 per cent

Deciding how to weight assignments should be done carefully as it communicates to students and parents what

teachers believe is important, and also may be used to decide how much effort students will exert (e.g. “If

homework is only worth 5 per cent, it is not worth completing twice a week”).

Should social skills or effort be included? Elementary school teachers are more likely than middle or high school

teachers to include some social skills into report cards (Popham, 2005). These may be included as separate criteria

Educational Psychology 272 A Global Text

11. Teacher-made assessment strategies

in the report card or weighted into the grade for that subject. For example, the grade for mathematics may include

an assessment of group cooperation or self regulation during mathematics lessons. Some schools and teachers

endorse including social skills arguing that developing such skills is important for young students and that students

need to learn to work with others and manage their own behaviors in order to be successful. Others believe that

grades in subject areas should be based on the cognitive performances—and that if assessments of social skills are

made they should be clearly separated from the subject grade on the report card. Obviously, clear criteria such as

those contained in analytical scoring rubrics should be used if social skills are graded.

Teachers often find it difficult to decide whether effort and improvement should be included as a component of

grades. One approach is for teachers to ask students to submit drafts of an assignment and make improvements

based on the feedback they received. The grade for the assignment may include some combination of the score for

the drafts, the final version, and the amount of improvement the students made based on the feedback provided. A

more controversial approach is basing grades on effort when students try really hard day after day but still cannot

complete their assignments well. These students could have identified special needs or be recent immigrants that

have limited English skills. Some school districts have guidelines for handling such cases. One disadvantage of

using improvement as a component of grades is that the most competent students in class may do very well initially

and have little room for improvement—unless teachers are skilled at providing additional assignments that will

help challenge these students.

Teachers often use “hodgepodge grading”, i.e. a combination of achievement, effort, growth, attitude or class

conduct, homework, and class participation. A survey of over 8,500 middle and high school students in the US state

of Virginia supported the hodgepodge practices commonly used by their teachers (Cross & Frary, 1999).

How should grades be calculated?

Two options are commonly used: absolute grading and relative grading. In absolute grading grades are assigned

based on criteria the teacher has devised. If an English teacher has established a level of proficiency needed to

obtain an A and no student meets that level then no A's will be given. Alternatively if every student meets the

established level then all the students will get A's (Popham, 2005). Absolute grading systems may use letter grades

or pass/fail.

In relative grading the teacher ranks the performances of students from worst to best (or best to worst) and

those at the top get high grades, those in the middle moderate grades, and those at the bottom low grades. This is

often described as “grading on the curve” and can be useful to compensate for an examination or assignment that

students find much easier or harder than the teacher expected. However, relative grading can be unfair to students

because the comparisons are typically within one class, so an A in one class may not represent the level of

performance of an A in another class. Relative grading systems may discourage students from helping each other

improve as students are in competition for limited rewards. In fact, Bishop (1999) argues that grading on the curve

gives students a personal interest in persuading each other not to study as a serious student makes it more difficult

for others to get good grades.

What kinds of grade descriptions should be used?

Traditionally a letter grade system is used (e.g. A, B, C, D, F ) for each subject. The advantages of these grade

descriptions are they are convenient, simple, and can be averaged easily. However, they do not indicate what

273

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

objectives the student has or has not met nor students’ specific strengths and weaknesses (Linn & Miller 2005).

Elementary schools often use a pass-fail (or satisfactory-unsatisfactory) system and some high schools and colleges

do as well. Pass-fail systems in high school and college allow students to explore new areas and take risks on

subjects that they may have limited preparation for, or is not part of their major (Linn & Miller 2005). While a pass-

fail system is easy to use, it offers even less information about students’ level of learning.

A pass-fail system is also used in classes that are taught under a mastery-learning approach in which students

are expected to demonstrate mastery on all the objectives in order to receive course credit. Under these conditions,

it is clear that a pass means that the student has demonstrated mastery of all the objectives.

Some schools have implemented a checklist of the objectives in subject areas to replace the traditional letter

grade system, and students are rated on each objective using descriptors such as Proficient, Partially Proficient, and

Needs Improvement. For example, the checklist for students in a fourth grade class in California may include the

four types of writing that are required by the English language state content standards

(http://www.cde.ca.gov/be/st/ss/enggrade4.asp)

• writing narratives

• writing responses to literature

• writing information reports

• writing summaries

The advantages of this approach are that it communicates students’ strengths and weaknesses clearly, and it

reminds the students and parents the objectives of the school. However, if too many objectives are included then

the lists can become so long that they are difficult to understand.

Chapter summary

The purpose of classroom assessment can be assessment for learning or assessment of learning. Essential steps

of assessment for learning include communicating instructional goals clearly to students; selecting appropriate high

quality assessments that match the instructional goals and students’ backgrounds; using assessments that enhance

student motivation and confidence, adjusting instruction based on assessment, and communicating assessment

results with parents and guardians. Action research can help teachers understand and improve their teaching. A

number of questions are important to consider when devising grading systems.

Key terms

Absence of bias

Action research

Alternative assessment

Assessment

Assessment for learning

Assessment of learning

Authentic assessment

Constructed response items

Evaluation

Formative assessment

Formal assessment measurement

Informal assessment

Educational Psychology 274 A Global Text

11. Teacher-made assessment strategies

Performance assessment

Portfolios

Reliability

Selected response items

Summative assessment

Validity

References

Airasian, P. W. (2000). Classroom Assessment: A concise approach 2nd ed. Boston: McGraw Hill.

Airasian, P. W. (2004). Classroom Assessment: Concepts and Applications 3rd ed. Boston: McGraw Hill.

Bangert-Downs, R. L.,Kulik, J. A., & Kulik, C-L, C. (1991). Effects of frequent classroom testing. Journal of

Educational Research, 85 (2), 89-99.

Black, P., Harrison, C., Lee, C., Marshall, B. & Wiliam, D. (2004). Working inside the black box.: Assessment

for learning in the classroom. Phi Delta Kappan, 86 (1) 9-21.

Black, P., & Wiliam,D. (2006). Assessment for learning in the classroom. In J. Gardner (Ed.). Assessment

and learning (pp. 9-25). Thousand Oaks, CA:Sage.

Bishop, J. H. (1999). Nerd harassment, incentives, school priorities, and learning.In S. E. Mayer & P. E.

Peterson (Eds.) Earning and learning: How school matters (pp. 231-280). Washington, DC: Brookings

Institution Press.

Borko, H. & Livingston, C. (1989) Cognition and Improvisation: Differences in Mathematics Instruction by

Expert and Novice Teachers. American Educational Research Journal, 26, 473-98.

Cross, L. H., & Frary, R. B. (1999). Hodgepodge grading: Endorsed by students and teachers alike. Applied

Measurement in Education, 21(1) 53-72.

Dempster, F. N. & Perkins, P. G. (1993). Revitalizating classroom assessment: Using tests to promote

learning. Journal of Instructional Psychology, 20 (3) 197-203.

Dweck, C. S. (2000) Self-theories: Their role in motivation, personality, and development. Philadelphia, PA:

Psychology Press.

Elliott, A., McGregor, H., & Thrash, T. (2004). The need for competence. In E. Deci & R. Ryan (Eds.),

Handbook of self-determination research (pp. 361-388). Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press.

Harlen, W. The role of assessment in developing motivation for learning. In J. Gardner (Ed.). Assessment

and learning (pp. 61-80). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Hubbard, R. S., & Power, B. M. (2003). The art of classroom inquiry, A handbook for teachers-researchers

(2nd ed.). Portsmith, NH: Heinemann.

Koretz, D. Stecher, B. Klein, S. & McCaffrey, D. (1994). The evolution of a portfolio program: The impact

and quality of the Vermont program in its second year (1992-3). (CSE Technical report 385) Los

Angeles: University of California, Center for Research on Evaluation Standards and student Testing.

Accessed January 25, 2006 from http://www.csr.ucla.edu.

275

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Linn, R. L., & Miller, M. D. (2005). Measurement and Assessment in Teaching 9th ed. Upper Saddle River,

NJ: Pearson .

Mertler, C. A. (2006). Action research: Teachers as researchers in the classroom. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Popham, W. J. (2005). Classroom Assessment: What teachers need to know. Boston, MA: Pearson.

Rowe, M. B. (2003). Wait-time and rewards as instructional variables, their influence on language, logic and

fate control: Part one-wait time. Journal of Research in science Teaching, 40 Supplement, S19-32.

Stiggins, R. J. (2002). Assessment crisis: The absence of assessment FOR learning. Phi Delta Kappan, 83

(10), 758-765.

Sutton, R. E. (2004). Emotional regulation goals and strategies of teachers. Social Psychology of Education,

7(4), 379-398.Teel, K. M., Debrin-Parecki, A., & Covington, M. V. (1998). Teaching strategies that honor

and motivate inner-city African American students: A school/university collaboration. Teaching and

Teacher Education, 14(5), 479-495.

Tschannen-Moran, M., Woolfolk-Hoy, A., & Hoy, W. K. (1998). Teacher efficacy: Its meaning and measure.

Review of Educational Research, 68, 202-248.

Educational Psychology 276 A Global Text

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

12. Standardized and other formal assessments

Understanding standardized testing is very important for beginning teachers as K-12 teaching is increasingly

influenced by the administration and results of standardized tests. Teachers also need to be able to help parents and

students understand test results. Consider the following scenarios.

• Vanessa, a newly licensed physical education teacher, is applying for a job at a middle school.

During the job interview the principal asks how she would incorporate key sixth grade math

skills into her PE and health classes as the sixth grade students in the previous year did not

attain Adequate Yearly Progress in mathematics.

• Danielle, a first year science teacher in Ohio, is asked by Mr Volderwell, a recent immigrant

from Turkey and the parent of a tenth grade son Marius, to help him understand test results.

When Marius first arrived at school he took the Test of Cognitive Skills and scored on the eighty-

fifth percentile whereas on the state Science Graduation test he took later in the school year he

was classified as “proficient” .

• James, a third year elementary school teacher, attends a class in gifted education over summer

as standardized tests from the previous year indicated that while overall his class did well in

reading the top 20 per cent of his students did not learn as much as expected.

• Miguel, a 1st grade student, takes two tests in fall and the results indicate that his grade

equivalent scores are 3.3 for reading and 3.0 for math. William’s parents want him immediately

promoted into the second grade arguing that the test results indicate that he already can read

and do math at the 3rd grade level. Greg, a first grade teacher explains to William’s parents

that a grade equivalent score of 3.3 does not mean William can do third grade work.

Understanding standardized testing is difficult as there are numerous terms and concepts to master and recent

changes in accountability under the No Child Left Behind Act of 2001 (NCLB) have increased the complexity of the

concepts and issues. In this chapter we focus on the information that beginning teachers need to know and start

with some basic concepts.

Basic concepts

Standardized tests are created by a team—usually test experts from a commercial testing company who consult

classroom teachers and university faculty—and are administered in standardized ways. Students not only respond

to the same questions they also receive the same directions and have the same time limits. Explicit scoring criteria

are used. Standardized tests are designed to be taken by many students within a state, province, or nation, and

sometimes across nations. Teachers help administer some standardized tests and test manuals are provided that

Educational Psychology 277 A Global Text

12. Standardized and other formal assessments

contain explicit details about the administration and scoring. For example, teachers may have to remove all the

posters and charts from the classroom walls, read directions out loud to students using a script, and respond to

student questions in a specific manner.

Criterion referenced standardized tests measure student performance against a specific standard or criterion.

For example, newly hired firefighters in the Commonwealth of Massachusetts in the United States have to meet

physical fitness standards by successfully completing a standardized physical fitness test that includes stair

climbing, using a ladder, advancing a hose, and simulating a rescue through a doorway (Human Resources

Division, nod.). Criterion referenced tests currently used in US schools are often tied to state content standards and

provide information about what students can and cannot do. For example, one of the content standards for fourth

grade reading in Kentucky is “Students will identify and describe the characteristics of fiction, nonfiction, poetry or

plays” (Combined Curriculum Document Reading 4.1, 2006) and so a report on an individual student would

indicate if the child can accomplish this skill. The report may state that number or percentage of items that were

successfully completed (e.g. 15 out of 20, i.e. 75 per cent) or include descriptions such as basic, proficient, or

advanced which are based on decisions made about the percent of mastery necessary to be classified into these

categories.

Norm referenced standardized tests report students’ performance relative to others. For example, if a student

scores on the seventy-second percentile in reading it means she outperforms 72 percent of the students who were

included in the test’s norm group. A norm group is a representative sample of students who completed the

standardized test while it was being developed. For state tests the norm group is drawn from the state whereas for

national tests the sample is drawn from the nation. Information about the norm groups is provided in a technical

test manual that is not typically supplied to teachers but should be available from the person in charge of testing in

the school district.

Reports from criterion and norm referenced tests provide different information. Imagine a nationalized

mathematics test designed to basic test skills in second grade. If this test is norm referenced, and Alisha receives a

report indicating that she scored in the eighty-fifth percentile this indicates that she scored better than 85 per cent

of the students in the norm group who took the test previously. If this test is criterion-referenced Alisha’s report

may state that she mastered 65 per cent of the problems designed for her grade level. The relative percentage

reported from the norm-referenced test provides information about Alisha’s performance compared to other

students whereas the criterion referenced test attempts to describe what Alisha or any student can or cannot do

with respect to whatever the test is designed to measure. When planning instruction classroom teachers need to

know what students can and cannot do so criterion referenced tests are typically more useful (Popham, 2004). The

current standard-based accountability and NCLB rely predominantly on criterion based tests to assess attainment

of content-based standards. Consequently the use of standardized norm referenced tests in schools has diminished

and is largely limited to diagnosis and placement of children with specific cognitive disabilities or exceptional

abilities (Haertel & Herman, 2005).

Some recent standardized tests can incorporate both criterion-referenced and norm referenced elements in to

the same test (Linn & Miller, 2005). That is, the test results not only provide information on mastery of a content

standard but also the percentage of students who attained that level of mastery.

278

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Standardized tests can be high stakes i.e. performance on the test has important consequences. These

consequences can be for students, e.g. passing a high school graduation test is required in order to obtain a diploma

or passing PRAXIS II is a prerequisite to gain a teacher license. These consequences can be for schools, e.g. under

NCLB an increasing percentage of students in every school must reach proficiency in math and reading each year.

Consequences for schools who fail to achieve these gains include reduced funding and restructuring of the school

building. Under NCLB, the consequences are designed to be for the schools not individual students (Popham, 2005)

and their test results may not accurately reflect what they know because students may not try hard when the tests

have low stakes for them (Wise & DeMars, 2005).

Uses of standardized tests

Standardized tests are used for a variety of reasons and the same test is sometimes used for multiple purposes.

Assessing students’ progress in a wider context

Well-designed teacher assessments provide crucial information about each student’s achievement in the

classroom. However, teachers vary in the types of assessment they use so teacher assessments do not usually

provide information on how students’ achievement compares to externally established criteria. Consider two eighth

grade students, Brian and Joshua, who received As in their middle school math classes. However, on the

standardized norm referenced math test Brian scored in the fiftieth percentile whereas Joshua scored in the

ninetieth percentile. This information is important to Brian and Joshua, their parents, and the school personnel.

Likewise, two third grade students could both receive Cs on their report card in reading but one may pass 25 per

cent and the other 65 per cent of the items on the Criterion Referenced State Test.

There are many reasons that students’ performance on teacher assessments and standardized assessments may

differ. Students may perform lower on the standardized assessment because their teachers have easy grading

criteria, or there is poor alignment between the content they were taught and that on the standardized test, or they

are unfamiliar with the type of items on the standardized tests, or they have test anxiety, or they were sick on the

day of the test. Students may perform higher on the standardized test than on classroom assessments because their

teachers have hard grading criteria, or the student does not work consistently in class (e.g. does not turn in

homework) but will focus on a standardized test, or the student is adept at the multiple choice items on the

standardized tests but not at the variety of constructed response and performance items the teacher uses. We

should always be very cautious about drawing inferences from one kind of assessment.

In some states, standardized achievement tests are required for home-schooled students in order to provide

parents and state officials information about the students’ achievement in a wider context. For example, in New

York home-schooled students must take an approved standardized test every other year in grades four through

eight and every year in grades nine through twelve. These tests must be administered in a standardized manner and

the results filed with the Superintendent of the local school district. If a student does not take the tests or scores

below the thirty-third percentile the home schooling program may be placed on probation (New York State

Education Department, 2005).

Diagnosing student’s strengths and weaknesses

Standardized tests, along with interviews, classroom observations, medical examinations, and school records are

used to help diagnose students’ strengths and weaknesses. Often the standardized tests used for this purpose are

Educational Psychology 279 A Global Text

12. Standardized and other formal assessments

administered individually to determine if the child has a disability. For example, if a kindergarten child is having

trouble with oral communication, a standardized language development test could be administered to determine if

there are difficulties with understanding the meaning of words or sentence structures, noticing sound differences in

similar words, or articulating words correctly (Peirangelo & Guiliani, 2002). It would also be important to

determine if the child was a recent immigrant, had a hearing impairment or mental retardation. The diagnosis of

learning disabilities typically involves the administration of at least two types of standardized tests—an aptitude test

to assess general cognitive functioning and an achievement test to assess knowledge of specific content areas

(Peirangelo & Guiliani, 2006). We discuss the difference between aptitude and achievement tests later in this

chapter.

Selecting students for specific programs

Standardized tests are often used to select students for specific programs. For example, the SAT (Scholastic

Assessment Test) and ACT (American College Test) are norm referenced tests used to help determine if high school

students are admitted to selective colleges. Norm referenced standardized tests are also used, among other criteria,

to determine if students are eligible for special education or gifted and talented programs. Criterion referenced tests

are used to determine which students are eligible for promotion to the next grade or graduation from high school.

Schools that place students in ability groups including high school college preparation, academic, or vocational

programs may also use norm referenced or criterion referenced standardized tests. When standardized tests are

used as an essential criteria for placement they are obviously high stakes for students.

Assisting teachers’ planning

Norm referenced and criterion referenced standardized tests, among other sources of information about

students, can help teachers make decisions about their instruction. For example, if a social studies teacher learns

that most of the students did very well on a norm referenced reading test administered early in the school year he

may adapt his instruction and use additional primary sources. A reading teacher after reviewing the poor end-of-

the-year criterion referenced standardized reading test results may decide that next year she will modify the

techniques she uses. A biology teacher may decide that she needs to spend more time on genetics as her students

scored poorly on that section of the standardized criterion referenced science test. These are examples of

assessment for learning which involves data-based decision making. It can be difficult for beginning teachers to

learn to use standardized test information appropriately, understanding that test scores are important information

but also remembering that there are multiple reasons for students’ performance on a test.

Accountability

Standardized tests results are increasingly used to hold teachers and administrators accountable for students’

learning. Prior to 2002, many States required public dissemination of students’ progress but under NCLB school

districts in all states are required to send report cards to parents and the public that include results of standardized

tests for each school. Providing information about students’ standardized tests is not new as newspapers began

printing summaries of students’ test results within school districts in the 1970s and 1980s (Popham, 2005).

However, public accountability of schools and teachers has been increasing in the US and many other countries and

this increased accountability impacts the public perception and work of all teachers including those teaching in

subjects or grade levels not being tested.

280

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

For example, Erin, a middle school social studies teacher, said:

“As a teacher in a 'non-testing' subject area, I spend substantial instructional time suporting the

standardized testing requirements. For example, our school has instituted 'word of the day', which

encourages teachers to use, define, and incorporate terminology often used in the tests (e.g.

“compare”, “oxymoron” etc.). I use the terms in my class as often as possible and incorporate them

into written assignments. I also often use test questions of similar formats to the standardized tests

in my own subject assessments (e.g. multiple choice questions with double negatives, short answer

and extended response questions) as I believe that practice in the test question formats will help

students be more successful in those subjects that are being assessed.”

Accountability and standardized testing are two components of Standards Based Reform in Education that was

initiated in the USA in 1980s. The two other components are academic content standards which are described later

in this chapter and teacher quality which was discussed in Chapter 1.

Types of standardized tests Achievement tests

Summarizing the past: K-12 achievement tests are designed to assess what students have learned in a specific

content area. These tests include those specifically designed by states to access mastery of state academic content

standards (see more details below) as well as general tests such as the California Achievement Tests, The

Comprehensive Tests of Basic Skills, Iowa Tests of Basic Skills, Metropolitan Achievement Tests, and the Stanford

Achievement Tests. These general tests are designed to be used across the nation and so will not be as closely

aligned with state content standards as specifically designed tests. Some states and Canadian Provinces use

specifically designed tests to assess attainment of content standards and also a general achievement test to provide

normative information.

Standardized achievement tests are designed to be used for students in kindergarten though high school. For

young children questions are presented orally, and students may respond by pointing to pictures, and the subtests

are often not timed. For example, on the Iowa Test of Basic Skills (http://www.riverpub.com/) designed for

students are young as kindergarten the vocabulary test assesses listening vocabulary. The teacher reads a word and

may also read a sentence containing the word. Students are then asked to choose one of three pictorial response

options.

Achievement tests are used as one criterion for obtaining a license in a variety of professions including nursing,

physical therapy, and social work, accounting, and law. Their use in teacher education is recent and is part of the

increased accountability of public education and most States require that teacher education students take

achievement tests in order to obtain a teaching license. For those seeking middle school and high school licensure

these are tests are in the content area of the major or minor (e.g. mathematics, social studies); for those seeking

licenses in early childhood and elementary the tests focus on knowledge needed to teach students of specific grade

levels. The most commonly used tests, the PRAXIS series, tests I and II, developed by Educational Testing Service,

include three types of tests (www.ets.org):

• Subject Assessments, these test on general and subject-specific teaching skills and knowledge. They include

both multiple-choice and constructed-response test items.

Educational Psychology 281 A Global Text

12. Standardized and other formal assessments

• Principles of Learning and Teaching (PLT) Tests assess general pedagogical knowledge at four grade levels:

Early Childhood, K-6, 5-9, and 7-12. These tests are based on case studies and include constructed-response

and multiple-choice items. Much of the content in this textbook is relevant to the PLT tests.

• Teaching Foundations Tests assess pedagogy in five areas: multi-subject (elementary), English, Language

Arts, Mathematics, Science, and Social Science.

These tests include constructed-response and multiple-choice items which tests teacher education students. The

scores needed in order to pass each test vary and are determined by each state.

Diagnostic tests

Profiling skills and abilities: Some standardized tests are designed to diagnose strengths and weaknesses in

skills, typically reading or mathematics skills. For example, an elementary school child may have difficult in reading

and one or more diagnostic tests would provide detailed information about three components: (1) word recognition,

which includes phonological awareness (pronunciation), decoding, and spelling; (2) comprehension which includes

vocabulary as well as reading and listening comprehension, and (3) fluency (Joshi 2003). Diagnostic tests are often

administered individually by school psychologists, following standardized procedures. The examiner typically

records not only the results on each question but also observations of the child’s behavior such as distractibility or

frustration. The results from the diagnostic standardized tests are used in conjunction with classroom observations,

school and medical records, as well as interviews with teachers, parents and students to produce a profile of the

student’s skills and abilities, and where appropriate diagnose a learning disability.

Aptitude tests

Predicting the future: Aptitude tests, like achievement tests, measure what students have learned, but rather

than focusing on specific subject matter learned in school (e.g. math, science, English or social studies), the test

items focus on verbal, quantitative, problem solving abilities that are learned in school or in the general culture

(Linn & Miller, 2005). These tests are typically shorter than achievement tests and can be useful in predicting

general school achievement. If the purpose of using a test is to predict success in a specific subject (e.g. language

arts) the best prediction is past achievement in language arts and so scores on a language arts achievement test

would be useful. However when the predictions are more general (e.g. success in college) aptitude tests are often

used. According to the test developers, both the ACT and SAT Reasoning tests, used to predict success in college,

assess general educational development and reasoning, analysis and problem solving as well as questions on

mathematics, reading and writing (http://www.collegeboard.com; http://www.act.org/). The SAT Subject Tests

that focus on mastery of specific subjects like English, history, mathematics, science, and language are used by

some colleges as entrance criteria and are more appropriately classified as achievement tests than aptitude tests

even though they are used to predict the future.

Tests designed to assess general learning ability have traditionally been called Intelligence Tests but are now

often called learning ability tests, cognitive ability tests, scholastic aptitude tests, or school ability tests. The shift in

terminology reflects the extensive controversy over the meaning of the term intelligence and that its traditional use

was associated with inherited capacity (Linn & Miller 2005). The more current terms emphasize that tests measure

developed ability in learning not innate capacity. The Cognitive Abilities Test assesses K-12 students' abilities to

282

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

reason with words, quantitative concepts, and nonverbal (spatial) pictures. The Woodcock Johnson III contains

cognitive abilities tests as well as achievement tests for ages 2 to 90 years (http://www.riverpub.com).

High-stakes testing by states

While many States had standardized testing programs prior to 2000, the number of state-wide tests has grown

enormously since then because NCLB required that all states test students in reading and mathematics annually in

grades third through eighth and at least once in high school by 2005-6. Twenty-three states expanded their testing

programs during 2005-6 and additional tests are being added as testing in science is required by 2007-8. Students

with disabilities and English language learners must be included in the testing and provided a variety of

accommodations so the majority of staff in school districts are involved in testing in some way (Olson, 2005). In

this section we focus on these tests and their implications for teachers and students.

Standards based assessment Academic content standards

NCLB mandates that states must develop academic content standards that specify what students are expected to

know or be able to do at each grade level. These content standards used to be called goals and objectives and it is

not clear why the labels have changed (Popham, 2004). Content standards are not easy to develop—if they are too

broad and not related to grade level, teachers cannot hope to prepare students to meet the standards.

An example, a broad standard in reading is:

“Students should be able to construct meaning through experiences with literature, cultural events

and philosophical discussion” (no grade level indicated). (American Federation of Teachers, 2006, p.

6).

Standards that are too narrow can result in a restricted curriculum. An example of a narrow standard might be:

Students can define, compare and contrast, and provide a variety of examples of synonyms and

antonyms.

A stronger standard is:

“Students should apply knowledge of word origins, derivations, synonyms, antonyms, and idioms to

determine the meaning of words (grade 4) (American Federation of Teachers, 2006, p. 6).

The American Federation of Teachers conducted a study in 2005-6 and reported that some of the standards in

reading, math and science were weak in 32 states. States set the strongest standards in science followed by

mathematics. Standards in reading were particularly problematic and with one-fifth of all reading standards

redundant across the grade levels, i.e. word-by-word repetition across grade levels at least 50 per cent of the time

(American Federation of Teachers, 2006).

Even if the standards are strong, there are often so many of them that it is hard for teachers to address them all

in a school year. Content standards are developed by curriculum specialists who believe in the importance of their

subject area so they tend to develop large numbers of standards for each subject area and grade level. At first

glance, it may appear that there are only several broad standards, but under each standard there are subcategories

called goals, benchmarks, indicators or objectives (Popham, 2004). For example, Idaho’s first grade mathematics

Educational Psychology 283 A Global Text

12. Standardized and other formal assessments

standard, judged to be of high quality (AFT 2000) contains five broad standards, including 10 goals and a total of

29 objectives (Idaho Department of Education, 2005-6).

Alignment of standards, testing and classroom curriculum

The state tests must be aligned with strong content standards in order to provide useful feedback about student

learning. If there is a mismatch between the academic content standards and the content that is assessed then the

test results cannot provide information about students’ proficiency on the academic standards. A mismatch not

only frustrates the students taking the test, teachers, and administrators it undermines the concept of

accountability and the “theory of action” (See box “Deciding for yourself about the research”) that underlies the

NCLB. Unfortunately, the 2006 Federation of Teachers study indicated that in only 11 states were all the tests

aligned with state standards (American Federation of Teachers, 2006).

State standards and their alignment with state assessments should be widely available—preferably posted on the

states websites so they can be accessed by school personnel and the public. A number of states have been slow to do

this.Table 43 summarizes which states had strong content standards, tests that were aligned with state standards,

and adequate documents on online. Only 11 states were judged to meet all three criteria in 2006.

Table 43: Strong content standards, alignment, and transparency: evaluation for each state in 2006 (Adapted

from American Federation of Teachers, 2006).

Standards are

strong

Test documents match

standards

Testing documents

online

Alabama +

Alaska + +

Arizona + +

Arkansas +

California + + +

Colorado +

Connecticut

Delaware +

District of Columbia + +

Florida + +

Georgia + +

Hawaii +

Idaho + +

Illinois +

284

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Indiana + +

Iowa +

Kansas + +

Kentucky + +

Louisiana + + +

Maine +

Maryland +

Massachusetts + +

Michigan + +

Minnesota + +

Mississippi +

Missouri

Montana

Nebraska

Nevada + + +

New Hampshire + +

New Jersey +

New Mexico + + +

New York + + +

North Carolina +

North Dakota + +

Ohio + + +

Oklahoma + +

Oregon + +

Pennsylvania +

Rhode Island + +

South Carolina

Educational Psychology 285 A Global Text

12. Standardized and other formal assessments

South Dakota + +

Tennessee + + +

Texas + +

Utah +

Sampling content

When numerous standards have been developed it is impossible for tests to assess all of the standards every

year, so the tests sample the content, i.e. measure some but not all the standards every year. Content standards

cannot be reliably assessed with only one or two items so the decision to assess one content standard often requires

not assessing another. This means if there are too many content standards a significant proportion of them are not

measured each year. In this situation, teachers try to guess which content standards will be assessed that year and

align their teaching on those specific standards. Of course if these guesses are incorrect students will have studied

content not on the test and not studied content that is on the test. Some argue that this is a very serious problem

with current state testing and Popham (2004) an expert on testing even said: “What a muddleheaded way to run a

testing program.” (p. 79)

Adequate Yearly Progress (AYP)

Under NCLB three levels of achievement, basic, proficient and advanced, must be specified for each grade level

in each content area by each state. States were required to set a time table from 2002 that insured an increasing

percentage of students would reach the proficient levels such that by 2013-14, soevery child is performing at or the

proficient level. Schools and school districts who meet this timetable are said to meet adequate yearly progress

(AYP).

Because every child must reach proficiency by 2013-14 greater increases are required for those schools that had

larger percentages of initially lower performing students.

Exhibit 16 illustrates the progress needed in three hypothetical schools. School A, initially the lowest performing

school, has to increase the number of students reaching proficiency by an average of 6 per cent each year, the

increase is 3 per cent for School B, and the increase is only 1 per cent for School C. Also, the checkpoint targets in

the timetables are determined by the lower performing schools. This is illustrated on the figure by the arrow—it is

obvious that School A has to make significant improvements by 2007-8 but School C does not have to improve at all

by 2007-8. This means that schools that are initially lower performing are much more likely to fail to make AYP

during the initial implementation years of NCLB.

286

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Exhibit 15: Adequate Yearly Progress requires greater student improvement in

schools with lower levels of initial proficiency

Schools A, B and C all must reach 10 per cent student proficiency by 2013-4. However the school that initially

has the lowest level of performance (A) has to increase the percentage of students proficient at a greater rate than

schools with middle (B) or high (C) levels of initial proficiency rates.

Subgroups

For a school to achieve AYP not only must overall percentages of the students reach proficiency but subgroups

must also reach proficiency in a process called desegregation. Prior to NCLB state accountability systems typically

focused on overall student performance but this did not provide incentives for schools to focus on the neediest

students, e.g. those children living below the poverty line (Hess & Petrilli, 2006). Under NCLB the percentages for

each racial/ethnic group in the school (white, African American, Latino, Native American etc.), low income

students, students with limited English proficiency, and students with disabilities are all calculated if there are

enough students in the subgroup. A school may fail AYP if one group, e.g. English language learners do not make

adequate progress. This means that it is more difficult for large diverse schools (typically urban schools) that have

many subgroups to meet the demands of AYP than smaller schools with homogeneous student body (Novak &

Fuller, 2003). Schools can also fail to make AYP if too few students take the exam. The drafters of the law were

concerned that some schools might encourage low-performing students to stay home on the days of testing in order

to artificially inflate the scores. So on average at least 95 per cent of any subgroup must take the exams each year or

the school may fail to make AYP (Hess & Petrilli, 2006).

Sanctions

Schools failing to meet AYP for consecutive years, experience a series of increasing sanctions. If a school fails to

make AYP for two years in row it is labeled “in need of improvement” and school personnel must come up with a

school improvement plan that is based on “scientifically based research”. In addition, students must be offered the

Educational Psychology 287 A Global Text

12. Standardized and other formal assessments

option of transferring to a better performing public school within the district. If the school fails for three

consecutive years, free tutoring must be provided to needy students. A fourth year of failure requires “corrective

actions” which may include staffing changes, curriculum reforms or extensions of the school day or year. If the

school fails to meet AYP for five consecutive years the district must “restructure” which involves major actions such

as replacing the majority of the staff, hiring an educational management company, turning the school over to the

state.

Growth or value added models

One concern with how AYP is calculated is that it is based on an absolute level of student performance at one

point in time and does not measure how much students improve during each year. To illustrate this, Exhibit 16

shows six students whose science test scores improved from fourth to fifth grade. The circle represents a student’s

score in fourth grade and the tip of the arrow the test score in fifth grade. Note that students 1, 2, and 3 all reach the

level of proficiency (the horizontal dotted line) but students 4, 5 and 6 do not. However, also notice that students 2,

5 and 6 improved much more than students 1, 3, and 4. The current system of AYP rewards students reaching the

proficiency level rather than students’ growth. This is a particular problem for low performing schools who may be

doing an excellent job of improving achievement (students 5 and 6) but do not make the proficiency level. The US

Department of Education in 2006 allowed some states to include growth measures into their calculations of AYP.

While growth models traditionally tracked the progress of individual students, the term is sometimes used to refer

to growth of classes or entire schools (Shaul, 2006).

Exhibit 16: An illustration of value added vs proficiency approach to assessment.

Each arrow represents the mathematics achievement results of one student who was

tested in the fourth grade (shown by the dot) and also the fifth grade (shown by the tip

of the arrow).

Some states include growth information on their report cards. For example, Tennessee (http://www.k-

12.state.tn.us/rptcrd05/) provides details on which schools meet the AYP but also whether the students’ scores on

tests represent average growth, above average, or below average growth within the state. Exhibit 17 illustrates in a

288

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

simple way the kind of information that is provided. Students in schools A, B, and C all reached proficiency and

AYP but in Schools D, E, and F did not. However, students in schools A and D had low growth, in schools B and E

average growth, in schools C and F high growth. Researchers have found that in some schools students have high

levels of achievement but do not grow as much as expected (School A), and also that in some schools, the

achievement test scores are not high but the students are growing or learning a lot (School F). These are called

“school effects” and represent the effect of the school on the learning of the students.

Ach

iev

em

en

t

School A School B School C

Proficiency School D School E School F

Low growth Average growth High growth

Exhibit 17: Proficiency and growth information

Growth over one year

Schools can vary on overall school achievement (proficiency) as well as the amount of growth in student

learning, For example schools A, B, and C all have high achievement levels but only in School C do students have,

on average, high growth. Schools D, C, and F all have low levels of proficiency but only in school D do students, on

average, have low growth.

Growth models have intuitive appeal to teachers as they focus on how much a student learned during the school

year—not what the student knew at the start of the school year. The current research evidence suggests that

teachers matter a lot—i.e. students learn much more with some teachers than others. For example, in one study

low-achieving fourth grade students in Dallas, Texas were followed for three years and 90 per cent of those who had

effective teachers passed the seventh grade math test whereas only 42 per cent of those with ineffective teachers

passed (cited in Bracey, 2004). Unfortunately, the same study reported that low achieving students were more

likely to be assigned to ineffective teachers for three years in a row than high achieving students. Some policy

makers believe that teachers who are highly effective should receive rewards including higher salaries or bonuses

and that a primary criterion of effectiveness is assessed by growth models, i.e. how much students learn during a

year (Hershberg, 2004). However, using growth data to make decisions about teachers is controversial as there is

much more statistical uncertainty when using growth measures for a small group or students (e.g. one teacher’s

students) than larger groups (e.g. all fourth graders in a school district).

Growth models are also used to provide information about the patterns of growth among subgroups of students

that may arise from the instructional focus of the teachers. For example, it may be that highest performing students

in the classroom gain the most and the lowest performing students gain the least. This suggests that the teacher is

focusing on the high achieving students and giving less attention to low achieving students. In contrast, it may be

the highest performing students gain the least and the low performing students grow the most suggesting the

teacher focuses on the low performing students and paying little attention to the high performing students. If the

Educational Psychology 289 A Global Text

12. Standardized and other formal assessments

teacher focuses on the students “in the middle” they may grow the most and the highest and lowest performing

students grow the least. Proponents of the value-added or growth models argue that teachers can use this

information to help them make informed decisions about their teaching (Hershberg, 2004).

Differing state standards

Under NCLB each state devises their own academic content standards, assessments, and levels of proficiency.

Some researchers have suggested that the rules of NCLB have encouraged states to set low levels of proficiency so it

is easier to meet AYP each year (Hoff, 2002). Stringency of state levels of proficiency can be examined by

comparing state test scores to scores on a national achievement test called the National Assessment of Educational

Progress (NAEP). NCLB requires that states administer reading and math NAEP tests to a sample of fourth and

eighth grade students every other year. The NAEP is designed to assess the progress of students at the state-wide or

national level not individual schools or students and is widely respected as a well designed test that uses current

best practices in testing. A large percentage of each test includes constructed-response questions and questions that

require the use of calculators and other materials (http://nces.ed.gov/nationsreportcard).

Exhibit 18 illustrates that two states, Colorado and Missouri had very different state performance standards for

the fourth grade reading/language arts tests in 2003. On the state assessment 67 per cent of the students in

Colorado but only 21 per cent of the students in Missouri were classified as proficient. However, on the NAEP tests

34 per cent of Colorado students and 28 per cent of Missouri students were classified as proficient (Linn 2005).

These differences demonstrate that there is no common meaning in current definitions of “proficient achievement”

established by the states.

Exhibit 18: Relationship between state proficiency levels and

scores on NAEP (Adapted from Linn 2005)

Implications for beginning teachers:

Dr Mucci is the principal of a suburban fourth through sixth grade school in Ohio that continues to

meet AYP. We asked her what beginning teachers should know about high stakes testing by the

290

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

states. “I want beginning teachers to be familiar with the content standards in Ohio because they

clearly define what all students should know and be able to do. Not only does teaching revolve

around the standards, I only approve requests for materials or professional development if these are

related to the standards. I want beginning teachers to understand the concept of data-based decision

making. Every year I meet with all the teachers in each grade level (e.g. fourth grade) to look for

trends in the previous year's test results and consider remedies based on these trends. I also meet

with each teacher in the content areas that are tested and discuss every student's achievement in his

or her class so we can develop an instructional plan for every student. All interventions with students

are research based. Every teacher in the school is responsible for helping to implement these

instructional plans, for example the music or art teachers must incorporate some reading and math

into their classes. I also ask all teachers to teach test taking skills, by using similar formats to the

state tests, enforcing time limits, making sure students learn to distinguish between questions that

required an extended response using complete sentences versus those that only requires one or two

words, and ensuring that students answer what is actually being asked. We begin this early in the

school year and continue to work on these skills, so by spring, students are familiar with the format,

and therefore less anxious about the state test. We do everything possible to set each student up for

success.”

The impact of testing on classroom teachers does not just occur in Dr Mucci’s middle school. A national survey

of over 4,000 teachers indicated that the majority of teachers reported that the state mandated tests were

compatible with their daily instruction and were based on curriculum frameworks that all teachers should follow.

The majority of teachers also reported teaching test taking skills and encouraging students to work hard and

prepare. Elementary school teachers reported greater impact of the high stakes tests: 56 per cent reported the tests

influenced their teaching daily or a few times a week compared to 46 per cent of middle school teacher and 28 per

cent of high school teachers. Even though the teachers had adapted their instruction because of the standardized

tests they were skeptical about them with 40 per cent reporting that teachers had found ways to raise test scores

without improving student learning and over 70 per cent reporting that the test scores were not an accurate

measure of what minority students know and can do (Pedulla, Abrams, Madaus, Russell, Ramos, & Miao; 2003).

International testingTesting in the Canadian provinces

Canada has developed a system of testing in the provinces as well as national testing. Each province undertakes

its own curriculum based assessments. At the elementary school level provinces assess reading and writing

(language arts) as well as mathematics (also called numeracy). In the middle grades science and social studies is

often assessed in addition to language arts and mathematics. Summary results of these tests are published but there

are no specific consequences for poor performance for schools. In addition, these tests are not high stakes for

students. At the secondary school level high stakes curriculum based exit tests are common

(http://edudata.educ.ubc.ca/Data_Pages/PanCan.htm).

Canada has developed pan-Canada assessment in mathematics, reading and writing, and science that are

administered to a random sample of schools across the country. These assessments are intended to determine

whether, on average, students across Canada reach similar levels of performance at about the same age

Educational Psychology 291 A Global Text

12. Standardized and other formal assessments

(http://www.cmec.ca/pcap/indexe.stm). They are not intended to provide individual feedback to students are

similar in purpose to the NAEP tests administered in the United States.

International comparisons

Along with the increasing globalization has come an interest with international comparisons in educational

achievement and practices and more than 40 countries participate in two major testing initiatives. The Trends in

International Mathematics and Science Study (TIMSS) have assessed students in fourth and eighth grades four

times through 2007. The Programme for International Assessment (PISA) have assessed 15-year-olds in reading,

mathematical and science literacy in more than forty countries on three times since 2000. The items on both series

of tests include multiple choice, short answer and constructed response formats and are translated into more than

30 languages.

Policy makers are often interested in the comparison of average students’ scores across countries. For example,

in eighth grade science on the 2003 TIMMS students from Canada, United States, Hong Kong, and Australia scored

significantly higher than the international average whereas students from Egypt, Indonesia, and the Philippines

scored significantly below the international average (TIMMS 2003). On the mathematics test in the 2003 PISA, 15-

year-old students from Hong Kong, China and Finland scored higher than students from Canada and New Zealand

who in turn scored higher than the students from United States and Spain, who in turn scored higher than the

student from Mexico and Brazil (OECD, 2004).

Both series of tests also collect survey data from students, teachers or school principals allowing for information

about instructional practices and student characteristics. For example, teachers from the Philippines report

spending almost twice as much time teaching science to fourth graders than in the United States (Martin, Mullis,

Gonzalez, & Chrostowski, (2004). Student reports from PISA indicate that there is considerable cross-country

variation in how much students feel anxiety when doing mathematics. Students in France, Italy, Japan, Korea

report feeling the most anxious whereas students in Denmark, Finland and Netherlands and Sweden feel the least

anxious (OECD 2004).

Understanding test results

In order to understand test results from standardized tests it is important to be familiar with a variety of terms

and concepts that are fundamental to “measurement theory”, the academic study of measurement and assessment.

Two major areas in measurement theory, reliability and validity, were discussed in the previous chapter; in this

chapter we focus on concepts and terms associated with test scores.

The basicsFrequency distributions

A frequency distribution is a listing of the number of students who obtained each score on a test. If 31 students

take a test, and the scores range from 11 to 30 then the frequency distribution might look like Table 44. We also

show the same set of scores on a histogram or bar graph inExhibit 20. The horizontal (or x axis) represents the

score on the test and the vertical axis (y axis) represents the number or frequency of students. Plotting a frequency

distribution helps us see what scores are typical and how much variability there are in the scores. We describe more

precise ways of determining typical scores and variability next.

292

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Table 44: Frequency distribution for 30 scores

Score on test Frequency Central tendency measures

17 1

18 1

19 0

20 3

21 2

22 6 Mode

23 3 Median

24 2 Mean

25 0

26 2

27 6 Mode

28 2

29 2

30 1

TOTAL 31

Exhibit 19: Tests scores from Table 44represented as a bar graph

Educational Psychology 293 A Global Text

12. Standardized and other formal assessments

Central tendency and variability

There are three common ways of measuring central tendency or which score(s) are typical. The mean is

calculated by adding up all the scores and dividing by the number of scores. In the example in Table 44, the mean is

24. The median is the “middle” score of the distribution—that is half of the scores are above the median and half are

below. The median on the distribution is 23 because 15 scores are above 23 and 15 are below. The mode is the score

that occurs most often. In Table 44 there are actually two modes 22 and 27 and so this distribution is described as

bimodal. Calculating the mean, median and mode are important as each provides different information for

teachers. The median represents the score of the “middle” students, with half scoring above and below, but does not

tell us about the scores on the test that occurred most often. The mean is important for some statistical calculations

but is highly influenced by a few extreme scores (called outliers) but the median is not. To illustrate this, imagine a

test out of 20 points taken by 10 students, and most do very well but one student does very poorly. The scores might

be 4, 18, 18, 19, 19, 19, 19, 19, 20, 20. The mean is 17.5 (170/10) but if the lowest score (4) is eliminated the mean is

now is 1.5 points higher at 19 (171/9). However, in this example the median remains at 19 whether the lowest score

is included. When there are some extreme scores the median is often more useful for teachers in indicating the

central tendency of the frequency distribution.

The measures of central tendency help us summarize scores that are representative, but they do not tell us

anything about how variable or how spread out are the scores. Exhibit 20 illustrates sets of scores from two

different schools on the same test for fourth graders. Note that the mean for each is 40 but in School A the scores

are much less spread out. A simple way to summarize variability is the range, which is the lowest score subtracted

from the lowest score. In School A with low variability the range is (45—35) = 10; in the school B the range is ( 55-

22 = 33).

Exhibit 20: Fourth grade math scores in two different schools with the

same mean but different variability

However, the range is only based on two scores in the distribution, the highest and lowest scores, and so does

not represent variability in all the scores. The standard deviation is based on how much, on average, all the

294

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

scores deviate from the mean. In the example in Exhibit 17 the standard deviations are 7.73 for School A and 2.01

for School B. In the exercise below we demonstrate how to calculate the standard deviation.

Calculating a standard deviation

Example: The scores from 11 students on a quiz are 4, 7, 6, 3, 10, 7, 3, 7, 5, 5, and 9

5. Order scores.

6. Calculate the mean score.

7. Calculate the deviations from the mean.

8. Square the deviations from the mean.

9. Calculate the mean of the squared deviations from the mean (i.e. sum the squared deviations from the

mean then divide by the number of scores). This number is called the variance.

10. Take the square root and you have calculated the standard deviation.

Score

(Step 1, order)

Deviation

from the mean

Squared deviation

from the mean

3 -3 9

3 -3 9

4 -2 4 (Step 4-5, complete the calculations)

5 -1 1 Formula:

5 -1 1Standard deviation=∑ Score−Mean2

N

N = Number of scores

6 0 0

7 1 1

7 1 1

7 1 1

9 3 9

10 4 4

TOTAL = 66 40

(Step 2, calculate

mean)

MEAN 66 /11=6

(Step 3, calculate

deviations)

Mean=40 /11=3.64

(Step 6, find the standard deviation)

Standard deviation=3.64=1.91

Exhibit 21: Calculating a standard deviation

The normal distribution

Knowing the standard deviation is particularly important when the distribution of the scores falls on a normal

distribution. When a standardized test is administered to a very large number of students the distribution of scores

Educational Psychology 295 A Global Text

12. Standardized and other formal assessments

is typically similar, with many students scoring close to the mean, and fewer scoring much higher or lower than the

mean. When the distribution of scores looks like the bell shape shown in Exhibit 19 it is called a normal

distribution. In the diagram we did not draw in the scores of individual students as we did in Exhibit 20, because

distributions typically only fall on a normal curve when there are a large number of students; too many to show

individually. A normal distribution is symmetric, and the mean, median and mode are all the same.

Exhibit 22: Bell shaped curve of normal distribution

Normal curve distributions are very important in education and psychology because of the relationship between

the mean, standard deviation, and percentiles. In all normal distributions 34 per cent of the scores fall between the

mean and one standard deviation of the mean. Intelligence tests often are constructed to have a mean of 100 and

standard deviation of 15 and we illustrate that in Exhibit 15.

Exhibit 23: Normal distribution for an IQ test with mean 100 and standard

deviation 15

296

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

In this example, 34 per cent of the scores are between 100 and 115 and as well, 34 per cent of the scores lie

between 85 and 100. This means that 68 per cent of the scores are between -1 and +1 standard deviations of the

mean (i.e. 85 and 115). Note than only 14 per cent of the scores are between +1 and +2 standard deviations of the

mean and only 2 per cent fall above +2 standard deviations of the mean.

In a normal distribution a student who scores the mean value is always in the fiftieth percentile because the

mean and median are the same. A score of +1 standard deviation above the mean (e.g. 115 in the example above) is

the 84 per cent tile (50 per cent and 34 per cent of the scores were below 115). In Exhibit 10 we represent the

percentile equivalents to the normal curve and we also show standard scores. 1

Kinds of test scores

A standard score expresses performance on a test in terms of standard deviation units above of below the

mean (Linn & Miller, 2005). There are a variety of standard scores:

Z-score: One type of standard score is a z-score, in which the mean is 0 and the standard deviation is 1. This

means that a z-score tells us directly how many standard deviations the score is above or below the mean. For

example, if a student receives a z score of 2 her score is two standard deviations above the mean or the eighty-

fourth percentile. A student receiving a z score of -1.5 scored one and one half deviations below the mean. Any score

from a normal distribution can be converted to a z score if the mean and standard deviation is known. The formula

is:

Z−score=Score−mean scoreStandard deviation

So, if the score is 130 and the mean is 100 and the standard deviation is 15 then the calculation is:

Z=130−10015

=2

If you look at Exhibit 15 you can see that this is correct—a score of 130 is 2 standard deviations above the mean

and so the z score is 2.

T-score: A T-score has a mean of 50 and a standard deviation of 10. This means that a T-score of 70 is two

standard deviations above the mean and so is equivalent to a z-score of 2.

Stanines: Stanines (pronounced staynines) are often used for reporting students’ scores and are based on a

standard nine point scale and with a mean of 5 and a standard deviation of 2. They are only reported as whole

numbers and Figure 11-10 shows their relation to the normal curve.

Grade equivalent sores

A grade equivalent score provides an estimate of test performance based on grade level and months of the school

year (Popham, 2005, p. 288). A grade equivalent score of 3.7 means the performance is at that expected of a third

grade student in the seventh month of the school year. Grade equivalents provide a continuing range of grade levels

and so can be considered developmental scores. Grade equivalent scores are popular and seem easy to understand

however they are typically misunderstood. If, James, a fourth grade student, takes a reading test and the grade

equivalent score is 6.0; this does not mean that James can do sixth grade work. It means that James performed on

1 Exhibit 11.10 must be re-drawn. Please contact the Associate Editor for the original.

Educational Psychology 297 A Global Text

12. Standardized and other formal assessments

the fourth grade test as a sixth grade student is expected to perform. Testing companies calculate grade equivalents

by giving one test to several grade levels. For example a test designed for fourth graders would also be given to third

and fifth graders. The raw scores are plotted and a trend line is established and this is used to establish the grade

equivalents. Note that in Error: Reference source not found the trend line extends beyond the grades levels actually

tested so a grade equivalent above 5.0 or below 3.0 is based solely on the estimated trend lines.

Exhibit 24: Using trend lines to estimate grade equivalent

scores

Grade equivalent scores also assume that the subject matter that is being tested is emphasized at each grade

level to the same amount and that mastery of the content accumulates at a mostly constant rate (Popham, 2005).

Many testing experts warn that grade equivalent scores should be interpreted with considerable skepticism and that

parents often have serious misconceptions about grade equivalent scores. Parents of high achieving students may

have an inflated sense of what their child’s levels of achievement.

Issues with standardized tests

Many people have very strong views about the role of standardized tests in education. Some believe they provide

an unbiased way to determine an individual’s cognitive skills as well as the quality of a school or district. Others

believe that scores from standardized tests are capricious, do not represent what students know, and are misleading

when used for accountability purposes. Many educational psychologists and testing experts have nuanced views

and make distinctions between the information standardized tests can provide about students’ performances and

how the tests results are interpreted and used. In this nuanced view, many of the problems associated with

standardized tests arise from their high stakes use such as using the performance on one test to determine selection

into a program, graduation, or licensure, or judging a school as high vs low performing.

Are standardized tests biased?

In a multicultural society one crucial question is: Are standardized tests biased against certain social class,

racial, or ethnic groups? This question is much more complicated than it seems because bias has a variety of

298

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

meanings. An everyday meaning of bias often involves the fairness of using standardized test results to predict

potential performance of disadvantaged students who have previously had few educational resources. For example,

should Dwayne, a high school student who worked hard but had limited educational opportunities because of the

poor schools in his neighborhood and few educational resources in his home, be denied graduation from high

school because of his score on one test. It was not his fault that he did not have the educational resources and if

given a chance with a change his environment (e.g. by going to college) his performance may blossom. In this view,

test scores reflect societal inequalities and can punish students who are less privileged, and are often erroneously

interpreted as a reflection of a fixed inherited capacity. Researchers typically consider bias in more technical ways

and three issues will be discussed: item content and format; accuracy of predictions, and stereotype threat.

Item content and format. Test items may be harder for some groups than others. An example of social class bias

in a multiple choice item asked students the meaning of the term field. The students were asked to read the initial

sentence in italics and then select the response that had the same meaning of field (Popham 2004, p. 24):

My dad’s field is computer graphics.

a. The pitcher could field his position

b. We prepared the field by plowing it

c. The doctor examined my field of vision

d. What field will you enter after college?

Children of professionals are more likely to understand this meaning of field as doctors, journalists and lawyers

have “fields”, whereas cashiers and maintenance workers have jobs so their children are less likely to know this

meaning of field. (The correct answer is D).

Testing companies try to minimize these kinds of content problems by having test developers from a variety of

backgrounds review items and by examining statistically if certain groups find some items easier or harder.

However, problems do exist and a recent analyses of the verbal SAT tests indicated that whites tend to scores better

on easy items whereas African Americans, Hispanic Americans and Asian Americans score better on hard items

(Freedle, 2002). While these differences are not large, they can influence test scores. Researchers think that the

easy items involving words that are used in every day conversation may have subtly different meanings in different

subcultures whereas the hard words (e.g. vehemence, sycophant) are not used in every conversation and so do not

have these variations in meaning. Test formast can also influence test performance. Females typically score better

at essay questions and when the SAT recently added an essay component, the females overall SAT verbal scores

improved relative to males (Hoover, 2006).

Accuracy of predictions

Standardized tests are used among other criteria to determine who will be admitted to selective colleges. This

practice is justified by predictive validity evidence—i.e. that scores on the ACT or SAT are used to predict first year

college grades. Recent studies have demonstrated that the predictions for black and Latino students are less

accurate than for white students and that predictors for female students are less accurate than male students

(Young, 2004). However, perhaps surprisingly the test scores tend to slightly over predict success in college for

black and Latino students, i.e. these students are likely to attain lower freshman grade point averages than

Educational Psychology 299 A Global Text

12. Standardized and other formal assessments

predicted by their test scores. In contrast, test scores tend to slightly under predict success in college for female

students, i.e. these students are likely to attain higher freshman grade point averages than predicted by their test

scores. Researchers are not sure why there are differences in how accurately the SAT and ACT test predict freshman

grades.

Stereotype threat

Groups that are negatively stereotyped in some area, such as women’s performance in mathematics, are in

danger of stereotype threat, i.e. concerns that others will view them through the negative or stereotyped lens

(Aronson & Steele, 2005). Studies have shown that test performance of stereotyped groups (e.g. African Americans,

Latinos, women) declines when it is emphasized to those taking the test that (a) the test is high stakes, measures

intelligence or math and (b) they are reminded of their ethnicity, race or gender (e.g. by asking them before the test

to complete a brief demographic questionnaire). Even if individuals believe they are competent, stereotype threat

can reduce working memory capacity because individuals are trying to suppress the negative stereotypes.

Stereotype threat seems particularly strong for those individuals who desire to perform well. Standardized test

scores of individuals from stereotyped groups may significantly underestimate actual their competence in low-

stakes testing situations.

Do teachers teach to the tests?

There is evidence that schools and teachers adjust the curriculum so it reflects what is on the tests and also

prepares students for the format and types of items on the test. Several surveys of elementary school teachers

indicated that more time was spent on mathematics and reading and less on social studies and sciences in 2004

than 1990 (Jerald, 2006). Principals in high minority enrollment schools in four states reported in 2003 they had

reduced time spent on the arts. Recent research in cognitive science suggests that reading comprehension in a

subject (e.g. science or social studies) requires that students understand a lot of vocabulary and background

knowledge in that subject (Recht & Leslie, 1988). This means that even if students gain good reading skills they will

find learning science and social studies difficult if little time has been spent on these subjects.

Taking a test with an unfamiliar format can be difficult so teachers help students prepare for specific test

formats and items (e.g. double negatives in multiple choice items; constructed response). Earlier in this chapter a

middle school teacher, Erin, and Principal Dr Mucci described the test preparation emphasis in their schools. There

is growing concern that the amount of test preparation that is now occurring in schools is excessive and students

are not being educated but trained to do tests (Popham, 2004).

Do students and educators cheat?

It is difficult to obtain good data on how widespread cheating is but we know that students taking tests cheat and

others, including test administrators, help them cheat (Cizek, 2003; Popham 2006). Steps to prevent cheating by

students include protecting the security of tests, making sure students understand the administration procedures,

preventing students from bringing in their notes or unapproved electronic devices as well as looking at each others

answers. Some teachers and principals have been caught using unethical test preparation practices such as giving

actual test items to students just before the tests, giving students more time than is allowed, answering students’

questions about the test items, and actually changing students’ answers (Popham, 2006). Concerns in Texas about

cheating led to the creation of an independent task force in August 2006 with 15 staff members from the Texas

300

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Education Agency assigned investigate test improprieties. (Jacobson, 2006). While the pressure on schools and

teachers to have their student perform well is large these practices are clearly unethical and have lead to school

personnel being fired from their jobs (Cizek, 2003).

Chapter summary

Standardized tests are developed by a team of experts and are administered in standard ways. They are used for

a variety of educational purposes including accountability. Most elementary and middle school teachers are likely to

be responsible for helping their students attain state content standards and achieve proficiency on criterion-

referenced achievement tests. In order for teachers to interpret test scores and communicate that information to

students and parents they have to understand basic information about measures of central tendency and

variability, the normal distribution, and several kinds of test scores. Current evidence suggests that standardized

tests can be biased against certain groups and that many teachers tailor their curriculum and classroom tests to

match the standardized tests. In addition, some educators have been caught cheating.

Key terms Achievement tests

Aptitude testsAYP (Annual Yearly Progress)

Criterion referenced testsDiagnostic tests

Frequency distributionGrade equivalent scores

High stakes tests

Mean

MedianMode

Norm referenced tests Range

Standard deviationStanine

Z-score

On the Internet

The National Center for Research on Evaluation, Standards, and Student

Testing (CRESST) at UCLA focuses on research and development that improves assessment and accountability

systems. It has resources for researchers, K-12 teachers, and policy makers on the implications of NCLB as well as

classroom assessment.

This is the home page of Educational Testing services which administers the PRAXIS II series

of tests and has links to the testing requirements for teachers seeking licensure in each state District of Columbia

and the US Virgin Islands.

This is US Department of Education website devoted to

promoting information and supporting and NCLB. Links for teachers and the summaries of the impact of NCLB in

each state are provided.

References

American Federation of Teachers (2006, July) Smart Testing: Let’s get it right. AFT Policy Brief. Retrieved

August 8th 2006 from http://www.aft.org/presscenter/releases/2006/smarttesting/Testingbrief.pdf

Aronson, J., & Steele, C. M. (2005). Stereotypes and the Fragility of Academic Competence, Motivation, and

Self-Concept. In A. J. Elliott & C. S. Dweck (Eds.). Handbook of competence and motivation. (pp.436-

456) Guilford Publications, New York.

Educational Psychology 301 A Global Text

12. Standardized and other formal assessments

Bracey, G. W. (2004). Value added assessment findings: Poor kids get poor teachers. Phi Delta Kappan, 86,

331- 333

Cizek, G. J. (2003). Detecting and preventing classroom cheating: Promoting integrity in assessment. Corwin

Press, Thousand Oaks, CA.

Combined Curriculum Document Reading 4.1 (2006). Accessed November 19, 2006 from

http://www.education.ky.gov/KDE/Instructional+Resources/Curriculum+Documents+and+Resources/

Teaching+Tools/Combined+Curriculum+Documents/default.htm

Freedle, R. O. (2003). Correcting the SAT’s ethnic and social–class bias: A method for reestimating SAT

scores. Harvard Educational Review, 73 (1), 1-42.

Fuhrman, S. H. (2004). Introduction, In S. H. Fuhrman & R. F. Elmore (Eds). Redesigning accountability

systems for education. (pp. 3-14). New York: Teachers College Press.

Haertel, E. & Herman, J. (2005) A historical perspective on validity arguments for accountability testing. In

J. L.Herman & E. H. Haertel (Eds.) Uses and misuses of data for educational accountability and

improvement. 104th Yearbook of the National Society for the Study of Education. Malden, MA: Blackwell

Hershberg, T. (2004). Value added assessment: Powerful diagnostics to improve instruction and promote

student achievement. American Association of School Administrators, Conference Proceedings. Retrieved

August 21 2006 from www.cgp.upenn.edu/ope_news.html

Hess, F. H. Petrilli, M. J. (2006). No Child Left Behind Primer. New York: Peter Lang.

Hoff, D. J. (2002) States revise meaning of proficient. Educational Week, 22,(6) 1,24-25.

Hoover, E. (2006, October 21). SAT scores see largest dip in 31 years. Chronicle of Higher Education, 53(10),

A1.

Human Resources Division (n. d.). Firefighter Commonwealth of Massachusetts Physical Abilities Test (PAT)

Accessed November, 19, 2006 from http://www.mass.gov/?

pageID=hrdtopic&L=2&L0=Home&L1=Civil+Service&sid=Ehrd

Idaho Department of Education (2005-6). Mathematics Content standards and assessment by grade level.

Accessed November 22 2006 from http://www.sde.idaho.gov/instruct/standards/

Jacobson, L. (2006). Probing Test irregularities: Texas launches inquiry into cheating on exams. Education

Week, 28(1), 28

Jerald, C. D (2006,August).The Hidden costs of curriculum narrowing. Issue Brief, Washington DC: The

Center for Comprehensive School Reform and Improvement. Accessed November 21, 2006 from

www.centerforcsri.org/

Joshi, R. M. (2003). Misconceptions about the assessment and diagnosis of reading disability. Reading

Psychology, 24, 247-266.

Linn, R. L., & Miller, M. D. (2005). Measurement and Assessment in Teaching 9th ed. Upper Saddle River,

NJ: Pearson .

302

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Linn, R. L. (2005). Fixing the NCLB Accountability System. CRESST Policy Brief 8. Accessed September 21,

2006 from http://www.cse.ucla.edu/products/policybriefs_set.htm

New York State Education Department (2005). Home Instruction in New York State. Accessed on November

19, 2006 from http://www.emsc.nysed.gov/nonpub/part10010.htm

Martin, M.O., Mullis, I.V.S., Gonzalez, E.J., & Chrostowski, S.J. (2004). Findings From IEA’s Trends in

International Mathematics and Science Study at the Fourth and Eighth Grades Chestnut Hill, MA:

TIMSS & PIRLS International Study Center, Boston College. Accessed September 23, 2006 from

http://timss.bc.edu/timss2003i/scienceD.html

Novak, J. R. & Fuller, B (2003, December), Penalizing diverse schools? Similar test scores, but different

students bring federal sanctions. Policy analysis for policy education. University of California, Berkeley

School of Education: Berkeley CA. Accessed on September 21, 2006 from

http://pace.berkeley.edu/pace_index.html

(OECD 2004). Learning for Tomorrow's World—First Results from PISA 2003. Accessed on September 23,

2006 from http://www.pisa.oecd.org/document/

Olson, L. (2005, November 30th). State test program mushroom as NCLB kicks in. Education Week 25(13)

10-12.

Pedulla, J Abrams, L. M. Madaus, G. F., Russell, M. K., Ramos, M. A., & Miao, J. (2003). Perceived effects of

state-mandated testing programs on teaching and learning: Findings from a national survey of

teachers. Boston College, Boston MA National Board on Educational Testing and Public Policy. Accessed

September 21 2006 from http://escholarship.bc.edu/lynch_facp/51/

Popham, W. J. (2004). America’s “failing” schools. How parents and teachers can copy with No Child Left

Behind. New York: Routledge Falmer.

Popham, W. J. (2005). Classroom Assessment: What teachers need to know. Boston:, MA: Pearson.

Popham, W. J. (2006). Educator cheating on No Child Left Behind Tests. Educational Week, 25 (32) 32-33.

Recht, D. R. & Leslie, L. (1988). Effect of prior knowledge on good and poor readers’ memory of text. Journal

of Educational Psychology 80, 16-20.

Shaul, M. S. (2006). No Child Left Behind Act: States face challenges measuring academic growth.

Testimony before the House Committee on Education and the Workforce Government Accounting Office.

Accessed September 25, 2006 from www.gao.gov/cgi-bin/getrpt?GAO-06-948T

Stiggins, R (2004). New Assessment Beliefs for a New School Mission, Phi Delta Kappan, 86 (1) 22 -27.

Wise, S. L. & DeMars, C. W. (2005). Low examinee effort in low-stakes assessment: Problems and potential

solutions. Educational Assessment 10(1), 1-17.

Young, J. W. (2004). Differential validity and prediction: Race and sex differences in college admissions

testing. In R. Zwick (Ed). Rethinking the SAT: The future of standardized testing in university

admissions. New York (pp. 289-301). Routledge Falmer.

Educational Psychology 303 A Global Text

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Appendix A: Preparing for licensure

If you live in the United States or another country that certifies or licenses teachers with some form of test or

assessment of knowledge of teaching, you will find the following case studies helpful in preparing for at least the

test. The cases each deal with a realistic teaching problem or dilemma. They are followed by a few questions that

can, in principle, be answered in short (half-page) essay format. (This style parallels the style of the PRAXIS II

examination taken by many future teachers in the United States.) The content or topic of the cases parallel major

topics of the chapters of Educational Psychology—one case per chapter.

Readers who are planning to take the PRAXIS II test, especially the version called “Principles of Learning and

Teaching”, will know that the test also includes a number of structured, multiple-choice items. We have not

included any examples of multiple-choice test items here, but they are widely available in various published study

guides for the PRAXIS II. Perhaps the most authoritative is the one published by the administrators of the PRAXIS

itself, the Educational Testing Service:

Educational Testing Service. (2004). Study guide for Principles of Learning and Teaching, 2nd edition.

Princeton, NJ, USA: Author.

Educational Psychology 304 A Global Text

Appendix A: Preparing for licensure

Preparing for licensure : the decline and fall of Jane Gladstone

See also Chapter 2, The learning process; Chapter 7, Classroom management and the learning environment.

Jane Gladstone was student teaching in a sixth-grade classroom. She had been there for several

weeks, helping with activities and occasionally leading specific activities that she had devised

herself. She liked the students and felt that she had been developing good relationships with them.

One morning Ms Wilson, her supervising teacher, had to leave unexpectedly. “Something’s come

up, Jane, and the principal needs me to come to a meeting right away. It could be awhile before I’m

back, so you’ll need to take care of things. But you know the routines now, don’t you?”

Jane was surprised and a bit worried, but also excited by the challenge. She did indeed know the

routines, so she smiled cheerfully as Ms Wilson went out the door. “OK, everyone”, she said. “We’ll

start with language arts. Turn to where we left off yesterday, page 46.”

“But Ms Gladstone”, said Paul, “We actually left off on page 32.”

“No, dummy!” chimed in Katherine, “You were absent yesterday, and the day before we had an

assembly. Remember?” Suddenly three or four students were discussing where in fact the class had

left off in the book, and therefore where Jane ought to begin. Jane was wondering that herself.

“Page 46!” she said firmly—actually more firmly than she had intended. But the students agreed,

and the lesson began. The lesson turned out to be a short story about an athlete who trained hard

as a runner for a local competition. Students took turns reading selections from the story, and in

this way got about half way through it. Then Joe raised his hand.

“Ms Gladstone”, he asked. “Do you think athletes should be arrested for taking steroids?” Jane was

taken off guard by this. She had been determined to finish the lesson smoothly. All she could think

to say was, “Well I don’t know. That’s a hard question.”

“My dad says they should be arrested, and that no one should have any doubts about that.”

In seconds the language arts lesson was forgotten and students were arguing about whether

athletes should take drugs. For the moment Jane was on the sidelines.

“My uncle took steroids at university”, said Frank, “and it never hurt him.”

“Gross!” called out Jill from across the room. “I suppose you take them too, then?”

“What’s that supposed to mean?” asked Frank, obviously annoyed.

“She’s saying your too fat, Frank”, said Joe. “That’s what steroids do, you know.”

Jane was getting worried. How could she get the discussion back on track? Students were just

getting more worked up.

305

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

“I’ve never taken any drugs!”

“Not real drugs—steroids—you weren’t listening.”

“I bet you have, though…”

On it went, with some students getting annoyed and others clearly tuning out. What if Ms Wilson

came back now?

“BE QUIET!” Jane shouted, surprised at hearing herself be so loud. Everyone got still instantly,

stunned and surprised. But not for long.

“Be quiet!” someone mimicked softly from the back of the room. A few snickers. Then someone else

said it, with sarcasm dripping from the words. “Be quiet!” Jane glowered at the class, wondering

what to do next.

Questions

➢ What did Jane do wrong?

➢ (a) How could the students’ inappropriate behaviors be considered examples of operant

behaviors being reinforced?

➢ (b) In what way did Jane’s “clamping down” on the students reinforce Jane?

➢ Describe briefly a way for Jane and Ms Wilson to prevent behavior problems from

occurring when and if Jane has to take over the class unexpectedly.

Educational Psychology 306 A Global Text

Appendix A: Preparing for licensure

Preparing for licensure : Joey’s individual educational plan

See also Chapter 5, Students with special educational needs; Chapter 10, Planning instruction.

The following are excerpts from two parts of the Individual Educational Plan (IEP) for a fourth-

grade student named Joey who has an intellectual disability. The excerpts list various performance

objectives and actions, but only some of these are complete. For others—marked with question

marks (??)—relevant information or plans have been deliberately left blank. Read the excerpts and

then respond to the questions that follow:

Part 1: domain—communication

Performance

Objectives

Methods, Materials,

or Strategies

Roles and

Responsibilities

Assessment

1. Joey will increase his

vocabulary in all areas—

people, things, and

actions.

– Joey will use pictures

to learn new words

– TA will prompt active

responses—e.g. “Show me

__.”

– Conversation book

with pictures of Joey doing

things

Teacher:

-monitor

-plan daily activities

TA (i.e. “Teacher

Assistant”):

-modeling

-direct instruction

-informal observation

-checklists re whether

desired vocabulary is being

learned

2. Joey will begin using

2- or 3-word sentences

more often.

-Joey will be provided

with model phrases such

as: “__??__”

-Joey will be given an

entire sentence and then

__??__

Teacher:

-modeling

TA:

-modeling

-facilitating

-informal observations

-checklists of particular

sentences used

307

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Part 2: domain—academic/cognitive

Performance Objectives Methods, Materials,

or Strategies

Roles and

Responsibilities

Assessment

3. Joey will recognize and print his

name.

-Cards with one

student’s name per card.

Joey will __??__

-Make name using

__??__

-__??__

Teacher:

-monitor

TA:

-facilitate

-model

– __??__

4. ___??___ -__??__

-__??__

Teacher:

-monitor

TA:

-facilitate

-model

-__??__

-__??__

Questions

➢ For Performance Objectives #2 and #3, the sample phrases and model sentences are

missing in the “Methods, Materials, and Strategies” column. Suggest two reasonable

sample phrases and two model sentences to fill in these blanks. Then suggest how each of

the teaching strategies illustrate principles of learning.

➢ For Performance Objective #3, what is missing in the “Assessment” column? Suggest a

reasonable method of assessment and then explain (1) why the method would be both valid

and practical, and (2) any cautions the teacher should be aware of in using the method of

assessment.

➢ Performance Objective #4 is missing both “Methods, Materials, and Strategies” and

“Assessment.” Fill in both boxes—i.e. suggest two ways of implementing the objective and

two ways of assessing it. Then explain how your suggestions reflect the nature of

Performance Objective #4.

Educational Psychology 308 A Global Text

Appendix A: Preparing for licensure

Preparing for licensure: Rosemary’s instructional decision

See also Chapter 8, Nature of classroom communication; Chapter 9, Facilitating complex thinking.

Rosemary had planned a lesson for her second grade class about personal and social management,

but she was not satisfied with it. She had taken the general goal directly from the state’s official

curriculum guide for health education: “Students will identify positive communication skills”, it

said. But the guide said nothing about how to translate this goal into practice.

She was thinking that she would use puppets to demonstrate how to communicate in positive ways.

The puppets would engage in dialogue, during which they would nod their heads appropriately,

focus on the speaker, not interrupt, and keep still while listening. Maybe she would include a few

communication mistakes as well—times when a puppet might interrupt in appropriately, for

example—and challenge students to identify those moments.

Her plan seemed fine as far as it went, but she felt unsure about two things. One concern was how

to make sure that students got the point of the activity, and did not regard it simply as

entertainment. How should she introduce the activity? What should she say about it, either

beforehand, during, or afterwards? What exactly should she tell students she is expecting from

them?

The other concern was with the very format of the activity. She did not want students just to know

about good communication skills; she wanted them to use them as well. The puppets did not seem

to help with this latter purpose. How, she wondered, could she get students to take responsibility

for practicing good communication? Was there a way to modify or extend the puppet activity that

would do this? Or perhaps additional activities that students could do?

Think of the range of instructional strategies available to Rosemary. Then answer each of the

following questions.

Questions

➢ Choose two strategies that would help her with the first of her concerns—with making sure

that students understood the purpose of the puppets lesson. Compose an imaginary script

of what she might say before, during, and after using the puppets in the way described.

➢ Devise one way to modify the puppet activity so that it focused less on students’ knowledge

of communication and more on students’ skills with communication.

➢ Devise one additional activity to develop students’ skills with communication and their

sense of responsibility for doing so. Outline each activity in point (or summary) form.

309

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

➢ Comment briefly on how each of your answers above (to Questions #1-3) draws on

principles and methods of major instructional strategies.

Educational Psychology 310 A Global Text

Appendix A: Preparing for licensure

Preparing for licensure: Mr Cullen teaches about houses

See also Chapter 10, Planning instruction; Chapter 4, Student diversity.

Mr Cullen teaches fifth grade at an urban elementary school, where one of the normal curriculum

topics is about “where people live”. The general goal of the unit, as expressed in the curriculum

framework document from the State Department of Education, is for students “to understand the

nature and purposes of houses and how they are affected by the circumstances of their society”. To

get started in planning the unit, Mr Cullen brainstormed the conceptual web of ideas and topics

shown below as “Document 1” below. He also introduced students to making conceptual webs

about houses, one of which is shown below as “Document 2”. Part way through the unit, in

addition, he took the class on a simple field trip to look at the houses in the residential

neighborhood near the school. His notes about that field trip are shown as “Document 3”.

Document 1: Mr Cullen’s conceptual map about houses

Styles Sizes Types Brick Wood Plaster

Appearance Construction materials

HOUSES Costs Purchase Price

Maintenance Rental costs

Parts of a House Effects of Location

Inside

Bath Outside Neighboring Schools Street

Bedroom(s) Garage Houses & Stores traffic

Kitchen Yard

311

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Document 2: Jill’s conceptual map about houses

Color Size Painted? Yard tidy?

Similarity & Differences Appearance of

among Houses Houses

HOUSES

Parts of Houses Location of Houses

Bedrooms Furnace/heater Near friends? Near stores?

Privacy? Warm enough? Near bus stop

Document 3: Notes on Field Trip Looking at Neighborhood Houses

20 students walked about ½ mile around the neighborhood (30 minutes) and viewed

approximately 100 houses in the process. Behavior during the walk was generally good.

Several questions asked by students (no particular order of importance):

(a) “Who lives in these houses?”

(b) “Why are some houses painted nicely but others are not?”

(c) “Why are some houses bigger than others?”

(d) “Where are some really BIG houses?”

(e) “Do children play on these yards and streets?”

Question to myself: How to address these questions back in class?

Questions

➢ Compare the concept webs of Mr Cullen and of the two students. Identify one topic or goal

that appears to be a priority for all three individuals. Then devise a strategy or activity

appropriate for teaching about this topic or goal. Use principles of instructional planning

such as described in this chapter.

Educational Psychology 312 A Global Text

Appendix A: Preparing for licensure

➢ Identify a topic or goal that appears to be a priority only for Mr Cullen (i.e. that does not

appear on either student’s concept web). Devise a strategy or activity appropriate for

teaching about this topic or goal, taking into account the possibility that the students have

less prior knowledge or motivation about this topic than about the one identified in

Question #1.

➢ Identify a topic or goal that appears to be a priority only for one of the students (i.e. that

appears only on that student’s concept web). Devise a strategy or activity appropriate for

supporting the student in pursuing this topic, and for connecting it to at least one other

topic in Mr Cullen’s concept web.

➢ Choose any two of the questions cited in Mr Cullen’s field trip notes. Discuss how these

questions might be addressed in the context of one of the other activities described in

Questions #1-#3 “Deciding for yourself about research”.

313

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Preparing for licensure: facilitating students’ communication through group work

See also Chapter 7, Classroom management and the learning environment; Chapter 8, The nature of classroom

communication.

Scene 1: Barbara Fuller makes plans

It is late August.

Barbara Fuller, a third grade teacher, peers about her classroom, wondering how she will organize

her program for the coming year. She wants to try some sort of collaborative group work because

she has heard good things about it—especially that it gets students talking to each other in ways

that are productive rather than mere chit-chat. Ms Fuller is thinking of trying a group project for

social studies that she is calling “How many people does it take to raise a child?” Students are

supposed to explore how people outside the family contribute to the welfare of infants and

children. If they do collaborate successfully, then students can pool their research, share ideas and

interpretations, and present their results to class more effectively.

Ms Fuller has read some very specific literature about how to get started with collaborative group

work. One book recommends, for example, that she assign the members of each group rather than

letting students select their own groups; this procedure is supposed to avoid cliques and ensure

that everyone is included. But it also means that some group mates may not be each other’s first

choice. Ms Fuller considers this trade-off carefully, and finally decides to go ahead and assign the

group members herself. To minimize possible complaints, she also decides to give each group an

immediate task: choose a leader as well as a name for the group.

Scene 2: Collaborate groups that don’t collaborate

Two weeks into the term, Ms Fuller begins the collaborative project about community helpers. She

describes the purposes and advantages of group work: students can help each other, cover more

reading material, and enjoy each other’s company. They will also be challenged to explain what

they learn to each other and to justify to each other their ideas for the final report and presentation.

Once the work begins, she begins noticing a variety of reactions from groups and the individuals

within them. In one group (Ms Fuller dubs it “Group 1”), for example, Tom complains to her that

he is the only boy in the group. “You’ll be fine,” she says to Tom. “If you look around, you’ll see that

most people are with other people they don’t know terribly well.” Tom looks uncomfortable with

her response, but continues working.

In Group 2, Jasmine takes over almost immediately; everyone seems to agree that she should have

this job. Unfortunately Jasmine is not pleased to be in charge: she issues orders reluctantly (“Kyla,

you look up about nurses”), to which her group happily agree.

Educational Psychology 314 A Global Text

Appendix A: Preparing for licensure

In Group 3, Ken and Serge confer about the project, but ignore the girls in the group. The girls soon

are chatting about activities outside school, doodling in their notebooks, and apparently

daydreaming.

In Group 4, Ms Fuller can hear voices periodically rising in anger. She can’t make out who is saying

what, but it seems to involve Jennifer, Sean, and possibly Lavar. The other two group members are

sitting quietly, simply observing the argument and presumably waiting for it to be over.

Questions

➢ If you could speak to Ms Fuller right now (at the end of Scene 2), what advice could you

give her to assist in continuing the activity? For this question, take the situation as it has in

fact evolved so far; avoid giving advice, that is like “You should never have done X in the

first place.” Focus your advice on developing effective strategies of communication, either

for Ms Fuller, for the students, or for both.

➢ Now imagine that you can, miraculously, turn the clock back to the beginning of Scene 1,

when Ms Fuller was planning the collaborative activity in the first place. What advice could

you give her at that initial point in time? Again, focus your advice on developing effective

strategies of communication, either for Ms Fuller, for the students, or for both.

➢ Consider how nonverbal communication among the students might be affecting students’

experience in particular. Describe a way in which one or more features of nonverbal

communication might cause a collaborate group to fall apart or become unproductive.

Then suggest ways that Ms Fuller might be able to help so that members of the group

remained mutually supportive and productive.

315

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Preparing for licensure: Ms Scanton teaches second grade

See also Chapter 10 Planning instruction; Appendix C: The reflective practitioner.

When Ms Scanton taught second grade, she kept a journal about her experiences. Sometimes she

simply recorded interesting facts or information individuals, but other times she commented and

reflected on individuals at more length. Here are three of her journal entries.

Document 1: Ms Scanton’s observation of Ashley’s writing

October 4: Ashley procrastinated a lot again today during journal-writing time—stared at the

ceiling, at the kids near her, etc. etc. I reminded everyone that they were supposed to write about

“this week at school,” but it didn’t seem to sink in with Ashley. After sitting a long time, she drew a

picture of her family—Mom, Dad, cat.

I know she especially loves her cat, but I also thought she should follow directions more closely. I

ask her, “Is that all you’ve done?” She frowns. I smile—a sincere one.

“It’s hard,” she says cautiously—referring to the writing itself.

“But that’s why it’s important to work at it—to actually write,” say I.

A pause. Then, “How do you spell ‘Mom’?” she asks.

I tell her to sound it out; ask what is the first sound, etc. /m/…/ah/…/m/… She says these sounds

slowly, maybe to please me.

“/m/,” she repeats, and then write down one letter: M. I have to leave to check on others. From a

distance I see her write down K, then erase and switch it to C. Was she sounding out “cat”?

Document 2: Later that year: Ms Scanton’s log of Ashley’s misspellings

Ashley, November 21: Ashley is still misspelling so many words in her writing that I’m getting

exasperated, to put it nicely. Here’s a list of her misspellings from the past two weeks—mostly from

her journal:

techrs (teachers)

milmen(mailmen)

peliec (police)

fier pepel (fire people)

pepl (people)

librein (librarian)

lectrisudie (electricity)

Educational Psychology 316 A Global Text

Appendix A: Preparing for licensure

doctrs (doctors)

nrsis (nurses)

loyors (lawyers)

* What to do for her? *

Document #3: Still later: Ms Scanton journal reflections

April 21: Just finished a cool book, GNYS AT WRK, by Glenda Bissex, that made me think about

Ashley and her misspellings. The author described her son’s invented spellings and how they

became more plentiful and complicated at first, but eventually became more “adultlike” or

conventional. Fascinating! The mom was in no hurry to cure her child of his spelling problems, but

he seemed to outgrow them on his own. The chief point seemed to be that invented spellings may

be good because they show active efforts by the child to figure out the rules of spelling.

So maybe Ashley will outgrow her misspellings too? I do note that her misspellings—her inventions

—have become more complicated across the year. Here’s from her journal last week:

TDA W WT T JM NAD W PLY TWLIT TAG

With a little help from Ashley, I figured this out as “Today we went to gym and we played toilet

tag.”

Here’s from the week before:

TIZ WK W MAD PTY MAX.

Translation: “This week we made party masks.”

Quite a difference from the start of the year, when she would only write down a couple of letters

during journal-writing time! Maybe I need to support her efforts more and worry about them less.

But how to do that and still make sure she really is learning how to spell?

P.S. If you are interested, the book about invented spellings that Ms Scanton mentioned in

Document #3 is listed in the bibliography.

Questions

➢ In these excerpts from her journal, Ms Scanton has observed and reflected on Ashley’s

learning to spell. Considered her activities as a whole, how much do they qualify as action

research in the sense described in this chapter? What should be added or changed to make

her activities a full-fledged example of action research?

➢ Consider the list of misspellings (or invented spellings) in Document #2. Suggest how, if at

all, Ms Scanton might address those misspellings with Ashley. In particular, comment on

whether she should make time to work with Ashley in the same way that she did in

317

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Document #1. Assuming that she does make time, how she might set priorities about

which word(s) to focus on if she does not have time to deal with them all?

➢ Suppose that Ms Scanton does not have time to review every misspelled word with Ashley.

How else could she address Ashley’s spelling problems? Consider briefly how she might

use each of the following: (1) peer tutor, (2) classroom computer, (3) homework. Comment

on how Ms Scanton might collect information about each of these strategies.

Educational Psychology 318 A Global Text

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Appendix B: Deciding for yourself about the research

In most of this book, educational research is used to establish ideas and advice about teaching. Except in

Chapter 13 (“The reflective practitioner”), however, we do not analyze research studies in depth. In this appendix

therefore we look at examples or cases of research studies in detail. We describe how they were conducted, reflect

on their significance and meaning, and pose a few questions to consider about each study. There is approximately

one case per chapter, and their topics roughly parallel one or more of the major themes of each chapter. The

references for the books cited are listed at the end of each case study, as well as in the bibliographies of the chapter

related to each case. Looking at the research studies in detail gives a different perspective on the research than

simply using the results: “up close” the implications of a study are often not as clear as when seen from a distance.

Educational Psychology 319 A Global Text

Appendix B: Deciding for yourself about the research

Deciding for yourself about the research

Chapter 1, The changing teaching profession and you: effects of high-stakes testing on learning educational psychology

As indicated in this chapter, the trend toward high-stakes testing has affected even the preparation

of teachers themselves. Many American states now require new teachers to pass a standardized test

of subject matter knowledge, and many also require them to pass a test about educational

psychology—the sort of content that is the focus of this textbook (Cochran-Smith, 2003;

Educational Testing Service [ETS], 2004). These changes highlight make the issues about testing

very vivid—and at times anxiety-provoking—for many new teachers.

Rosemary Sutton studied the effects of high-stakes testing on her own teaching of educational

psychology as well as on her undergraduate students’ responses to studying this subject (Sutton,

2004). In her state of Ohio, new teachers must all take a test called the “PRAXIS II: Principles of

Learning and Teaching” (ETS, 2004). She reported experiencing a number of new instructional

dilemmas as a result of this test being introduced as a requirement for teacher licensing and

certification, and she described how she resolved them. The effects of the dilemmas and of her

solutions to them were not uniform, but depended on the particular feature of the course.

One negative effect was that Professor Sutton felt more pressure to cover as much of the content of

the PRAXIS in her course as possible, so that students could be prepared as well as possible for the

test. Doing so, however, meant covering more material and therefore reducing depth of coverage of

certain topics. This was a serious problem, she feared, because some parts of the course became

more shallow or fragmented. She also had less time for open-ended discussions that truly followed

interests expressed by the students.

On the other hand, Professor Sutton also reported diversifying her teaching methods—for example

by using more group work and less lecturing—as a way to make class sessions more interesting and

motivating, and therefore insuring that students learned the increased material as well as possible.

She also began using more assignments that resembled the PRAXIS test itself. In this case

imitating the PRAXIS meant giving “case study quizzes” throughout the semester, which were

featured prominently on the PRAXIS. The quizzes consisted of short anecdotes or stories followed

by open-ended questions which students answered the space of a few sentences or brief paragraph.

Since the students knew that the quizzes were a type of preparation for licensing, they tolerated

them well, and even welcomed them. She and the students felt as if they were “on the same side”,

working together to help the students pass their exam. The relationship was therefore more

positive and less “conflicted” compared to earlier times when Professor Sutton was expected not

only to teach the students, but also to evaluate them.

320

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

With the introduction of the licensing exam, finally, some students seemed to regard educational

psychology as more important than in the past—even using university break weeks for additional

study of the textbook! On the other hand, some students seemed to worry about their performance

on the test, and their anxiety may have interfered with learning about educational psychology

itself. Their worries created a dilemma that Professor never truly resolved: how to get students to

prepare for the test seriously without arousing undue worry or anxiety in them?

Questions

➢ How well do you feel that Professor Sutton’s dilemmas about high-stakes testing reflect the

dilemmas that public school teachers might face in preparing their own students for high-

stakes tests?

➢ On balance, and taking into account Professor Sutton’s experience, do you think that high-

stakes tests are desirable?

References

Cochran-Smith, M. (2003). Assessing assessment in teacher education. Journal of Teacher Education, 54(3),

187-191.

Educational Testing Service. (2004). Study guide for Principles of Learning and Teaching, 2nd edition.

Princeton, NJ: Author.

Educational Psychology 321 A Global Text

Appendix B: Deciding for yourself about the research

Deciding for yourself about the research

Chapter 2, The learning process: behaviorist and constructivist teaching compared

Numerous educators have planned and implemented activities and curriculum units that use either

behaviorist or constructivist principles in one way or another. Often the demonstrated activities or

units are hard to compare directly simply because behaviorism and constructivism address

different aspects of learning, and therefore call upon teachers to perform somewhat different roles.

To see what I mean, look at these two examples of instructional research. The first is grounded in

behaviorism and the second is grounded in constructivism.

Behaviorism in action: a remedy for stuttering Mark Onslow and his colleagues have

described a way to help young children overcome stuttering, a problem in which sounds and words

are repeated or stretched unduly, so that fluent conversation is difficult (2001). Onslow’s research

strategy was simple, at least in principle: he trained parents of children who stuttered to praise

their child more strongly is the child spoke fluently (without any stutter), and to correct the child

quietly, but non-punitively whenever the child did stutter. A fluent sentence therefore produced

praise, or even a gold star, from parents. A stuttered sentence produced an immediate sentence like

“I think that was a stutter”, stated factually and quietly. Value judgments and criticisms were not

allowed.

Onslow’s program contradicted the conventional advice to parents about stuttering, which was to

ignore it wherever possible. Nonetheless the program produced very positive results. All of the

stuttering children reduced or even eliminated their stuttering after a few weeks of the differential

reinforcement by their parents, and the stuttering did not return when they were tested even one

year after the program finished.

Constructivism in action: project-based learning Juliette Goldman, an educator working in

Australia, demonstrated how this can be done with health education for middle years students

(Goldman, 2006). She designed a project for seventh-grade students in which they had to publish a

training manual for fellow-students on the topic of “good food handling”, advice for restaurant

workers about how they can keep for contaminating either themselves or the food that they serve.

The writers of the manual worked in groups of three, researching information on a range of topics

related to food handling. Then they used computer self-publishing software to prepare and print

copies of their information. They also made oral presentations about their manuals to a school

assembly to which local food-industry representatives were invited, and they arranged to display

the finished manuals at the local public library.

The initiative used constructivist principles in a number of ways. For example, it challenged

learners to make decisions about what their particular manual should “teach”. The decision-

making required learners constantly to monitor their own knowledge and learning—engage in

322

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

metacognition—in order to insure that the content was complete, accurate, and important to learn.

It also grouped students into teams, so that they could, to some extent, teach each other whatever

they needed to learn, including helping each other to sense whether they actually were learning

from their research.

Questions

➢ Obviously these two studies are about different educational problems or issues. What if the

learning theories underlying them were switched? Could a stuttering program be built

around constructivist principles of learning, and a health education program be built

around behaviorist principles? What would each program look like?

➢ Be a skeptic for a moment. What do you suspect might be the hardest part of

implementing behavioral conditioning for stuttering described by Onslow? And what

might be hardest part of implementing the constructivist program about health education?

References

Onslow, M., Menzies, R., & Packman, A. (2001). An operant intervention for early stuttering. Behavior

modification 25(1), 116-139.

Goldman, J. (2006). Web-based designed activities for young people in health education: A constructivist

approach. Health Education Journal 65(1), 14-27.

Educational Psychology 323 A Global Text

Appendix B: Deciding for yourself about the research

Deciding for yourself about the research

Chapter 3, Student development: schools and the obesity epidemic

Childhood obesity leads not just to teasing by peers, but eventually also to a variety of serious

medical problems, ranging from back pain to heart disease and diabetes. That is why medical

experts are quite concerned that obesity in children has increased markedly since the 1950s, to the

point of being considered a genuine health “epidemic” (Ogden, et al., 2002). Recent projections

suggest that fully one-third of all children born in 2000 will eventually develop diabetes as a side-

effect of being overweight (Narayan, et al., 2003).

Why have these changes happened? One factor is probably the vast increase in individuals’

consumption of sugar especially “disguised” forms like corn syrup (Bawa, 2005). Another is a more

sedentary, “stay-inside” lifestyle than in the past. The latter has happened, among other reasons,

because of population shifts: cities and towns have increased in population and size, while rural

areas have decreased in population, causing more people to rely on car travel more than ever

before. The changes have also led residents in many areas of many cities to consider their

neighborhoods less safe than in the past, causing them to respond with inactivity: either they stay

inside more or they rely even more on cars to get around.

Even schools have contributed to the trend toward obesity. “Junk food” drinks and snacks are

widely available in many schools through commercial vending machines—each one giving a

student an extra 150 and 300 unneeded calories per day. More insidiously, perhaps, is the effect of

the trend toward high-stakes testing: because of them, many schools tend to strengthen courses

and special programs that prepare students in the “basic” subjects that they know will be tested,

and to trim programs (like physical education, but also the arts) that will not be tested.

These considerations led a group of physicians at the University of Georgia to organize an after-

school program of physical activity for elementary school students (Yin, et al., 2005), and to assess

whether the program actually helped prevent weight gain in students. Students volunteered for a

program that had three components: 30 minutes of physical activity, a healthy snack, and

assistance with the students’ homework. (The researchers explain that assistance with homework

had nothing to do with weight loss, but was very important in getting students to attend and

getting parents to support the program!) Staff for the program included a mix of regular teachers

from the school (not necessarily physical education specialists) and other individuals hired

specifically for the project. Physical activities were chosen in part by the students, and were

generally non-competitive and cooperative in nature.

Initial results of the program have been very encouraging; students and parents support the

program strongly, and teachers have been successful in making sure that students are actually

324

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

active enough during the program sessions. Since the program is still ongoing, however, it will be a

few more years before there will be definitive results about weight gain, or lack thereof.

Questions

➢ It is hard to disagree with the purposes of this study—reducing the prevalence of obesity.

But does it really show what it claims? Be a deliberate skeptic for a moment and ask

yourself these questions:

➢ If the students were volunteers, how typical do you think they are of all students?

➢ And if the teachers are receiving a large research grant to implement the program, might

they be working harder to do a good job than most of us ordinarily work?

➢ An important ambiguity about the program was the fact that it included both physical

activity and homework assistance. Think about this ambiguity.

➢ If the researchers split up these two elements—offering only one or the other at any one

school—how might the split affect the outcomes?

➢ Would different sorts of students volunteer as a result of the split, and how if at all would

their selection matter?

References

Ogden, C., Flega, K., Carroll, M. & Johnson, C. (2002). Prevalence and trends in overweight among U.S.

children and adolescents, 1999-2000. Journal of the American Medical Assocation, 288(14), 1728-1732.

Narayan, K., Boyle, J., Thompson, T., Sorensen, S., & Williamson, D. (2003). Lifetime risk for diabetes

mellitus in the United States. Journal of the American Medical Association, 290(14), 1884-1890.

Bawa, S. (2005). The role of the consumption of beverages in the obesity epidemic. Journal of the Royal

Society for the Promotion of Health, 125(3), 124-128.Yin, Z., Hanes, J., Moore, J., Humbles, P., Barbeau,

& Gutin, B. (2005). An after-school physical activity program for obesity prevention in children.

Evaluation and the Health Professions, 28(1), 67-89.

Educational Psychology 325 A Global Text

Appendix B: Deciding for yourself about the research

Deciding for yourself about the research

Chapter 4, Student diversity: using African-American English to enrich classroom discourse

In addition to speaking a language other than English at home, many students learn another

version or dialect of English. A dialect is a version of a language with somewhat unique vocabulary,

grammar, and pronunciation. The most prominent dialect of English in North America is African-

American English, sometimes also called Ebonics. Intellectually and emotionally, Ebonics is just as

rich and capable of expressiveness as “Standard English,” the dialect usually used, for example, by

radio and television news broadcasters. It is used by many African Americans in the United States,

though not by all and often not in every possible situation.

But Ebonics has distinctive features not shared with Standard English. In grammar, for example,

the verb to be is used differently than in Standard English. Instead of simply indicating existence or

non-existence, to be can also distinguish between a one-time event and an ongoing, continuous

state. Consider these two sentences and their meanings:

(1) He tired. (“He is tired right now.)

(2) He be tired. (“He is often or always tired.”)

Ebonics also has features of language use or communication, just like a “foreign” language, which

are different from Standard English. One is the use of repetitive, rhythmic phrases for emphasis—

not unlike the style of an enthusiastic “preacher” in church. Another is the use of call-and-

response, in which an individual asks a question or makes a statement to which the group expects

to respond in unison.

What is interesting and important about the features of language use is that teachers can use them

to communicate more effectively with students, often even if they themselves have not personally

learned to speak African-American dialect. Anita Bohn (2003) illustrated this principle by carefully

observing the teaching styles of two teachers who regularly incorporated the dialect into

conversations with students and who used these conversations as a bridge for students to learn

Standard English.

In one observation, for example, the class was beginning a writing activity and the teacher said,

“This morning we are going to practice some sentences, and when we do that I want you to listen.

Can you say that?” The class responded in chorus, “Listen!” The teacher said, “Do what?” The class

replied even louder, “Listen!” The teacher repeated the work a few times together with the

students: “Listen…listen…” Then she began clapping in between words: “Listen… [clap]…listen…

[clap]…listen… [clap]…” Suddenly she stopped, leaned forward and asked the class, “How you

gonna listen? With your feet?” All responded loudly: “Nooo!” After a pause, she asked again, “You

326

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

gonna listen with your nose?” All responded again: “Nooo!” She asked, “How?” Everyone

responded loudly together, “With our ears!”

The teacher’s approach used both repetitive, rhythmic language and a call-and-response style with

which, as it happened, many of her students were already familiar. By using these features of

African-American communication, she gained students’ attention effectively, but also used the

style of communication to support an activity embedded in Standard English, writing sentences. In

addition to being a familiar style of interaction, however, the technique worked for another reason:

it implied respect for the language and communication skills that students had acquired already.

Such respect has been shown to be important for success not only when students are learning two

dialects, but also when they are learning two languages, such as Spanish and English (Marinez-

Roldan & Malave, 2004).

Questions

➢ Most teachers agree that part of teachers’ goals should be to encourage students in

learning Standard English, both spoken and written. But an issue that we may disagree

about, and that is raised by Bohn’s study, is whether teachers should do so by using non-

standard dialect in class. You could think about it this way: by “speaking Ebonics”, is a

teacher modeling inappropriate English or is he/she providing students with a bridge from

Ebonics to standard English? What do you think about this?

➢ There is also a related question that is more practical. Is using non-standard dialect really

practical for every teacher? Presumably using it is easier for those with prior experience

speaking the dialect, than for those without experience. But could other teachers learn it

well enough to be effective with students? If not, then how else, if at all, could such

teachers communicate with students effectively?

References

Bohn, A. (2003). Familiar voices: Using Ebonics communication techniques in the primary classroom.

Urban Education, 38(6), 688-707.

Martinez-Roldan, C. & Malave, G. (2004). Language ideologies mediating literacy and identity in bilingual

contexts. Journal of early childhood literacy, 4(2), 155-180.

Educational Psychology 327 A Global Text

Appendix B: Deciding for yourself about the research

Deciding for yourself about the research

Chapter 5, Students with special educational needs: How well does inclusion occur in high schools?

The Individuals with Disabilities Education Act applies to all levels of schooling, from kindergarten

through twelfth grade, and one of its revisions (Public Law 101-336, 104 Stat. 327, 1990)

further addressed the needs of secondary schools by including provisions for school-to-work

transitions for students with disabilities. Yet progress at including such students has generally been

more rapid and complete in elementary schools—especially at the youngest grade levels—than in

secondary schools. The reasons for the difference do not necessarily have to do high school

teachers’ attitudes about disabilities as compared to elementary teachers’ attitudes. Much of it

stems from differences in how the two levels of schooling are structured, with secondary schools

being much larger and organized by a complex timetable of classes that tends to sort students—and

even teachers themselves!—by academic background (Kelly, 2004; Oakes, 2005). One effect of this

organization is to make it harder for special education and general education teachers to

collaborate, and therefore to integrate learning experiences for students with disabilities into high

school as a whole.

Yet some teachers and schools manage to collaborate anyway. A research study by Joseph

Stowitschek and his colleagues explored the factors that account for comparative success at

including students with disabilities in secondary school (Stowitschek, Lovitt, & Rodriguez, 2001).

The researchers were interested, first, in how much teachers actually do collaborate to design and

carry out programs for youth with disabilities, and second, in what specific circumstances or

practices were associated with collaborating successfully. They chose three contrasting high

schools to study in detail: a large urban public high school, a rural public high school in a small

town, and a private urban high school. For each school they collected information from a wide

range of staff—special education teachers, general education teachers, administrators, parents, and

students with disabilities themselves. The information came from surveys, interviews, reviews of

official school documents, and observations of classrooms.

What did they find? Among other things, they found that special education teachers at all of the

schools strongly supported inclusion of students with disabilities to the fullest possible extent; they

did not, that is, seek to strengthen or increase the schools’ reliance on segregated special education

classes. They also found significant interest and support from parents of the students with

disabilities in the educational programs of their children. These factors suggested that change

toward fuller inclusion may continue in the years ahead.

But they also found limitations on how much the teachers could collaborate at any of the schools. A

major problem was the teachers’ dependency on informal communication with general education

328

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

teachers. Instead of regularly announced meetings to discuss inclusion initiatives, teachers had to

“catch” each other in the hallway or during lunch hours, for example, in order to have

conversations about students and ways of including them in class or school activities. At these

moments the teachers tended already to be busy. A partial result was that the general education

teachers ended up with limited knowledge both about the special education program at their

school, and about why particular students might be placed successfully in their particular

classrooms. All of the students with disabilities had IEPs, but the general teachers had little or no

knowledge of their contents—or even of their existence. Not surprisingly, under these conditions

there were few major collaborative activities, such as the co-teaching of a course by a special

education teacher and a general teacher or jointly operated activities or programs.

Yet for each school there were also individual teachers and activities that boosted collaboration in

the school, and that could in principle be tried elsewhere as well. The private high school, for

example, had an especially effective, vital program for involving parents: there were regular

advisory group meetings to assess the current needs of the special education program and to

develop and sustain support for it among the parents. Another especially effective collaboration

involved peer tutoring—using high school students to tutor the students with disabilities on a

regular basis, often with course credit given as “payment” to the tutors. Peer tutoring proved a

good way to communicate the nature and extent of the special education program to the student

population as a whole. A third effective form of collaboration involved using a teacher as a

“community coordinator”, someone who developed linkages to agencies and potential employers in

the community. The linkages proved especially helpful in students’ transitions to work and life

after high school.

All in all, there were limitations on inclusion in the secondary schools, but also grounds for

optimism because of the collaborative successes and the dedication of the teachers. Although

Stowitschek and his colleagues focused on only three schools, their findings suggested three key

points: (1) that the motivation for inclusion and collaboration definitely exists among secondary

teachers, (2) that it is possible to work around the organizational constraints of high schools, and

(3) that changes in those constraints in the future should further increase levels of inclusion and

collaboration.

Questions

➢ If you were a teacher in a high school (as many readers of this book plan to become), how

would you prepare your students to receive a student with a disability into one of your

classes? Consider actions that you would take both before and after the student actually

arrives.

Educational Psychology 329 A Global Text

Appendix B: Deciding for yourself about the research

➢ Sometimes teachers at every grade level express concern about receiving students with

disabilities into their classes, even if it can be arranged easily. Why do you think that the

teachers feel this way? Think of three possible objections to inclusion, and then think of

how an advocate for inclusion might respond to each of them.

References

Public Law 101-336, 104 Stat. 327 (July 26, 1990). Americans with Disabilities Act of 1990. Washington,

D.C.: United States Government Printing Office.

Stowitschek, J., Lovitt, T., & Rodriguez, J. (2001). Patterns of collaboration in secondary education for youth

with special needs: Profiles of three high schools. Urban Education, 36(1), 93-128.

Kelly, S. (2004). Are teachers tracked? On what basis and with what consequences. Social psychology in

education, 7(1), 55-72.

Oakes, J. (2005). Keeping track: How schools structure inequality, 2nd edition. New Haven, CT: Yale

University Press.

330

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Deciding for yourself about the research

Chapter 6, Student motivation: Is self-efficacy culturally biased?

As we explain in this chapter, self-efficacy beliefs are based heavily on experiences—mastery,

vicarious (or observed) mastery, and social persuasion. Research has found that these experiences

are effective in a wide variety of situations, such as making decisions about careers, performing

tasks at work, choosing courses at school, deciding whether to join after-school sports teams, and

planning effective instruction as a teacher (Allison, Dwyer, & Makin, 1999; Bandura, 1997;

Goddard, Hoy, & Hoy, 2004). Because it has proved valid in so many situations, self-efficacy

seems relatively universal cognitive process—as if it “works” everywhere, for everyone.

But does it? The very fact that self-efficacy is based on experience should make us suspicious of its

limits, since there are few experiences that are literally shared by all people in all places or

societies. And the wide diversity among students in most schools should lead to similar skepticism.

Maybe it is true that self-efficacy promotes motivation for many students, or even for most, but

does it do so for all students? And if it does not, then what are the reasons?

These questions prompted a psychologist named Lori Lindley to investigate whether self-efficacy

has in fact proved useful and valid for understanding motivation in unusually diverse populations

(Lindley, 2006). She searched the research literature for studies about self-efficacy in each of the

following groups:

(a) women with careers

(b) ethnic minorities living in the United States

(c) societies and cultures outside the United States

(d) self-identified gay, lesbian, and bisexual individuals

(e) people with disabilities

What has research shown about the self-efficacy of members of these groups? Compared to the

“classic” research about this concept, is self-efficacy higher, lower, confined to just limited areas of

activity, or not even a meaningful idea?

What Lindley found was that self-efficacy beliefs were (like the people she studied) complex and

varied. Women, for example, were just as likely to express high self-efficacy and low motivation

about using computers, as to express low self-efficacy and high motivation to use them. Among

ethnic minority students (Hispanics, Asian-Americans, and African Americans), some studies

found lower self-efficacy about choosing careers than among white students. But other studies

found no differences.

Educational Psychology 331 A Global Text

Appendix B: Deciding for yourself about the research

Among societies outside the United States, however, high self-efficacy seemed to predict

motivation, much as it does in the “classic” research with white American populations—though

again with some differences. Among Italian high school students, for example, self-efficacy beliefs

about career choice are strongly associated with the students’ interest in and choice of career (Lent,

et al., 2003). Yet the connection between self-efficacy and motivation was found to have a different

character for students from Taiwan (Mau, 2000): for them high self-efficacy was associated not

with being highly motivated, but with relying heavily on others to assist with making decisions and

with being highly rational or logical in making them. Self-efficacy, under these conditions, was not

so much a belief in yourself as a belief in your community.

Among the remaining groups—the gay/lesbian/bisexual individuals and the people with

disabilities—research was especially scarce and conclusions were therefore hard to reach. The

publications that did exist emphasized a belief in the potential value of self-efficacy for these

groups, but they did not report research studies describing whether in fact self-efficacy in fact

motivated the individuals, or even existed consistently and meaningfully as a concept or belief.

What does the diversity of these findings suggest (beyond Grandmother’s rule that “sometimes one

thing happens, and sometimes another”)? Lindley noted two points, both of which were hinted at

by some of the studies that she reviewed. The first point is that self-efficacy may be a belief about

personal capacity only for some individuals in some situations. For others, efficacy may really be a

belief in the group or community, such as your family, classroom, or workplace. Self-efficacy may

really be collective self-efficacy—a belief that your group can accomplish its goals. Believing

primarily in the group may be quite motivating, but also be quite a different experience from

believing primarily in yourself. In recent years some psychologists and educators have

acknowledged this possibility and begun studying the dynamics of collective self-efficacy (Bandura,

1997; Gordon, Hoy, & Hoy, 2004).

Lindley’s second point is that for some groups, the main barriers to success are not beliefs in

personal capacity, but real, external obstacles independent of personal beliefs. Imagine, for

example, that a person encounters daily, real social prejudice because he or she is non-white,

homosexual, or has a disability. For that person, self-confidence may only go part of the way to

insuring success, and removing the real social barriers may be needed to go the rest of the way.

For teachers, three implications of this research seem clear. First, individual self-efficacy beliefs do

often motivate students, and teachers should therefore encourage them in these students. Second,

some students may see their personal capacity in terms of the capacity of groups to which they

belong. Teachers can motivate these students by strengthening the capacity of their groups—

perhaps using strategies like the ones described in this chapter and the next. Third, some students

rightly perceive genuine injustices in their world which limit their chances of success; teachers

should not deny the importance of these injustices, but recognize them and do what they can to

reduce them.

332

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Questions

➢ Think about your own belief in your capacity to teach well, as well as your doubts and

worries about your capacity. You might call this your teaching self-efficacy. What is

that belief primarily based on, and what would it take to raise that belief even higher?

➢ Suppose your students consisted of about equal numbers who believed in individual self-

efficacy and in collective self-efficacy. Imagine and describe one advantage of having such

a mix in your class, as well as one potential source of conflict that you might have to deal

with between the two groups.

References

Allison, K., Dwyer, J., & Makin, S. (1999). Self-efficacy and participation in vigorous physical activity by high

school students. Health Education and Behavior, 26(1), 12-24.

Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: Freeman.

Goddard, R., Hoy, W., & Hoy, A. (2004). Collective efficacy beliefs: Theoretical developments, empirical

evidence, and future directions. Educational Researcher, 33(3), 3-13.

Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press.

Lent, R., Brown, S., Nota, L., & Soresi, S. (2003). Teaching social cognitive interest and choice hypotheses

across Holland types in Italian high school students. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 62, 101-118.

Mau, W.-C. (2003). Factors that influence persistence in science and engineering career aspirations. Career

Development Quarterly, 51, 234-243

Educational Psychology 333 A Global Text

Appendix B: Deciding for yourself about the research

Deciding for yourself about the research:

Chapter 7, Classroom management and the learning environment: Culturally responsive classroom management

Even though teachers might believe that consistency is a mark of good classroom management, it is

not always true that “one size fits all” when it comes to matching particular management strategies

to specific students. A lot of research suggests, on the contrary, that success in classroom

management needs to be adjusted to the cultural background and expectations of students.

Educators sometimes call this approach culturally responsive classroom management.

Support for culturally responsive management comes a number of research studies. In one study,

for example, David Brown interviewed 13 teachers who taught in classrooms from 1st through 12th

grade in urban, inner-city schools in several different cities (Brown, 2004). A few of the teachers

were themselves culturally diverse—one was from Sri Lanka, one was African American, two were

Hispanic American—but most were white. Although the interviews were lengthy and detailed, they

centered on just three underlying questions:

(1) How do you interact with students?

(2) How would you describe your management style?

(3) What works well for you in communicating with students?

The teachers’ views could be summarized in several points. First, the teachers considered it

important to show students that the teachers cared and respected them: this attitude was

significantly more important, they felt, when teaching urban students than when teaching

suburban students. The teachers therefore made more effort than usual to be friendly with all

students and to get to know them as individuals. To do so, though, the teachers also had to know

how the students themselves preferred to be publicly known or acknowledged. In some classrooms

with Hispanic American students, for example, the teachers found it wise not to call on individuals

during class, because some of the students preferred to be recognized for their relationships with

classmates—for their membership in the class as a group—rather than for their distinctiveness

from the group.

Interestingly, “caring” involved more than simple warmth and nurturance. It also meant teachers’

asserting their authority to make clear demands on students both for high quality work and for

appropriate classroom behavior. Asserting authority indirectly in order to sound polite (like saying

“Would you like to sit down now?”) was less effective and was often interpreted by students as a

sign of a teacher’s indifference. Direct, specific commands (like “Take your seat”) were more

effective, provided they did not also express hostility.

334

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

The teachers’ caring had to be consistent in every way: not only did they have to say friendly things

to students, but also they had to look friendly with eye contact and smiles. What made such

consistency initially challenging for some teachers was recognizing students’ own signs of

friendliness for what they were. In some classrooms with African American students, for example,

students engaged in a “call response” pattern of interaction: as the teacher gave instructions or

explained an idea, some students would say or speak their own feelings or mention their own ideas.

The pattern was not meant to interrupt the teacher, however, so much as to show involvement in

the lesson or activity, and the teacher needed to acknowledge it as such.

Other educational researchers besides David Brown have found similar results, though some point

out that actually practicing culturally responsive management can be harder than simply knowing

what it involves (Weinstein, Tomlinson-Clarke, & Curran, 2004). To become skillful with the

strategies described by Brown and others, for example, teachers also need to look honestly at their

own preconceptions about ethnic, cultural and racial differences, so that they do not misconstrue

culturally ambiguous behaviors of students just because students have a background different from

the teacher’s own. Teachers also need to be aware of how much society-wide prejudice on students’

sense of efficacy, since pervasive prejudice and discrimination can stimulate some students to

withdraw in ways that may be mistaken for laziness.

Perhaps the most challenging aspect of culturally responsive management, however, is for teachers

to accommodate to students’ cultural differences while also helping them learn how to function

well in the somewhat bureaucratic, middle-class oriented “culture” of school. This challenge is full

of dilemmas. How much, for example, should a teacher sacrifice conventional “politeness”

behaviors (like using indirect questions) simply because students understand and respect

directness more easily? How much should a teacher encourage students to critique each other’s or

the teacher’s ideas even if students’ families give higher priority to cooperation and compliance

with authorities? And what if a particular class is itself culturally diverse, containing students from

many cultural backgrounds in one room? What should a teacher do then?

Questions

➢ Think about the issue of politeness versus directness mention in the final paragraph above.

Presumably teachers and students need some sort of mutual accommodation about this

issue. If you were the teacher, what would the accommodation look like? Obviously it

might depend on the particular students and on the precise the behavior at hand. But go

beyond this generality. Imagine—and describe—what you might actually say to students to

show respect for their preferred styles of talking while still encouraging them to respect or

even adopt styles of speech that lead to more success in school?

Educational Psychology 335 A Global Text

Appendix B: Deciding for yourself about the research

➢ Culturally responsive classroom management has sometimes been criticized on the

grounds that it encourages teachers to “profile” or stereotype students according to their

ethnic, racial, or cultural backgrounds. Being culturally responsive, it is said, makes

teachers overlook the individual differences among students. Others argue that treating

students strictly as individuals makes teachers overlook students’ obvious cultural

heritages. How might you resolve this issue in your own mind? Again, go beyond the

obvious: imagine an actual conversation that you might have with your own students about

this issue.

References

Brown, D. (2004). Urban teachers’ professed classroom management strategies: Reflections of culturally

responsive teaching. Urban Education, 39(3), 266-289.

Weinstein, C.,Tomlinson-Clarke, S., & Curran, M. (2004). Toward a conception of culturally responsive

classroom management. Journal of Teacher Education, 55(1), 25-38.

336

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Deciding for yourself about research

Chapter 8, Nature of classroom communication: When is a student lying?

Although we might wish that it were not true, students do occasionally tell a deliberate lie to the

teacher. In explaining why an assignment is late, for example, a student might claim to have been

sick when the student was not in fact sick. Worse yet, a student might turn in an assignment that

the student claims to have written when in fact it was “borrowed” from another student or

(especially among older students) even from Internet.

In situations like these, is there any way to discern when a person actually is lying? Many of the

signs would have to be nonverbal, since by definition a liar’s verbal statements may not indicate

that falsehood is occurring. A large body of research has studied this question—looking for

nonverbal signs by which deception might be detected. The research can be summarized like this:

people generally believe that they can tell when someone is lying, but they can not in fact do so very

accurately. In a survey of 75 countries around the world, for example, individuals from every

nation expressed the belief that liars avoid eye contact (Global Deception Research Team, 2006).

(This is an unusually strong trend compared to most in educational and psychological research!)

Individuals also named additional behaviors: liars shift on the feet, for example, they touch and

scratch themselves nervously, and their speech is hesitant or flawed. But the most important belief

is about eye contact: a liar, it is thought, cannot “look you in the eye”.

Unfortunately these beliefs seem to be simply stereotypes that have little basis in fact. Experiments

in which one person deliberately lies to another person find little relationship between averting eye

contact and lying, as well as little relationship between other nonverbal behaviors and lying

(DePaulo et al., 2003). A person who is lying is just as likely to look directly at you as someone

telling the truth—and on the other hand, also just as likely to look away. In fact gaze aversion can

indicate a number of things, depending on the context. In another study of eye contact, for

example, Anjanie McCarthy and her colleagues observed eye contact when one person asks another

person a question. They found that when answering a question to which a person already knew the

answer (like “What is your birthday?”), the person was likely to look the questioner directly in the

eye (McCarthy, et al., 2006). When answering a question which required some thought, however,

the person tended to avert direct gaze. The researchers studied individuals from three societies and

found differences in where the individuals look in order to avoid eye contact: people from Canada

and Trinidad looked up, but people from Japan looked down. All of their answers, remember, were

truthful and none were lies.

If gaze aversion does not really indicate lying, then why do people believe that it does anyway? The

research team that studied this belief suggested that the belief does not actually reflect our

experiences with liars, but instead function as a deterrent to lying behavior (Global Deception

Educational Psychology 337 A Global Text

Appendix B: Deciding for yourself about the research

Research Team, 2006). Since nearly everyone disapproves of lying, and since detecting it is often

difficult, the next best strategy is to persuade potential liars that they might in fact be detected.

Furthermore, if we believe that liars should feel ashamed of their behavior, it is reasonable to

suppose that they would show signs of shame—i.e. gaze aversion, shifting on their feet, hesitation,

and the like. The irony is that if we begin to doubt a person’s truthfulness, a truthful person is more

likely to feel uncomfortable, so the person is likely to begin averting gaze and showing other signs

of nervousness anyway. The end result is to reinforce the stereotype of gaze aversion, but not to

identify an actual liar.

For teachers, the implications of this research are twofold. First, it suggests that we should be very

careful before deciding whether or not a particular student is lying on a particular occasion. We

should encourage students to be equally careful with each other; it is too easy, it seems, to jump to

conclusions about this sort of judgment. Second, it implies that a better way to reduce lying by

students is to develop high-quality relationships with them, so that students will not feel a need to

lie. Obviously, developing high-quality relationships is a big job and it may be easier with some

students than with others. But it appears to be more effective than falsely accusing truthful

students while overlooking actual deceptions.

Questions

➢ Classrooms are like any other social setting in that they can only function well if their

members’ are truthful with each other. How could you communicate this message to

students so that they endorse it themselves? One way, of course, is to discuss the problem

with students. But another way might be to stage “simulated lying” between students, and

have students see how well they can discern true liars. Would a simulation be a good idea,

or would it be undesirable by giving students practice at lying effectively? Explain your

views about this question.

➢ Recall the study above in which thought-questions caused individuals to avert direct gaze.

Why do you suppose that people from Trinidad and Canada averted gaze by looking up,

while people from Japan averted by looking down? What sort of cultural significance does

this difference have, if any? In answering this question, think as well about how you could

find out if the answer is valid.

References

Global Deception Research Team. (2006). A world of lies. Journal of Cross-cultural Psychology, 37(6), 60-

74.

DePaulo, B., Lindsay, J., Malone, B., Muhlenbruck, L., Charlton, K., & Cooper, H. (2003). Cues to deception.

Psychological Bulletin, 129, 74-118.

338

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

McCarthy, A., Lee, K., Itakura, S., & Muir, D. (2006). Cultural display rules drive eye gaze during thinking.

Journal of Cross-cultural Psychology, 37(6), 717-722.

Educational Psychology 339 A Global Text

Appendix B: Deciding for yourself about the research

Deciding for yourself about research

Chapter 9, Facilitating complex thinking: identifying attitude-treatment interactions

As we have stated in various places in this chapter, and as many teachers will confirm from

experience, there seems to be no instructional strategy that is best for all students. Instead a more

guarded comment may be more accurate: there seem to be strategies that are especially good for

certain students under certain conditions. Educational psychologists have long studied this idea

and call it aptitude-treatment interaction (abbreviated ATI) (Cronbach & Snow, 1977; Snow,

1989). The aptitude in this term is the unique quality, talent, or skill of a student; the treatment is

the instructional strategy or approach being used; and the interaction is the combination of the

two.

The idea seems intuitively appealing, but it has proved surprisingly difficult to identify particular

ATIs scientifically. Part of the problem is the ambiguity of the term aptitude. Numerous qualities,

talents, and skills of students have been identified and studied, including memory for verbal

material, memory for visual material, memory for sequences of ideas, ability to analyze a problem

into its parts, and creativity .

The situation is just as ambiguous in defining treatment. Is it a specific teacher-directed strategy

such as the use of advance organizers described in this chapter? Or does treatment mean a broad

approach such as Madeline Hunter’s effective teaching model that we describe in this chapter, or

like student-centered inquiry learning that we also describe? Since both key terms have multiple

possible meanings, it is not surprising that research studies of their combinations have also yielded

ambiguous results. Sometimes a particular combination of aptitude and treatment help learning,

but other times it makes little difference.

In spite of these problems with the research as a whole, the specific studies of ATIs have clearly

been helpful to teachers. In one, for example, the researchers investigated human ecology students’

preferred styles of learning—their aptitudes (Crutsinger, Knight, & Kinley, 2005). Did they prefer,

for example, to learn from visual information (pictures, diagrams) or from verbal information (text

and oral explanations)? Did they prefer to scan new information in sequence, or to skip around in

it and piece it together at the end? The researchers found that this particular group of students

tended to prefer new information to be visual and sequential. As a result, they were able to improve

students’ learning by adding to the course more computer-based instruction, which was relatively

visual and sequential in its organization.

In another study, the researcher who initially was studying cooperative learning groups in

university students discovered—and wondered why—some of the groups were more productive

than others (Peterson, 2004). On closer investigation of the groups he found an ATI-related

problem. Students in this particular university course were choosing their own group partners.

340

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

They therefore tended to choose their own friends, a practice that inadvertently reduced the talents

and resources available in some groups. Friends, it seemed, tended to duplicate each other’s styles

of problem solving and of performing academic tasks, rendering the group as a whole less rich in

talents and therefore less productive or successful.

To remedy this problem, the instructor undertook to identify students’ strong points in different

aspects of problem solving. He identified which students were inclined to take action, which were

good at decision-making, which at identifying problems, and which at brainstorming. Then he

assigned students to groups so that each group had one person strong in each of these areas. The

results were a striking increase in the productivity of all groups. But there was a catch: although the

students were indeed more productive, they did not like being assigned partners as well as

choosing their own! Maintaining this particular ATI may therefore prove difficult over the long

term—perhaps another reason by ATI research has not always found consistent results.

Questions

➢ Think about the fact that results of ATI have been inconsistent, even though it seems

reasonable given the obvious diversity among students in every classroom. Assuming that

you support the idea of ATIs, explain how you would justify it to two kinds of people: (1) a

fellow teacher in your school, and (2) a professor of educational psychology.

➢ Given the results of Peterson’s research study, what is the best advice you could give to

teachers (or to yourself) about how to set up cooperative learning groups? Should students

choose their own partners, or should the teacher choose them? Keep in mind the proviso

mentioned at the end—that the students preferred to choose their own partners, even

though it meant learning less.

References

Cronbach, L. & Snow, R. (1977). Aptitudes and instructional methods: A handbook for research on

interaction. New York: Irvington.

Crutsinger, C., Knight, D., & Kinley. (2005). Learning style preferences: Implications for Web-based

instruction. Clothing and Textiles Research Journal, 23(4), 266-276.

Snow, R. (1989). Aptitude-treatment interaction as a framework for research on individual differences in

learning. In P. Ackerman, R. Sternberg, & R. Glaser (Eds.), Learning and individual differences, pp. 13-

60. New York: W. H. Freeman.

Peterson, T. (2004). So you’re thinking of trying problem-based learning?: Three critical success factors for

implementation. Journal of Management Education, 28(5), 630-647.

Educational Psychology 341 A Global Text

Appendix B: Deciding for yourself about the research

Deciding for yourself about the research

Chapter 10, Planning instruction: How does multicultural curriculum affect racial knowledge and biases?

Multicultural and anti-racist curricula work partly by portraying and discussing individuals of

diverse racial or ethnic background in ways that counteract stereotypes. Students read stories,

watch videos, and talk about respected citizens—doctors, political leaders, celebrities, and the like

—who happen to be African-American, Hispanic, or of some other non-Caucasian origin. In some

cases, especially at the early childhood level, students’ interests and concerns are used to guide the

selection and integration of diversity-related activities (Derman-Sparks, 1994).

One way of thinking about such a curriculum is that it tries to make students into “experts,” even at

relatively young ages, about racial and ethnic differences. Instead of thinking about diversity in

superficial terms—as based merely on skin color, for example—students learn to see diversity as

complex and multi-faceted. An African-American child and a White child do not simply differ in

color, for example; they are both similar and different in many ways. Hopefully the greater subtlety

of their expert knowledge also reduces negative biases felt about race.

To test these possibilities, Donna Perkins and Carolyn Mebert interviewed 79 children at six

preschool and after-school child care centers (2005). Some of the centers emphasized multicultural

education, some emphasized multicultural education as well as an emergent curriculum, and some

emphasized neither. Perkins and Mebert assessed children’s knowledge and attitudes about race in

several ways. For example, they displayed pictures of other children over various races on a felt

board, and asked the participating children to arrange the pictures so that children were closer

together if more similar and farther apart if more different. They also asked participating children

to evaluate simple stories or anecdotes about three pictures, one of a white child, one of an African-

American child, and one of an Asian-American child. In one of the anecdotes, for example, the

researcher asked, “Some children are naughty because they draw with crayons on the walls. Which

of these children (in the pictures) might do that?” The participating child could then choose any or

all of the pictured children—or choose none at all.

What did Perkins and Mebert find from this study? Four ideas stood out especially clearly:

(1) Children indeed showed more “expertise” about race if they attended a child care center that

emphasized multicultural education—but only if they center also emphasized emergent

curriculum. To be effective, in other words, information about human diversity had to grow out of

children’s personal concerns and interests. It was not enough simply to tell them about human

diversity.

342

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

(2) Although a multicultural/emergent program was effective in sensitizing children to

differences between races, it was not especially effective for sensitizing them to differences within

races. When it came to differences among African-Americans, for example, the

multicultural/emergent children were no more subtle or “expert” in their judgments than any other

children.

(3) Children in the multicultural programs tended to view all children, regardless of race, in a

relatively more positive light, and this tendency increased as they got older (i.e. from age 4 to 6).

(4) Most important of all, the program orientation did affect the children’s knowledge and

attitudes, even at (or perhaps because of) their young age.

Questions

➢ Why do you suppose that multicultural education worked only in conjunction with an

emergent curriculum? Imagine that you writing a brief summary of this study for a school

newsletter, and that you need to comment on this question. What would you say about it?

➢ Skeptics might say that the study assessed only what children say about race, not how

children might act in racially related situations. In an interview, in other words, a child

might express positive sentiments about every race or ethnic group, but still behave in

prejudiced ways during play or other activities at school. Is this a legitimate criticism of the

Perkins and Mebert study? How could you devise another study to test whether there is

truth to the criticism?

References

Perkins, D. & Mebert, C. (2005). Efficacy of multicultural education for preschool children. Journal of Cross-

Cultural Psychology, 36(4), 497-512.

Derman-Sparks, L. (1994). Empowering children to create a caring culture in a world of differences.

Childhood Education, 70, 66-71.

Educational Psychology 343 A Global Text

Appendix B: Deciding for yourself about the research

Deciding for yourself about the research

Chapter 11, Teacher-made assessment strategies: the importance of establishing trust when giving critical feedback across the racial divide

Providing accurate but constructive feedback to students is difficult for teachers. Identifying

problems in student work and pointing out areas for improvement can undermine students’

confidence and motivation. However, students cannot make significant improvements in their

work if they do not get accurate information about their strengths and weaknesses. Cohen, Steele

and Ross (2002) argue that trust is crucial to this dilemma: students are more likely to respond

well to accurate feedback if they trust the teacher and believe that the feedback is not biased.

However, if the student distrusts the teacher feedback that points out weaknesses is likely to lower

motivation and confidence.

The dilemma of providing trust and accurate feedback that enhances motivation is particularly

acute when the students come from a group that has been stereotyped as less competent (e.g.

African American, Latino) and the teacher is white. Several studies suggest that feedback that is

“wise” can help establish trust and foster motivation even though the feedback includes

information about weaknesses. In one study, 45 African American and 48 White College students

were asked by a White experimenter to write a letter of commendation for their favorite teacher

(Cohen Steele, Ross, 1999). The students were told that the best letters would be published in a

journal and that the skills needed were similar to those needed to write an effective college paper. A

photo of each student was attached to their draft letter. A week later the students returned for the

second session of the study and were given one of three types of feedback:

(1) Unbuffered criticism: Spelling and grammatical errors were marked as well as some

shortcomings in the writing (e.g. stylistic concerns). Also two check marks acknowledging good

points were included.

(2) Criticism and positive buffer: In addition to the criticism described for the unbuffered group,

students were told that they did a good job and made a number of good points.

(3) Wise feedback: Criticism, positive buffer, and assurance. In addition to the criticism and

positive buffer described above, students were also told that the person critiquing the letter

believed that the student could meet the high standards needed for publication.

The researchers assessed how biased the students believed the reviewer was, how motivated

students were to revise the letter, and how much students identified with writing skills. All

students were less motivated and identified less with writing if they received unbuffered criticism.

However, for White students the distinction between criticism with positive buffer vs the wise

feedback was not as important as it was for African American students. The group of African

American students who received wise feedback was more motivated, identified more with writing,

344

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

and was significantly less likely to believe the reviewer was biased than the groups that received the

other forms of criticism.

This study suggests that the kind of feedback given is important: unbuffered criticism is

associated with lower motivation for all students. However, for African American students who

grow up amidst negative stereotypes about their competence, feedback that promotes motivation,

needs to include three components: some positive comments, criticism that identifies specific

weaknesses, and comments that make it clear the teacher believes the student can do well.

Questions

➢ This study was conducted using College students—do you think the findings would also

apply to elementary, middle, and high school students?

➢ This study focused on African American students. Do you think the findings might also

apply to Latino and Native American students who are also often stereotyped as less

competent?

➢ How important is trust in classroom interactions?

➢ Have you received the kinds of feedback described here? Did it influence your motivation?

References

Cohen, G., Steele, C., & Ross, L. (1999). The mentor's dilemma: Providing critical feedback across the racial

divide. Personality and social psychology bulletin, 25(10), 1302-1318.

Cohen, G., Steele, C., & Ross, L. (2002). A barrier of mistrust: How negative stereotypes affect cross-race

mentoring. In Aronson, J. M. & Aronson, J. (Eds.), Improving academic achievement: Impact of

psychological factors in education, 305-331. Emerald Group Publishing.

Educational Psychology 345 A Global Text

Appendix B: Deciding for yourself about the research

Deciding for yourself about the research

Chapter 12, Standardized assessment strategies: Why are standardized tests so important to NCLB?

The use of standardized testing in NCLB arises from reforms that were initiated in the 1980s.

These reforms were heavily influenced by business leaders who were concerned with the rising

productivity of international competitors and believed that improving education would aid USA

competitiveness. Corporate leaders who had orchestrated company turnarounds stressed the

importance of setting explicit goals, performance or outputs, use of benchmarks or standards, and

organizational restructuring. Policy makers needed support from the business leaders for the

additional financial resources needed for widespread education reforms they wished so it is not

surprising they adopted these business ideas as they devised ways to make teachers and students

more accountable.

According to Susan Fuhrman (2004) a “theory of action” underlies these new accountability

systems that contains the following assumptions:

(a) Schools’ primary focus should be student achievement in the key areas of math, reading and

science. Clear content standards developed by each State for each subject area and grade level help

schools in this focus. If rewards and sanctions are based on the basis of students’ meeting the

content standards in that school then the teachers and administrators will devote energy and

resources to improving student achievement.

(b) Standardized tests that are aligned with the content standards can accurately and

authentically measure student performance. Well designed tests are reliable and valid and so other

measures such as classroom observations are not needed to determine if teachers and schools are

doing a good job.

(c) Meaningful consequences will not only motivate teachers, students, and administrators but

also improve instruction. Positive consequences include bonuses for teachers and administrators

and negative consequences include denial of graduation or promotion for students, or school take

over and restructuring. Because these consequences are real, teachers will work harder to teach

and be more likely to additional professional development to improve their skills. Students will

also work harder to learn so teacher-student interactions around content will improve. Frequent

assessment will provide meaningful feedback on student performance which in turn will promote

improved teaching.

(d) There will be minimal unintended consequences if the systems work as intended. For

example, instruction will improve rather then becoming narrowly focused on test taking skills, and

high school graduation tests will promote learning not increase drop out rates.

346

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Questions

Our questions focus on part of each assumption.

➢ How much emphasis should schools place on reading, mathematics and science? What

role should art, physical education, social studies, and music play in school classrooms?

➢ Do standardized tests measure students’ performance on content standards adequately?

Should schools be judged on students’ scores on standardized tests? Is it important that

classroom observations of students (by teachers or others) are not included.

➢ Will students and teachers be motivated by the tests. Stiggins (2004) argued that while

high achieving students may be motivated by tests many students who find the tests

difficult will give up and so be less motivated. Do you agree with Stiggins or the

assumption underlying NCLB?

➢ Are the unintended consequences minimal? Is classroom instruction improving or

becoming narrowly focused on test taking skills and content?

References

Fuhrman, S. H. (2004). Introduction, In S. H. Fuhrman & R. F. Elmore (Eds). Redesigning accountability

systems for education. (pp. 3-14). New York: Teachers College Press.

Educational Psychology 347 A Global Text

Appendix B: Deciding for yourself about the research

Deciding for yourself about research

Appendix C, The reflective practitioner: action research as a way to deal with the isolation of teaching

Observers of education have sometimes noted that classroom teachers tend to be isolated from

each other by the very nature of their work (Lortie, 1975; Zeichner, 2007). A teacher may be

constantly surrounded by students, but chances are that no colleague will be there to witness what

the teacher does in class. Conversation about classroom experiences do happen, but they tend to

happen outside of class time—perhaps over lunch, or before or after school. This circumstance does

not prevent teachers’ from sharing experiences or concerns related to teaching altogether, but

delaying conversations probably makes them less frequent or likely. Fewer collegial conversations,

in turn, can limit teachers by reducing their opportunities to learn from each other—or even to

realize many of the instructional options open to them.

Action research addresses teachers’ isolation because it promotes not only reflection on practice,

but also collaboration and sharing (Hayes, 2006). The benefits of sharing may be the most obvious

when an action research project is actually published for a wider audience. Over the past 20 years,

numerous teachers and other educators have published studies of their own teaching or their own

students’ learning. There are now entire books compiling such accounts (for example, Samaras &

Freese, 2006; Tidwell & Fitzgerald, 2006), a comprehensive handbook discussing aspects of

teachers’ studies of their own teaching (Loughran, et al., 2004), several journals whose purpose is

largely or solely to publish examples of action research (one, for example, is called simply Action

Research), and a variety of blogs and websites that post action research projects. Collectively these

publications are a rich source of practical wisdom from which individual teachers can learn and

think about their own teaching.

But an action research project does not have to published formally in order to promote

collaboration or sharing. The benefits can happen locally—even within a single school building—

whenever a teacher plans, carries out, and talks about a research initiative. A teacher named Betty

Ragland, for example, described how this happened in her highly unusual teaching situation, a

juvenile correctional facility (Ragland, 2006). The facility functioned somewhat like a prison for

youth convicted of various crimes. As you might suppose, Ms Ragland’s students experienced

behavior problems and conflicts more often than usual in schools, to the extent that teachers

sometimes felt physically vulnerable themselves, as well as isolated from help if serious conflicts

developed during class. To deal with these stresses, Ms Ragland initiated a self-study of her

practice in which she wrote and thought about her experiences and her reactions to the

experiences. She shared the results, both in writing and through meetings, with fellow teachers. In

the course of doing so, she developed a number of insights which colleagues found helpful in

formulating their own thinking:

348

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

As Ms Ragland reflected on her work as a teacher, she realized that teaching in a correctional

facility had made her more cautious about her safety even outside of teaching hours. For example,

she had become more careful about locking her car door, where she walked at night, and even

where she sat in restaurants (she preferred to sit with her back to the wall).

Ms Ragland found it impossible to describe her work in a fully detached or objective way, and

finally decided that being detached was not even desirable. Her feelings and interpretations of

students’ behavior were essential to understanding experiences with them, so she decided that it

was better to include these in whatever she wrote about them.

As she wrote, talked, and reflected on her experiences, she found herself governed by two

incompatible perspectives about her work, which she called the educational perspective (try to

help students and turn their lives around) and the correctional perspective (remember that the

students had committed serious crimes and often could not be trusted).

More importantly, she discovered, through conversations with fellow staff, that they too felt torn

between these same two perspectives.

By talking with each other about the dilemmas in how to interpret students’ needs and

(mis)behaviors, she and the other staff were able to develop a common perspective about their

purposes, about appropriate ways of helping students, and about appropriate ways of dealing with

conflicts when they arose.

In the end, a study initiated by one teacher, Ms Ragland, benefited all the teachers. What began as

a self-study eventually became a group study, and teachers’ mutual isolation at work decreased.

Not many teachers, of course, find themselves teaching in a correctional facility. But many—

perhaps most—do experience serious dilemmas and stresses either about students’ behavior or

about their learning. Depending on circumstances, for example, a teacher may wonder how to

respond to students who treat the teacher or other students disrespectfully. Or a teacher may feel

lost about helping certain students who are struggling or wonder where the teacher’s responsibility

ends if a student persists in not learning even after receiving special help. Such uncertainties may

not lead to physical threats, as actually happened to Betty Ragland occasionally, but they can create

a lot of stress nonetheless. But action research can help—systematically studying and reflecting on

how to solve them, reading and listening to how others have done the same, and sharing what

teachers therefore learn. Because of these activities, questions about teaching can be resolved, or at

least clarified, and classroom practice can be enhanced. Most important, the benefits can be shared

not only with the teacher as researcher, but with a teacher’s colleagues as well.

Questions

➢ Consider the three ways discussed in this chapter that research articles can differ: (1) by

how much they seek universal truths, (2) by the response the author expects from the

Educational Psychology 349 A Global Text

Appendix B: Deciding for yourself about the research

reader, and (3) by the assumptions the author makes about the reader’s prior experiences.

Where is Ms Ragland’s action research situated on each of these dimensions of difference?

➢ Consider the ethical issues about action research discussed at the end of this chapter—

insuring privacy of students, gaining informed consent, and insuring freedom to

participate. Given the nature and focus of Ms Ragland’s particular action research, how

might she honor these ethical considerations? Does the fact that her students were

(literally) captives make any difference?

➢ Suppose that instead of an inside staff member like Ms Ragland studying youthful offenders,

an outsider unfamiliar with youth correctional facilities wanted to do so. How would outsider

status affect what could be learned about life in a juvenile correctional facility?

References

Lortie, D. (1975). Schoolteacher. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Zeichner, K. (2007). Accumulating knowledge across self-studies in teacher education. Journal of Teacher

Education, 58(1), 36-46.

Hayes, D. (2006). Telling stories: Sustaining improvement in schools operating under adverse conditions.

Improving Schools, 9(3), 203-213.

Samaras, A. & Freese, A. (Eds.). (2006). Self-study of teaching practices. New York: Peter Lang.

Tidwell, D. & Fitzgerald, L. (Eds.). (2006). Self-study and diversity. Rotterdam, The Netherlands: Sense

Publishers.

Ragland, B. (2007). Positioning the practitioner-researcher: Five ways of looking at practice. Action Research,

4(2), 165-182.

350

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Appendix C: The reflective practitioner

The experience in reflective teaching is that you must plunge into the doing, and try to educate

yourself before you know what it is you’re trying to learn.

(Donald Schön, 1987)

Donald Schön, a philosopher and educational researcher, makes an important observation: learning about

teaching often means making choices and taking actions without knowing in advance quite what the consequences

will be. The problem, as we have pointed out more than once, is that classroom events are often ambiguous and

ambivalent, in that they usually serve more than one purpose. A teacher compliments a student’s contribution to a

discussion: at that moment she may be motivating the student, but also focusing classmates’ thinking on key ideas.

Her comment functions simultaneously as behavioral reinforcement, information, and expression of caring. At that

moment complimenting the student may be exactly the right thing to do. Or not: perhaps the praise causes the

teacher to neglect the contributions of others, or focuses attention on factors that students cannot control, like their

ability instead of their effort. In teaching, it seems, everything cuts more than one way, signifies more than one

thing. The complications can make it difficult to prepare for teaching in advance, though they also make teaching

itself interesting and challenging.

The complications also mean that teachers need to learn from their own teaching by reflecting (or thinking

about the significance of) their experiences. In the classrooms, students are not the only people who need to learn.

So do teachers, though what teachers need to learn is less about curriculum and more about students’ behavior and

motivation, about how to assess their learning well, and about how to shape the class into a mutually supportive

community.

Thinking about these matters helps to make a teacher a reflective practitioner (Schön, 1983), a professional who

learns both from experience and about experience. Becoming thoughtful helps you in all the areas discussed in this

text: it helps in understanding better how students’ learning occurs, what motivates students, how you might

differentiate your instruction more fully, and how you can make assessments of learning more valid and fair.

Learning to reflect on practice is so important, in fact, that we have referred to and illustrated its value

throughout this book. In addition we devote this entire chapter to how you, like other professional teachers, can

develop habits of reflective practice in yourself. In most of this chapter we describe what reflective practice feels like

as an experience, and offer examples of places, people, and activities that can support your own reflection on

practice. We finish by discussing how teachers can also learn simply by observing and reflecting on their own

teaching systematically, and by sharing the results with other teachers and professionals. We call this activity

teacher research or action research. As you will see, reflective practice not only contributes to teachers’ ability to

make wise decisions, but also allows them to serve as effective, principled advocates on behalf of students.

Educational Psychology 351 A Global Text

Appendix C: The reflective practitioner

Types of resources for professional development and learning

At some level reflection on practice is something you must do for yourself, since only you have had your

particular teaching experiences, and only you can choose how to interpret and make use of them. But this rather

individual activity also benefits from the stimulus and challenge offered by fellow professionals. Others’ ideas may

differ from your own, and they can therefore help in working out your own thoughts and in alerting you to ideas

that you may otherwise take for granted. These benefits of reflection can happen in any number of ways, but most

fall into one of four general categories:

• talking and collaborating with colleagues

• participating in professional associations

• attending professional development workshops and conferences

• reading professional literature

In the next sections we explore what each of these activities has to offer.

Colleagues as a resource

Perhaps the simplest way to stimulate reflections about your own teaching is to engage fellow teachers or other

colleagues in dialogue (or thoughtful conversation) about teaching and learning: What do you think of this kind of

experience? Have you ever had one like it yourself, and what did you make of it? Note that to be helpful in

stimulating reflection, these conversations need to be largely about educational matters, not about personal ones

(“What movie did you see last night?”). Dialogues with individual colleagues have certain advantages to more

complex or formal professional experiences. Talking with an individual generally allows more participation for both

of you, since only two people may need to express their views. It also can provide a measure of safety or

confidentiality if your conversation partner is a trusted colleague; sometimes, therefore, you can share ideas of

which you are not sure, or that may be controversial.

A somewhat more complex way of stimulating reflection is group study. Several teachers at a school gather

regularly to bring themselves up to date on a new curriculum, for example, or to plan activities or policies related to

a school-wide theme (e.g. “the environment”). Group meetings often result in considerable dialog among the

members about the best ways to teach and to manage classrooms, as well as stories about students’ behavior and

learning experiences. For a beginning teacher, group study can be a particularly good way to learn from

experienced, veteran teachers.

Sharing of ideas becomes even more intense if teachers collaborate with each other about their work on an

extended basis. Collaboration can take many forms; in one form it might be “team teaching” by two or more

teachers working with one group of students, and in another form it might be two or more teachers consulting

regularly to coordinate the content of their courses. Collaborations work best when each member of the team brings

responsibilities and expertise that are unique, but also related to the other members’ responsibilities. Imagine, for

example, a collaboration between Sharon, who is a middle-years classroom teacher, and Pat, who is a resource

teacher—one whose job is to assist classroom teachers in working with students with educational disabilities or

special needs. If Pat spends time in Sharon’s classroom, then not only will the students benefit, but they both may

learn from each other’s presence. Potentially, Pat can learn the details of the middle-years curriculum and learn

352

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

more about the full range of students’ skills—not just those of students having difficulties. Sharon can get ideas

about how to help individuals who, in a classroom context, seem especially difficult to help. Achieving these

benefits, of course, comes at a cost: the two teachers may need to take time not only for the students, but also to talk

with each other. Sometimes the time-cost can be reduced somewhat if their school administrators can arrange for a

bit of extra planning and sharing time. But even if this does not happen, the benefits of collaboration will be very

real, and often make the investment of time worthwhile.

Professional associations and professional development activities

Another way to stimulate reflection about teaching is by joining and participating in professional associations—

organizations focused on supporting the work of teachers and on upholding high standards of teaching practice.

Table 45 lists several major professional associations related to education and their Internet addresses. Most of

them are composed of local branches or chapters serving the needs of a particular city, state, or region.

Table 45: A selection of professional associations related to education

• American Association for the Mentally Retarded (AAMR) [www.aamr.org]

• Association for Health, Physical Education, Recreation, & Dance (AAHPERD) [www.aahperd.org]

• Association for Experiential Education (AEE) [www.princeton.edu/~rcurtis/aee.html]

• Association for Retarded Citizens (ARC) [www.thearc.org/welcome.html]

• ENC Online Resources for Math and Science Education [www.enc.org/stan.htm]

• National Association for Bilingual Education (NABE) [www.nabe.org/]

• National Association for the Education of Young Children (NAEYC) [www.naeyc.org/]

• National Council for Teachers of Mathematics (NCTM) [[www.nctm.org/]

• National Council for the Social Studies (NCSS) [www.socialstudies.org/]

• National Council for Teachers of English (NCTE) [www.ncte.org]

• National Science Teachers Association [www.nsta.org/]

• Organization of American Historians (OAH) [www.oah.org]

To achieve their purposes, a professional association provides a mixture of publications, meetings, and

conferences intended for the professional development of educators, including classroom teachers. Typically the

publications include either a relatively frequent newsletter or a less frequent journal focused on issues of practice or

research. Very large associations often publish more than one newsletter or journal, each of which is focused on a

particular topic or type of news (for example, the National Education Association in the United States publishes

eight separate periodicals). Some also publish online journals (there are several listed as part of Table 46 or online

Educational Psychology 353 A Global Text

Appendix C: The reflective practitioner

versions of print journals. Whatever format they take, professionally sponsored publications stimulate thinking by

discussing issues and dilemmas faced by professional educators, and sometimes also by presenting recent

educational research and the recommendations for teaching that flow from that research. We discuss ways of using

these publications further in the next section of this chapter.

Table 46: A sampling of journals related to professional education

• Annotated Bibliography of Education Journals – annotations of over 426 education related journals

and extensive links to educational organizations and institutions that sponsor them.

• CSS Journal – Computers in the Social Studies – dedicated to the encouragement of the use of

computers and related technology in K-12 social studies classrooms.

• Education Policy and Analysis – published by the College of Education at Arizona State University

• Educational Theory – publishes work in the philosophy of education and other disciplines.

• Effective Teaching – electronic journal devoted to the exchange of ideas and information relevant to

college and university teaching in North Carolina.

• Harvard Educational Review – quarterly journal that provides an inter-disciplinary forum for

innovative thinking and research in education.

• Interact – European platform for interactive learning and new media.

• Journal of Computing in Higher Education – publishes articles that contribute to our understanding of

the issues, problems, and research associated with instructional technology

• New jour – electronic journal and newsletter archive.

• Revista Iberoamericana de Educacion – Revista de la OEI.

• Scholarly Electronic Journals – Trends and Attitudes: A Research Proposal

• WORLDSPEAKER online – an international academic journal written by and for international

scholars, university administrators, and researchers.

Meetings and conferences sponsored by a professional association also take a variety of forms. Depending on the

size of the association and on the importance of the topic, a meeting could be as short as a one half-day workshop or

as long as a full week with many sessions occurring simultaneously. Sometimes, too, an association might sponsor a

more extended course—a series of meetings focused on one topic or problem of concern to teachers, such as

classroom management or curriculum planning. In some cases, the course might carry university credit, though not

always.

As you might expect, the size of a professional association makes a difference in kinds of professional

development experiences it can provide. In general, the smaller the association, the more exclusively it focuses on

local news and educational needs, both in its publications and in its meetings or other activities. At a professional

development workshop sponsored by a local teachers’ association, for example, you are relatively likely to see

colleagues and acquaintances not only from your own school, but from other neighboring schools. Locally

354

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

sponsored events are also more likely to focus on local issues, such as implementing a new system for assessing

students’ learning within the local schools. In general, too, local events tend to cost less to attend, in both time and

money.

By the same token, the larger the association, the more its professional development opportunities are likely to

focus on large-scale trends in education, such as the impact of the No Child Left Behind legislation we discussed in

Chapter 1 or the latest trends in using computer technology for teaching. Conferences or other professional

development events are more likely to span several days and to be located outside the immediate town or region

whether you live and work. You may therefore see fewer of your everyday colleagues and acquaintances, but you

may also have a greater incentive to make new acquaintances whose interests or concerns are similar to your own.

The event is more likely to feature educators who are well-known nationally or internationally, and to call attention

to educational trends or issues that are new or unfamiliar.

Whether large or small, the activities of professional associations can stimulate thinking and reflecting about

teaching. By meeting and talking with others at a meeting of an association, teachers learn new ideas for teaching,

become aware of emerging trends and issues about education, and confront assumptions that they may have made

about their own practices with students. Professional meetings, conferences, and workshops can provide these

benefits because they draw on the expertise and experience of a wide range of professionals—usually wider than is

possible within a single school building. But compared simply to talking with your immediate colleagues, they have

a distinct disadvantage: they take effort and a bit of money to attend, and sometimes they are available at

convenient times. Well-balanced professional development should therefore also include activities that are

available frequently, but that also draw on a wide range of expertise. Fortunately, an activity with these features is

often easily at hand: the reading of professional publications about educational research and practice.

Reading and understanding professional articles

Although publications about educational issues and research can take many forms, they tend to serve three

major purposes in some sort of combination. A publication could either (1) provide a framework for understanding

teaching and learning, (2) offer advice about how to teach, or (3) advocate particular ideas or practices about

education. Benefiting from a professional publication depends partly on understanding which of these purposes a

particular article or book is emphasizing.

Three purposes of educational publications

Consider the first purpose, to provide a framework for understanding teaching and learning (Hittleman and

Simon, 2005). A “framework” in this context means a perspective or general viewpoint for understanding specific

events and actions. They are much like the theories described earlier in this book, though not always as formal or

broad. A published article might propose, for example, a way of understanding why certain students are

disrespectful in spite of teachers’ efforts to prevent such behavior (perhaps they are reinforced by peers for being

disrespectful). It might offer evidence supporting this perspective. In doing so, the author provides a sort of “theory

of disrespectful behavior”, though he or she may not call it a theory explicitly.

A second purpose is to offer advice about appropriate teaching practices. An article intended for this purpose,

for example, might suggest how to introduce reading instruction to first graders, or how to use fiction to teach high

school history, or how to organize a class to include a student with a disability. Often giving such advice overlaps

Educational Psychology 355 A Global Text

Appendix C: The reflective practitioner

with the first purpose, providing a framework for understanding, since thinking about an educational issue in a

particular way may imply certain ways of dealing with it in practice.

A third purpose of a published article is to advocate ideas and persuade others to take actions benefiting

students and society. It might take a position about important issues in education: Is it a good idea or not to retain

(or hold back) a student in grade level for another year if the student fails the curriculum the first time? Should

schools teach about sexuality? Should girls learn science in classrooms separate from boys? In advocating for ideas

or policies about such matters, the article may express concern about what is good, ethical or desirable in

education, not just about what is factually true or practical. The author may seek explicitly to persuade readers of

the author’s point of view. These features do not mean, however, that you need to give up thinking for yourself. On

the contrary, when reading an advocacy-oriented article, reflection may be especially important.

Whatever its purpose—understanding, advice, or advocacy—an article or book about a professional issue can

stimulate thinking about what you know and believe about teaching and learning. It should therefore create, rather

than undermine, your individuality as a teacher. Think of professional reading as a dialogue or conversation about

education: some of the comments in the conversation will probably be more helpful than others, but each

participant contributes somehow, even if none can give a final answer or everlasting truth. It is the same with

publications; some may be more helpful than others, but none will be so perfect that you can afford to cease further

reading or further thinking. If you are about to begin a teaching career, for example, you may be especially

interested in anything published about classroom management, but less interested in the problems of

administering schools or in the political issues that usually accompany educational systems. Yet some publications

may discuss these latter issues anyway, and eventually you may find yourself more concerned about them than at

the start of a career. Your job, as a reflective teacher, will be sort out the currently useful articles (or parts of

articles) from ones you cannot use immediately.

To experience educational publications in this way, however, you must think of the authors as your collaborators

as well as general authorities. As a reader, you need to assume that you are entitled to consider an author’s ideas,

but not obligated to accept it without journals related to professional education question. There are several

strategies for developing this attitude, but to keep the discussion focused, we will look at just two. We have already

discussed the first strategy, which is to understand the purposes of any particular piece of research which you

encounter, in order to assess its current usefulness to your daily work and your long-term professional goals. We

have already indicated several general purposes of educational research publications, but we will go into more

detail about this in the next section. The second strategy for relating to authors as collaborators is to think about

how you yourself might contribute to professional knowledge by engaging in research of your own, even as a

classroom teacher—an activity often called action research (Mills, 2006; Stringer, 2007). At the end of this chapter

we discuss what action research involves, and how you might consider using it.

Authors’ assumptions about readers

Authors of professional articles and books also make assumptions about their readers, and it helps to be aware of

these while you read. The assumptions affect the style, content, and significance of the author’s ideas in ways that

are both obvious and subtle.

356

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

One assumption is about the response which an author expects from you, the reader: does he or she expect you

actually to do something new, or simply to consider doing something new? Or does the author just want you to be

aware of a new idea? Consider, for example, an article reviewing best practices about inclusion of students with

special needs. The author may imply, or even urge you to take a moral position: you should include these students,

the author may seem to say. But in a different article—one recommending particular teaching practices—the author

may merely ask you to think about alternatives to your normal ways of teaching. Certain strategies worked under

certain teaching conditions, the author says, so simply consider whether they might work for you as well.

A second, less obvious difference among professional publications is in their un-stated assumptions about prior

experiences and attitudes of readers. This assumption may be either helpful or frustrating, depending on you actual

prior background. A piece intended as a “framework for understanding” may assume, for example, that you are

familiar with basic theories of learning already. If you have read and understood what we outlined in Chapter 2 of

this book, the article may turn out to be relatively accessible or understandable to you even if you have relatively

little experience in actual classroom teaching, and even if you have never studied learning theories in detail. The

article might seem more accessible than you expect because, for example, it focuses primarily on how teacher's

praise affects students' learning, an idea with which you may be somewhat familiar already.

On the other hand, a professional publication may assume that you have taught school for a number of years

already, or that you are at least familiar with classroom life from the point of view not of students, but of a teacher.

An author writing about “withitness” (discussed in Chapter 7), for example, may make this assumption, since the

concept originated by observing teachers managing large group classroom activities. If you yourself are experienced

at actual teaching, reading about withitness may trigger a lot of questions about just how withit teachers are able to

be in practice, and about whether in fact they always need to be withit. You can also ask yourself these questions

even if you have not yet been a teacher yourself, of course, but they may seem less immediate or urgent.

A professional article intended to advocate for a particular educational policy or practice may make very

different assumptions about you as a reader. It may assume, for example, that you do in fact enjoy persuading

others of your point of view, even when others initially disagree or react indifferently. This sort of assumption may

show up as much in what the writing omits, as in what it includes: if the term cooperative learning activity is used

without explanation, for example, the researcher may be assuming not only that you are the sort of person—

perhaps a teacher—who knows what that term means already, but also that you already believe in the value of

cooperative learning and are motivated to explain its value to others.

In making these distinctions among published articles, keep in mind a point we made at the outset: that an

individual article usually serves more than one purpose at a time and makes more than one assumption about your

prior knowledge and about how you are supposed to respond to the article. The differences are only about

emphasis. To illustrate these ideas about the purposes and effects of research, look in the next section at three

examples of actual published articles relevant to education. The studies are not a full cross-section of educational

research or publications, but they do suggest some of the variety possible (and necessary) among them. Each

example serves a mixture of purposes, but also emphasizes one purpose in particular (perspective-taking, teaching

recommendations, or advocacy) described earlier. The authors of each example also make particular assumptions

about you, the reader—about the intellectual work which the authors expect you to do and about the motivations

Educational Psychology 357 A Global Text

Appendix C: The reflective practitioner

which they assume you have or hope that you will acquire. For each example, we describe the reactions of one of us

(Kelvin Seifert) as he read the article.

Example #1: How do children acquire moral commitments?

In 1997, Herbert Saltzstein and several colleagues published a research-oriented article about how

children acquire moral beliefs (Saltzstein, et al., 1997). The group of researchers were all graduate students

and professors of psychology, working mostly at the City University of New York. When Kelvin read of their

affiliation with psychology, he suspected that they would talk about moral beliefs in general, and not

necessarily about moral issues in classrooms, such as cheating or treating classmates with care and respect.

Still, the article interested Kelvin as a former teacher and current university professor, because he had long

been concerned with fostering qualities like integrity, honesty, cooperation, and loyalty in students. If Kelvin

could find out about the mechanism or process by which children acquire mature moral beliefs, he reasoned,

maybe he could modify his teaching to take advantage of that knowledge.

So Kelvin began reading the article. He discovered some parts were challenging and required careful reflection,

whereas others were easier to read. One of the most challenging passages came almost immediately, in the second

and third paragraphs; these paragraphs, it seemed, required a bit of prior knowledge about theories of moral

development. But Kelvin was willing to concentrate more fully on these paragraphs, because he expected that they

might clarify the rest of the study. Here are the paragraphs, and some of Kelvin’s thoughts as he read them:

Initial problem: We began by re-examining the

phenomenon of heteronomy, Piaget's assertion

(1932/1965) following Kant (1785/1959) that young

children equate moral obligation with deference to

authority when justifying their moral judgments. The

concept is important because it is central to the

organismic account of moral development as a series of

differentiations and integrations…. [p. 37]

This was one of the difficult paragraphs, perhaps

especially because Kelvin had never read the specific book

by Piaget or by the philosopher Kant. But Kelvin did recall

reading, at various times over the years, about Piaget's

views on moral development. Piaget believed that at first,

children define morality in terms of what adults think: an

action is “good” if and only if adults (e.g. parents)

consider it good, and “bad” if and only if adults consider it

bad. This is the idea of “heteronomy” to which Saltzstein

is referring. Children, in this view, take quite awhile to

develop or “grow” into truly autonomous moral beliefs.

Autonomous beliefs form slowly out of earlier beliefs, in

the way that a young plant or animal might grow. This is

the “organismic account of moral development” that

Saltzstein is talking about.

…This account has been challenged by Turiel's

domain theory (Turiel, 1983). According to Turiel and

his colleagues, even young children intuitively

distinguish moral from conventional rules. [p. 37]

Here was an idea that was intriguing! Saltzstein and

his colleagues were pointing to research (by the person

cited, named Turiel) that suggests that even

preschoolers know the difference between truly moral

358

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

rules and merely conventional rules. Apparently they

believe, for example, that it would be wrong to steal toys

or to hit someone, even if adults gave you permission to

do so. But apparently they also know that it would be

OK for traffic lights to use different colors—for red to

mean “go” and green to mean “stop”—provided that

everyone agreed on changing the rule. That is what the

researcher named Turiel apparently meant by

distinguishing convention from morality.

The introduction continued in this challenging style for about two pages, requiring Kelvin to read slowly and

carefully in order to understand its points. Kelvin was not discouraged from continuing, though, because he wanted

to find out more about how, in general, children acquire moral beliefs. Did moral beliefs take time to develop—did

they “grow” on children slowly after initially being borrowed from parents or other adults? In this case, then maybe

Kelvin owed it to his students to adopt and express desirable moral attitudes myself, so as to provide a good model

for their developing beliefs. Or were students’ key moral beliefs already in place when they entered school—almost

as if “hard wired” in their minds, or at least already learned during infancy and the preschool years? In this second

case, it might still be desirable for Kelvin to adopt positive moral attitudes, but not for the purpose of modeling

them for students. Students already “hard wired” for key moral beliefs might not need a model so much as an

enforcer of desirable moral behaviors. Concerning the issue of cheating, for example, the students might already

understand the undesirable nature and implications of this behavior. As a result they might not need

demonstrations of honest integrity from their teacher as much as affirmations from the teacher of the importance

of honesty and integrity, along with consistent enforcement of appropriate sanctions against cheating when it did

occur.

For Kelvin, therefore, the outcomes of research on moral development—including Saltzstein's that he was

currently reading—posed issues of classroom management, both in university classrooms and in public school

classrooms. So Kelvin read on. Saltzstein proposed resolving the issues about the origins of moral development by

distinguishing between moral conflicts and moral dilemmas:

Moral conflicts are conflicts between moral duty or

right and a non-moral desire. An example might be the

conflict between whether to return a wallet to its

rightful owner or keep the coveted wallet with its extra

cash. In contrast, moral dilemmas are conflicts

involving two moral rights or duties. For example, [a

person might feel a dilemma between] whether to steal

a drug to save a spouse's life. [p. 38]

The distinction between conflicts and dilemmas

looked promising to Kelvin. Moral conflicts looked

fairly simple in cognitive terms, even if they were

sometimes difficult emotionally. The “right” action was

obvious. Moral dilemmas were more complex

cognitively as well as emotionally, because two “goods”

were being weighed against each other. The moral

alternatives might both be right and wrong at the same

time, and their relative “rightness” might not be

immediately obvious.

Educational Psychology 359 A Global Text

Appendix C: The reflective practitioner

Saltzstein and his colleagues proposed that when young children show awareness of moral rules, they may be

doing so in the simpler context of moral conflicts. A young child might believe that you should return a dollar to its

owner, even if the child has trouble in practice overcoming a selfish impulse to keep the dollar. The same child

might have trouble deciding, however, whether it is “right” to inform his teacher if a best friend has cheated on a

test. In that case two moral principles compete for attention—honesty and loyalty to a friend. To sort out the

implications of choosing between these principles, a young child might need to rely on older, wiser minds, such as

parents or other adults. The minute that he or she does so, the child is showing the moral heteronomy that Piaget

used to write about and that Saltzstein referred to early in the article.

Understanding these ideas took effort, but once Kelvin began figuring them out, the rest of the article was easier

to follow. In reading the remaining pages, he noted in passing that the researchers used several techniques common

in educational research. For example, they interviewed participants, a common way of gathering systematic

information about individuals' thinking. They also imposed controls on their procedures and on the selection of

participants. Procedures were controlled, for example, by posing the same three moral dilemmas and to all

participants, so that individuals' responses could be compared meaningfully. The selection of participants was

controlled by selecting two age groups for deliberate comparison with each other—one that was seven years old and

the other that was eleven. Since the researchers wanted to generalize about moral development as much as possible,

but they obviously could not interview every child in the world, they sampled participants: they selected a

manageable number (sixty-five, to be exact) from the larger student population of one particular school. In a second

part of the investigation, they also selected a comparable number of children of the same two ages (7 and 11) from

the city of Recife, located in Brazil. The Brazilian group's responses were compared deliberately with the American

group's responses, in order to allow for the impact of cultural beliefs on moral development in general. Kelvin

recognized this research strategy as an example of using control groups. In research terms, the Brazilian group

“controlled for” the impact of American culture on children's moral beliefs, and vice versa, the American group

controlled for the impact of Brazilian culture on children's moral beliefs. Altogether, these techniques helped insure

that the interviews of children's moral beliefs really illustrated what they were supposed to illustrate—that they

were reliable and valid, in the senses that we discussed in earlier chapters. As Kelvin noticed Saltzstein's attention

to good research techniques, he gained confidence in Saltzstein’s observations and in the interpretations that the

authors made from them.

What did Saltzstein and his colleagues find out—or more to the point, what did Kelvin Seifert learn from what

Saltzstein and his colleagues wrote about? There were three ideas that occurred to Kelvin. One was that in everyday

life, children probably deal with moral beliefs of all levels of cognitive complexity, and not just “simple” moral

conflicts and “complex” moral dilemmas. Saltzstein found that children's solutions to moral dilemmas depended a

lot on the content of the dilemma. Children advocated strongly for truthfulness in some situations (for example, in

deciding whether to tell the teacher about a friend's cheating), but not in other situations (like in deciding whether

to back up a friend who is being teased and who has lied in an effort to stop the teasing). But it was rare for all

children to support any one moral principle completely; they usually supported a mix.

Another idea that Kelvin learned from Saltzstein's research was about how children expressed moral

heteronomy versus moral autonomy. Age, it seemed, did not affect the beliefs that children stated; younger and

older children took similar positions on all dilemmas initially. But age did affect how steadfastly children held to

360

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

initial beliefs. Younger children were more easily influenced to switch opinions when an adult “cross-examined”

with probing questions; older children were more likely to keep to their initial position. Moral heteronomy was

revealed not by a child's views as such, but by the kind of dialogue a child has with adults.

A third idea that Kelvin learned was about children's perceptions of adults' moral beliefs. Saltzstein found that

even though older children (the 11-year-olds) showed more moral autonomy (were more steadfast) than younger

children, they tended to believe that adults thought about moral issues in ways similar to children who were

younger. In the “teasing” dilemma mentioned above, for example, the 11-year-olds opted much more often than 7-

year-olds for remaining loyal to a friend, even though doing so meant further untruthfulness with peers. Yet the 11-

year-olds also more often stated a belief that adults would resolve the same dilemma in a way characteristic of 7-

year-olds—that is, by telling the truth to peers and thus betraying loyalty to a friend. This finding puzzled Kelvin.

Why should older, and presumably more insightful, children think that adults are more like younger children than

like themselves? Saltzstein suggested an interpretation, however, that helped him make sense of the apparent

inconsistency:

…Consistent with our past research, children

attributed the kinds of moral choices made by younger

children to adults. In our view, this finding tends to

support a constructivist rather than a [social modeling]

view of morality, which would predict that the child's

judgments mirror (or develop toward) their

representation of adult judgments. [p. 41]

In other words, thought Kelvin, if children learned

moral beliefs by imitating (or modeling themselves

after) parents or other adults, then they ought to see

themselves as resembling adults more and more as they

get older. Instead, they see themselves as resembling

adults less, at least during middle childhood. This

would happen only if they were preoccupied with

“constructing” their own beliefs on the basis of their

experiences, and therefore failed to notice that adults

might also have constructed beliefs similar to their own.

Relevance: a framework for understanding moral development

The article by Saltzstein offered a way to understand how children develop moral beliefs, and especially to

understand the change from moral heteronomy to moral autonomy. By imposing controls on the procedures

(uniform interviews) and on the selection of participants (particular ages, particular societies or cultures), the

researchers eliminated certain sources of ambiguity or variability in children's responses. By framing their project

in terms of previous theories of moral development (Piaget’s, Turiel’s), furthermore, they made it easier to interpret

their new results in the general terms of these theories as well. In these ways the investigation aspired to provide a

general perspective about children's moral development. Providing a framework for understanding, you recall, is

one of the major purposes of many professional publications.

But note that the authors paid a price for emphasizing this purpose. By organizing their work around existing

general theory and research, they had to assume that readers already had some knowledge of that theory and

research. This is not an unreasonable assumption if the readers are expected to be fellow researchers; after all,

many of them make a living by “knowing the literature” of psychology. But assuming such knowledge can be an

obstacle if the authors intend to communicate with non-psychologists: in that case, either the authors must make

Educational Psychology 361 A Global Text

Appendix C: The reflective practitioner

more of an effort to explain the relevant background research, or readers must educate themselves about the

research. The latter activity is not necessarily difficult (the background knowledge for Saltzstein's work, for

example, took me only a few paragraphs to explain in writing), but it must be done to make full sense of research

that tries to provide a universal framework of psychological knowledge.

The reader's role: interested observer of children

In conducting and reporting their research, Saltzstein and his colleagues were not presenting themselves as

school teachers, nor were they expecting readers necessarily to respond as teachers. As they put it in the first

paragraph of the article, they sought to offer “a more contextualized perspective for understanding the development

of moral judgments” [p. 37]. Unlike most teachers, they seemed indifferent to recommending how children's moral

judgements ought to be fostered. Observation of children was their purpose, not intervention. The meaning of the

term “contextualized perspective” was not obvious to Kelvin when he first read it, but eventually it became clearer:

they were talking about the importance of distinguishing among types of moral decisions and moral beliefs. They

did sometimes note information relevant to teaching—for example, they pointed out that for cultural reasons,

teachers in Brazil do not command high respect and therefore compared to American children, Brazilian children

may feel less compelled to tell the truth to their teachers. But this comment was not the primary focus of their

research, nor did the authors discuss what (if anything) it might imply about teaching in the United States.

Yet the non-teaching perspective of the article did not keep Kelvin, a long-time school teacher and current

university teacher, from reflecting on the article in terms of its educational relevance. As we mentioned already,

Kelvin was attracted to the article because of his own concerns about character development in students—how do

they acquire moral beliefs and commitments, and how should he help them in doing so? Kelvin did not really expect

to find an answer to the second of these questions, given the “observation” orientation of the authors. He did hope

to find an answer to the first, although even here he also expected that to make allowances for the fact that research

interviews are not usually identical to classroom situations. Children might respond differently when interviewed

individually by a researcher, compared to how they might respond to a teacher in class. Or perhaps not. So in

reflecting on the article, Kelvin had to note the context and purposes of Saltzstein's study, and to remind himself

that once a teacher went beyond simply observing children to intervening on their behalf, the teacher might be led

to different conclusions about children’s moral development. But in spite of these cautions—or maybe because of

them—Kelvin found much food for thought in the article related to teaching.

Example #2: Learning disability as a misleading label

In 2006, Ray McDermott, Shelley Goldman, and Hervé Varenne published an article that discussed the use

of disability categories in education. The article attracted Kelvin’s attention because he had been concerned

for a long time about the ambiguities of disability categories (see Chapter 5 of this book) as well as about

their potential for stigmatizing individuals. He expected the article to document additional problems with

labeling when a student is from a non-white ethnic group. Kelvin’s expectation was fulfilled partially, but he

was surprised also to encounter an additional and tougher message in the article. Here is how the study

began:

Since about 1850…classifying human beings by

mental ability, accurately or not, has been a politically

Kelvin had a mixed reaction to this opening. In one

way it seemed to say something familiar—that

362

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

rewarded activity. Those with power have placed others,

usually the downtrodden, into ability and disposition

groups that they cannot escape… People who live

together in a culture must struggle constantly with the

constraints…of systems of classification and

interpretation used in the culture.

classification systems (such as categories for

disabilities) may create problems for individuals. But

the tone of the paragraph sounded more severely

critical than Kelvin had expected: it was saying that

power governed all classifications, implying that

misclassifications may be widespread or even universal.

Kelvin’s initial hunch was therefore that the article would express a radically critical view of disability

classifications—particularly as they affect the “downtrodden”, which presumably included children from

minority ethnic groups. His expectation proved correct as the authors explained their point of view, which

they called a cultural approach to understanding disability. Using learning disabilities (LD) as an example,

here is how they explained their position:

We are not as interested in LD behavior as in the

preoccupations—as seen from the level of classroom

organization—of all those adults who are professionally

poised to discover LD behavior. We are less interested

in the characteristics of LD children than in the cultural

arrangements that make an LD label relevant.

At this point Kelvin was not sure if he wanted to

continue reading the article because it seemed like it

might not be relevant to classroom life specifically. It

also implied a severe criticism of professional educators

—implied that they are too eager to find examples of LD

and for this reason may misclassify students. On the

other hand, Kelvin was already aware that LD are an

especially ambiguous category of disability; maybe the

article would help to show why. So he kept reading.

The authors continued by outlining the history of LD as a category of disability, describing this category as an

outgrowth of the general intelligence testing movement during the twentieth century. By the 1970s, they argued, the

concept of LD offered a way to classify children with academic difficulties without having to call the children

mentally disabled. Because of this fact, the LD category was needed—literally—by well-off parents who did not want

their children treated or educated as children with mental disabilities. LD as a concept and category came to be

applied primarily to children from the white middle-class, and mental disability became, by default, the equivalent

category for the non-white and poor.

To support this assertion, the authors reported a classroom observation of three non-white boys—Hector,

Ricardo, and Boomer—while they worked together to design an imaginary research station in Antarctica. Citing

actual transcripts of conversation while the boys worked, the authors concluded that all three boys showed

intelligence and insight about the assignment, but that the teacher was only aware of the contributions of one of the

boys. Hector systematically hid his knowledge from the teacher’s view by getting Boomer to speak for their group;

Ricardo participated well in the group work but was rarely acknowledged by the other two boys. Boomer received

considerable praise from the teacher, thanks to his speaking for the group. Yet the teacher was never aware of these

subtleties. The authors blamed her oversight not on the teacher herself, but on an educational and cultural system

that leads educators to classify or typify students too quickly or easily. Here is how they put it:

Educational Psychology 363 A Global Text

Appendix C: The reflective practitioner

The American classroom is well organized for the

production of display of failure, one child at a time if

possible, but group by group if necessary…Even if the

teacher manages to treat every child as capable, the

children can hammer each other into negative status;

and even if both…resist dropping everyone into

predefined categories, the children’s parents can take

over, demanding more and more boxes with which to

specify kinds of kids doing better than other kinds of

kids. In such a classroom, if there were no LD

categories, someone would have to invent them.

When Kelvin read this conclusion, he did not really

disagree, but he did feel that it was beside the point for

most teachers. Maybe children do get classified too

easily, he thought, but a teacher’s job is not just to

lament this possibility, as the authors seemed to be

doing. Instead their job is to help the real, live children

for whom they have daily responsibility. What teachers

need are therefore suggestions to avoid misclassifying

students by overlooking key information about them.

Kelvin wished, at the end, that the authors had made

some of these suggestions.

Relevance: a critical framework

In this study the authors offered a sort of backhanded framework of thinking about categories of disability; or

more precisely they offered a framework for understanding what the categories are not. In essence they said that

disability categories describe qualities “in” students only in the sense that educators and others happen to think of

disability categories in this way. An equally reasonable way to think about disabilities, they argued, is that modern

society is organized so that its citizens have to be classified for many different reasons. Educators are simply

helping to implement this society-wide expectation. A frequent result in classrooms is that teachers classify

students too easily and that key evidence of students’ capacity is overlooked.

In making this argument, the authors implied an indirect recommendation about how to teach, though the

recommendation actually focused on what teachers should not do. Instead of (mis)identifying children with

learning difficulties, the authors implied, teachers and other educators should stop concerning themselves with

classifying children, and seek to reorganize classrooms and schools so that classification is less important. “Change

the school”, they wrote, “and LD becomes less relevant”. This conclusion may be an important reminder, but it is

not especially helpful as a recommendation to practicing teachers, who usually need to know about more than what

to avoid.

The readers’ role: concerned advocate for social justice

It is not surprising that the article lacked concrete recommendations for teaching, given that the authors seemed

to speak to readers not as classroom teachers, but as general critics of society who are concerned about fairness or

social justice. Their comments made two assumptions: first, that readers will want to minimize unfair stereotypes of

students, and second, that readers will seek greater fairness in how teachers treat students. For readers who happen

to be teachers themselves, the first of these assumptions is a reasonable one; most of us would indeed like to

minimize unfair stereotyping of students. The second is also reasonable, but perhaps not in a way that the authors

intended. Teachers probably do try their best to treat students fairly and respectfully. Their responsibilities usually

mean, however, that they can only do this conveniently with their own students; the time available to work toward

364

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

general social justice is often limited. (As you might suspect, Kelvin was not fully satisfied after he finished reading

this article!)

Example #3: The impact of bilingualism on reading

In 1995, three education professors—Robert Jiménez, Georgia García, and David Pearson—published a

study about the impact of bilingualism on children's ability to read English (1995). The three specialized in

curriculum studies, literacy acquisition, and bilingual language development, and were therefore motivated

by a concern for the academic success of bilingual children and especially by concern for identifying why

bilingual children sometimes have difficulty learning to read English. Too much research on bilingualism,

they argued, was based on what they called a “deficit” framework: it focused on what bilingual children

lacked compared to monolinguals. They sought an alternative framework, one focused on bilingual students'

competence, and especially on their competence to read a second language.

To search for this alternative, the researchers mounted a large research program, and the article published in

1995 was one of the studies resulting from this research. It caught Kelvin’s interest not only because of its topic, but

because of its approach. Instead of surveying dozens of students with a questionnaire, as researchers sometimes do,

these investigators relied on just three students studied intensively. Each student became a case study and included

detailed, lengthy observations and interviews of that particular student. Each student was chosen deliberately for a

particular purpose. One was a highly proficient reader who was also bilingual (Spanish and English); a second was a

marginally proficient reader who was bilingual (Spanish and English); and a third was a highly proficient reader

who was monolingual in English. To qualify for the study, furthermore, each student had to be comfortable

reflecting on and talking about their own reading processes, so that the authors could interview them at length on

this topic. The researchers asked each student to read six one-page passages in English and (where relevant) in

Spanish. They invited all three to think aloud about their reading as they went along, commenting on how they

figured out particular words or passages. The oral readings and think-aloud commentaries were taped and

transcribed, and became the information on which the authors based their conclusions and recommendations.

Using these procedures, Jiménez, García, and Pearson discovered important differences among the three girls.

The proficient bilingual, Pamela, used her growing knowledge of each language to help in learning vocabulary from

the other language. When she encountered the English word “species” , for example, she guessed correctly that it

meant the same as the similar Spanish word “especies”; and when she encountered the Spanish “liquído”, she

guessed correctly that it meant the English “liquid”. Her focus on learning vocabulary was stronger than for the

proficient monolingual, Michelle, who commented less on specific words than how the overall reading passages

related to her prior general knowledge. The difference presumably stemmed from Michelle's greater familiarity with

English vocabulary—so much greater, in fact, that Michelle did not need to think about individual words

deliberately. Both Michelle and Pamela differed, however, from the less-proficient bilingual reader, Christine. Like

Pamela, Christine focused on vocabulary, but she did not think of her native Spanish as a resource for this task.

When reading a Spanish word, she was sometimes reminded of English equivalents (“cognates,” as language

teachers call them), but she did not use her much greater knowledge of Spanish to assist with her more limited

English. She did not search for equivalent words deliberately, as Pamela did.

Educational Psychology 365 A Global Text

Appendix C: The reflective practitioner

Relevance: recommendations for teaching english as an additional language

The authors of this article focused more directly on particular learning behaviors than did the authors of the two

articles described earlier. Jimenez and his colleagues emphasized the importance of regarding a child's native

language as a strength in the process, not a liability, and they then pointed out the importance of facilitating

vocabulary development. But they did not claim this recommendation to be appropriate for all children or for all

forms of bilingualism. They only focused on a particular pair of languages (Spanish and English in the USA), and on

three combinations of skill level in these two languages. These are common bilingual experiences in the United

States, but they are not the only ones, either in the United States or elsewhere in the world.

For other bilingual situations, their conclusions might not hold true. For some students (e.g. Chinese-

Americans), the native language and the second language are much more different in vocabulary, pronunciation,

and grammar than Spanish is to English, and therefore may provide less of a resource to a child learning to read. In

some settings, relationships between languages are more equal than in the United States. In Canada, for example,

both the numbers and the overall social status of English speakers and French speakers are more equal than in the

United States. In both of these situations, if a child fails to learn to read the second language, it may not be for the

reasons suggested by Robert Jiménez, but for other reasons, ranging from difficulties with reading per se to cultural

differences in how a child expects to be taught (Johnson, 2004).

The reader's role: both teacher and researcher

In the published article describing their research, Jiménez, García, and Pearson assumed that readers have some

familiarity with bilingual students and with issues related to teaching reading. They began their article by

describing previous research studies in these areas—more than a dozen of them, in fact. In the middle they

described numerous responses of the three bilingual students to the passages they were asked to read. At the end of

the article they made specific suggestions for teaching, such as “focus more on vocabulary development”. When

Kelvin read these various sections, he found that his prior knowledge of and reflections about teaching helped to

make sense of them. But he also found that did not need to be an expert in bilingualism order to understand the

authors' messages—he had never, in fact, taught English as a Second Language, nor had he ever conducted research

on reading or bilingual language development.

Action research: hearing from teachers about improving practice

Each of the professional articles just described offers ideas and recommendations that can stimulate reflection

about teaching and learning. But they all suffer from a particular limitation: Although they often relate to teachers

and classrooms, teachers’ role in influencing in designing and interpreting a study is minimal. In the world of

educational research, persons other than teachers—typically professors, educational administrators, or other

professional researchers—tend to speak on behalf of teachers. All three of the articles described earlier in this

chapter had this feature. Persons other than teachers chose the research topics.

The information that emerges from this arrangement often still relates to teaching and learning, and may

contain useful insights for classroom work. But by definition, it is framed by people whose interests and

fundamental commitments may not be identical with classroom teachers. As a result, the studies are somewhat

more likely to attend to problems posed by academic disciplines or by educational administrators. Two of the

studies which we described earlier—the ones about moral development and about labels for disabilities—showed

366

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

this quality. Classroom teachers are concerned, of course, about both moral development and categorizing of

students. But if teachers had designed the two projects themselves, they might have re-framed both of them to focus

more explicitly on the challenges of classroom teaching. In studying moral beliefs, for example, teachers might have

focused more squarely on how to foster moral beliefs in their students. In studying inclusive education, they might

have focused more fully on the practical difficulties faced by teachers in assessing students’ learning disabilities

with validity.

The nature of action research

In view of these issues, a particularly important kind of investigation for teachers is action research (sometimes

also teacher research), an activity referring to systematic, intentional inquiry by teachers for the purpose of

improving their own practice (Stenhouse, 1985; Brydon-Miller, Greenwood, & Maguire, 2003; Russell, T. &

Loughran, J. 2005). Action research is not to be confused with research about teaching and learning, which are

investigations by professional researchers on topics of teachers, teaching, or learning.

Action research has several defining characteristics, in addition to being planned and conducted by teachers.

First, it originates in the problems and dilemmas of classroom practice, or in chronic problems with certain

students, materials, or activities. Second, its outcomes offer information focused on particular teachers and

classrooms, rather than about teachers in general or students in general. Although this feature might make action

research seem less useful as a source of advice or knowledge that is truly general, supporters argue that focusing on

specific learning contexts makes action research more credible or valid as a source of practical information and

ideas. It is, they argue, simply more attuned to the context of real classrooms (St. Clair, 2005). Third, while the

audience for action research can certainly include professors and educational administrators, the audience tends to

be other teachers (Fenstermacher, 1994; Ackerman & MacKenzie, 2007). Action research is therefore in an

especially strong position to provide “insider” perspectives on educational problems.

Action research in practice

Action research makes a number of assumptions as a result of its nature and purposes (Richardson, 1994;

Schmuck, 2006). To varying degrees, most such studies support some combination of these ideas:

• that teaching is itself really a form of research

• that action research, like teaching itself, requires substantial reflection

• that collaboration among teachers is crucial for making teacher research meaningful, and for the

improvement of teaching

• that teachers' knowledge of teaching has to be shared publicly, especially when gained systematically

through action research

To see how these features look in practice, look at several examples of action research studies.

Example #1: Focusing on motivating students

A number of years ago, Patricia Clifford and Sharon Friesen published an account of their effort to develop a

classroom program based on students' out-of-school interests and experiences (1993). Clifford and Friesen

were co-teachers in a double-sized classroom which deliberately included children from first, second and

Educational Psychology 367 A Global Text

Appendix C: The reflective practitioner

third grades. Their interest in students’ out-of-school experiences grew out of three more basic questions

about teaching, which they phrased like this:

• How can curriculum remain open to children's unique experiences and connect with the world they know

outside the school? Too often, the official school curriculum lacked meaning for children because it seemed

cut off from the rest of the world. The result was unmotivated students and poor learning.

• Why is imaginative experience the best starting place for planning? The teachers felt that imaginative

experiences—make-believe play, stories, poems—provided access to children's lives outside school—their

make-believe play, or their stories or poems. Perhaps somehow these could be connected to the goals of the

official curriculum.

• What happens when teachers break down the barriers between school knowledge and real knowledge? In

drawing on children's outside experiences, would children actually become more motivated or not? Would

they take over the program, and fail to learn the official curriculum goals?

To answer these questions, the teachers kept extensive diaries or journals for one entire school year. These

became the “data” for the research. In the journals, they described and reflected on their daily teaching

experiences. The teachers also talked with each other extensively about classroom events and their significance,

and the results of the conversations often entered the journals eventually during the research. In their journal,

for example, the teachers recorded an experience with students about ways of telling time. In preliminary

discussions the students became interested in how a sundial worked. So the teachers and students went outside,

where they created a human sundial, using the students themselves. The teachers' journal kept a chronicle of

these events, and noted the comments and questions which students developed as a result:

• If you stood in the same place for a whole day you would see your shadow change places because the earth

changes position.

• Why is my shadow longer than I am in the evening, but shorter at noon?

• Clouds can block the sun's rays so sundials won't work on rainy days.

• How did people start to tell time?

As the year evolved and observations accumulated and were recorded, the teachers gradually began to

answer their own three questions. They found, for example, that connecting the curriculum with children's

interests and motives was most effective when they could establish a personal bond with a child. They also

found that imaginative expression helped certain children to feel safe to explore ideas. They found that

blending school-based and personal knowledge caused children to learn much more than before—although

much of the additional knowledge was not part of an official curriculum. With these conclusions in mind, and

with numerous examples to support them, Clifford and Friesen published their study so that others could

share what they had learned about teaching, learning, and students.

The study by Clifford and Friesen is interesting in its own right, but for our purposes think for a moment about

their work as an example of action research. One of its features is that it formed part of the normal course of

teaching: the authors were simply more systematic about how they observed the students and recorded information

368

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

about classroom events. Another feature is that the research required conscious reflection over an extended time:

their journals and conversations contained not only descriptions of events, but also interpretations of the events. A

third feature is that the study involved collaboration: it was not just one teacher studying the major questions, but

two. Th fourth feature is that the teachers not only developed their results and conclusions for themselves, but also

shared them with others. These four qualities make the study by Clifford and Friesen a clear example of teacher

research. Note, though, that sometimes studies conducted by teachers may not show all of these features so clearly;

instead they may show some of the key features, but not all of them, as in the next two examples.

Example #2: Focusing on development

Since 1981, Vivian Paley has published a series of short books documenting and interpreting her observations

of young children in classrooms (1981, 1986, 1991, 1998, 2000, 2005). Paley was interested in how young

children develop or change over the long term, and in particular how the development looks from the point of

view of a classroom teacher. In one of these books, for example, she observed one child in particular, Mollie,

from the time she entered nursery school just after her third birthday until after the child turned four years

old (Paley, 1986). Her interest was not focused on curriculum, as Clifford and Friesen's had done, but on

Mollie as a growing human being; “the subject which I most wished to learn,” she wrote, “is children” (p. xiv).

Paley therefore wrote extended narrative (or story-like) observations about the whole range of activities of

this one child, and wove in periodic brief reflections on the observations. Because the observations took

story-like form, her books read a bit like novels: themes are sometimes simply suggested by the story line,

rather than stated explicitly. Using this approach, Paley demonstrated (but occasionally also stated) several

important developmental changes. In Mollie at Three (1988), for example, she describes examples of Mollie's

language development. At three years, the language was often disconnected from Mollie’s actions—she would

talk about one thing, but do another. By four, she was much more likely to tie language to her current

activities, and in this sense she more often “said what she meant”. A result of the change was that Mollie also

began understanding and following classroom rules as the year went on, because the language of rules

became more connected in her mind to the actions to which they referred.

Vivian Paley's book had some of the characteristics of action research—but with differences from Clifford and

Friesen’s. Like their research, Paley's “data” was based on her own teaching, while her teaching was influenced in

turn by her systematic observations. Like Clifford and Friesen’s, Paley’s research involved numerous reflections on

teaching, and it led to a public sharing of the reflections—in this case in the form of several small books. Unlike

Clifford and Friesen, though, Paley worked independently, without collaboration. Unlike Clifford and Friesen, she

deliberately integrated observation and interpretation as they might be integrated in a piece of fiction, so that the

resulting “story” often implied or showed its message without stating it in so may words. In this regard her work

had qualities of what some educators call arts-based research, which are studies that take advantage of an artistic

medium (in this case, narrative or story-like writing) to heighten readers' understanding and response to research

findings (Barone and Eisner, 2006). If you are studying the use of space in the classroom, for example, then

aesthetically organized visual depictions (photos, drawings) of the room may be more helpful and create more

understanding than verbal descriptions. If you are studying children's musical knowledge, on the other hand,

recordings of performances by the children may be more helpful and informative than discussions of performances.

Educational Psychology 369 A Global Text

Appendix C: The reflective practitioner

Example #3: Focusing on collaboration

In 1996, an example of action research was published that was intended simultaneously for classroom

teachers and for university researchers, and which focused on the challenges of collaboration among

educators (Ulichny & Schoener, 1996). A teacher (Wendy Schoener) and a university researcher (Polly

Ulichny) explored how, or even whether, teachers and university researchers could participate as equals in

the study of teaching. Wendy (the two used first names throughout when they published their experiences)

was a teacher of adults learning English as a Second Language (ESL); Polly was a specialist in multicultural

education and wanted to observe a teacher who was successful at reaching the ethnically diverse students

who normally study ESL. Polly therefore asked Wendy for permission to study her teaching for an extended

period of time—to visit her class, videotape it, interview her about it, and the like.

What followed is best described as an extended negotiation between teacher and professor for access to Wendy's

class, on the one hand, and for mutual respect for each other's work, on the other. In the published article, the

negotiations are described separately by each participant, in order to honor the differences in their concerns and

perspectives. Before, during, and after the observations, it was necessary for Polly and Wendy each to adjust

expectations of what the other person could do and was willing to do. As the authors put it, some things were “easy

to hear” from the other and some things were “hard to hear”. Wendy, as a teacher, found it easier to hear criticisms

of her teaching if they came from herself, rather than from the higher-status university professor, Polly. Polly, for

her part, found it easier to hear Wendy's comments if she matched Wendy's self-criticisms and evaluations with

some of her own experiences. Polly therefore made sure to tell Wendy about dilemmas and problems she

experienced in her own (university) teaching. Because they needed to adjust to hearing and talking with each other,

the two educators eventually focused less on Polly's original purpose—studying multicultural teaching—and more

on the problem of how teachers and university researchers might collaborate effectively.

Overall, this study qualifies as a piece of action research, though it is not fully focused on classroom teaching.

For example, the teachers did collaborate and reflect on their experiences, but not all of the reflection was about

teaching in classrooms. The rest was about the relationship between Wendy and Polly. While the problem selected

was originally about classroom teaching—Wendy’s—it did not originate with the classroom teacher (Wendy) or

concerns she had about her own classroom; instead it was chosen by the university researcher (Polly) and her

desire to study multicultural teaching. The researchers did share what they learned by publishing their observations

and ideas, but their published report speaks only partly to classroom teachers as such; in addition it speaks to

academic researchers and educators of future teachers.

By pointing out differences among these examples of action research, we do not mean to imply that one is

“better” than another. The point is simply to show how diverse studies by teachers can be and to appreciate their

differences. Whatever their specific features, classroom studies by teachers hold in common the commitment to

giving a voice to teachers as they reflect on problems and challenges intrinsic to classroom life. This goal can be

accomplished in more than one way: through journals and other record-keeping methods, through oral discussions

with colleagues, and through written reflections created either for themselves or for others concerned about

teaching and learning. Diversity among topics and methods in action research studies should not surprise us, in

fact, since classrooms are themselves so diverse.

370

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

The challenges of action research

Well and good, you may say. Action research offers teachers a way to hear each other, to learn from their own

and other's experience. But there are also a few cautions to keep in mind, both ethical and practical. Look briefly at

each of these areas.

Ethical cautions about action research

One caution is the possibility of conflict of interest between the roles of teaching and conducting action research

(Hammack, 1997). A teacher’s first priorities should be the welfare of his or her students: first and foremost, you

want students to learn, to be motivated, to feel accepted by their peers, and the like. A researcher’s first priorities,

however, are to the field or topic being studied. The two kinds of priorities may often overlap and support each

other. Vivian Paley’s observations of children in her classes, described earlier, not only supported her children’s

learning, but also her studies of the children.

But situations can also occur in which action research and teaching are less compatible, and can create ethical

dilemmas. The problems usually relate to one of three issues: privacy, informed consent, or freedom to participate.

Each of these becomes an issue only if the results of a research project are made public, either in a journal or book,

as with the examples we have given in this chapter, or simply by being described or shared outside the classroom.

(Sharing, you may recall, is one of the defining features of action research.) Look briefly at each of the issues.

Insuring privacy of the student

Teachers often learn information about students that the students or their families may not want publicized.

Suppose, for example, you have a student with an intellectual disability in your class, and you wish to study how the

student learns. Observing the student work on (and possibly struggle with) academic activities may be quite

consistent with a teacher’s responsibilities; after all, teachers normally should pay attention to their students’

academic efforts. But the student or his family may not want such observations publicized or even shared

informally with other parents or teachers. They may feel that doing so would risk stigmatizing the student publicly.

To respect the student’s privacy and still study his learning behavior, the teacher (alias the “action researcher”)

therefore needs to disguise the student’s identity whenever the research results are made public. In any written or

oral report, or even in any hallway conversation about the project, the teacher/researcher would use a pseudonym

for the student, and change other identifying information such as the physical description of the student or even the

student’s gender. There are limits, however, to how much can be disguised without changing essential information.

The teacher could not, for example, hide the fact of the intellectual disability without compromising the point of the

study; yet the intellectual disability might be unusual enough that it would effectively identify the student being

studied.

Gaining informed consent

Students may not understand what is being studied about them, or even realize that they are being studied at all,

unless the teacher/researcher makes an explicit effort to inform them about the action research and how she will

use the results from it. The same is true for the students’ parents; unless the teacher-researcher makes an effort to

contact parents, they simply will not know that their child’s activities are being observed or may eventually be made

public. Students’ ignorance is especially likely if the students are very young (kindergarten) or have intellectual or

reading difficulties, as in the example we described above. As an action researcher, therefore, a teacher is obliged to

Educational Psychology 371 A Global Text

Appendix C: The reflective practitioner

explain the nature of a research project clearly, either in a letter written in simple language or in a face-to-face

conversation, or both. Parents and students need to give clear indications that they actually understand what class

activities or materials will constitute data that could be made public. In most cases, indicating informed consent

means asking students’ parents signing a letter giving permission for the study. Sometimes, in addition, it is a good

idea to recheck with students or parents periodically as the project unfolds, to make sure that they still support

participation.

Insuring freedom to participate

When a student fails to participate in an ordinary class activity, most teachers consider it legitimate to insist on the

student’s participation—either by persuading, demanding, or (perhaps) tricking the student to join. Doing so is ethical

for teachers in their roles as teachers, because teachers are primarily responsible for insuring that students learn, and

students’ participation presumably facilitates learning. If a teacher designates an activity as part of an action research

project, however, and later shares the results with them, the teacher then also becomes partly responsible for how other

teachers use knowledge of the research study. (Remember: sharing results is intrinsically part of the research process.)

The resulting dual commitment means that “forcing” a student to participate in an action research activity can no

longer be justified solely as being for the student’s own educational good.

Much of the time, a simultaneous commitment to both teachers and students presents no real dilemma: what is

good for the action research project may also be good for the students. But not always. Suppose, for example, that a

teacher wants to do research about students’ beliefs about war and global conflict, and doing so requires that students

participate in numerous extended group discussions on this topic. Even though the group discussions might resemble a

social studies lesson and in this sense be generally acceptable as a class activity, some parents (or students) may object

because they take too much class time away from the normal curriculum topics. Yet the research project necessitates

giving it lots of discussion time in class. To respond ethically to this dilemma, therefore, the teacher may need to allow

students to opt out of the discussions if they or their parents choose. She may therefore need to find ways for them to

cover an alternate set of activities from the curriculum. (One way to do this, for example, is to hold the special group

discussions outside regular class times—though this obviously also increases the amount of work for both the teacher

and students.)

Practical issues about action research

Is action research practical? From one perspective the answer has to be “Of course not!” Action research is not

practical because it may take teachers’ time and effort which they could sometimes use in other ways. Keep in mind,

though, that a major part of the effort needed for action research involves the same sort of work—observing,

recording information, reflecting—that is needed for any teaching that is done well. A better way to assess

practicality may therefore be to recognize that teaching students always takes a lot of work, and to ask whether the

additional thoughtfulness brought on by action research will make the teaching more successful.

Looked at in this way, action research is indeed practical, though probably not equally so on every occasion. If

you choose to learn about the quality of conversational exchanges between yourself and students, for example, you

will need some way to record these dialogues, or at least to keep accurate, detailed notes on them. Recording the

dialogues may be practical and beneficial—or not, depending on your circumstances. On the other hand, if you

choose to study how and why certain students remain on the margins of your class socially, this problem too may be

372

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

practical as action research. Or it may not, depending on whether you can find a way to observe and reflect on

students' social interactions, or lack thereof. Much depends on your circumstances—on the attention you can afford

to give to your research problem while teaching, in relation to the benefits that solutions to the problems will bring

students later. In general any action research project may require certain choices about how to teach, though it

should not interfere with basic instructional goals or prevent coverage of an important curriculum. The main point

to remember is that action research is more than passive observation of students and classrooms; it also includes

educational interventions, efforts to stimulate students to new thinking and new responses. Those are features of

regular teaching; the difference is primarily in how systematically and reflectively you do them.

Benefiting from all kinds of research

Although we authors both feel a degree of sympathy for the nature and purposes of action research, we are not

trying to advocate for it at the expense of other forms of educational research or at the expense of simply reading

and understanding professional publications in general. The challenge for you, as a classroom teacher, is to find the

value in all forms of professional development, whether it be participation in a professional association, reading

general articles about research, or engaging in your own action research. To the extent that you draw on them all,

your ways of learning about teaching will be enriched. You will acquire more ways to understand classroom life,

while at the same time acquiring perspective on that life. You will learn ways to grasp the individuality of particular

students, but also to see what they need in common. You will have more ways to interpret your own experiences as

a professional teacher, but also be able to learn from the professional experience of others. Realizing these benefits

fully is a challenge, because the very diversity of classrooms renders problems about teaching and learning complex

and diverse as well. But you will also gain good, professional company in searching for better understanding of your

work—company that includes both educational researchers, other professional teachers, and of course your

students.

Chapter summary

The complexities of teaching require teachers to continue learning throughout their teaching careers. To

become a lifelong reflective practitioner, teachers can rely on colleagues as a resource, on professional associations

and their activities, and on professional publications related to educational issues and needs. Understanding the

latter, in turn, requires understanding the purposes of the published material—whether it is offering a general

framework, recommending desirable teaching practices, or advocating for a particular educational policy or need.

Interpreting published material also requires understanding the assumptions that authors make about readers’

prior knowledge and beliefs.

An important additional strategy for becoming a reflective practitioner is action research—studies of teaching

and learning designed and carried out by teachers in order to improve their own practice. By nature, action

research studies are highly relevant to classroom practice, but there are also cautions about it to keep in mind, both

ethically and practically.

Key termsaction research

assumptions about readers’ prior knowledge

informed consentinsuring privacy of students

insuring freedom to participate

professional associationspurposes of educational research

reflective practitioner

Educational Psychology 373 A Global Text

Appendix C: The reflective practitioner

reliability validity

On the Internet

This is the official website of the American Educational Research Association (or

AERA), a major “umbrella” professional association supporting educational research of all kinds. The home page

has links to over two dozen special interest groups (called “SIGs”), each specializing in some form of educational

research or practice. There is, among others, a special interest group called “teacher as researcher”, intended

primarily for educators involved in action research.

This is the website of the National Education Association (or NEA), another major

professional association of educators. The difference between this association and the American Educational

Research Association, however, is that the NEA focuses less on presenting research as such, and more on issues of

teaching practice. Like the AERA website, it includes articles on numerous topics that can be downloaded or read

online.

This is the website of the United States Office of Educational Research

and Improvement. It summarizes current research initiatives about education that are sponsored by the United

States Federal government, and includes links for finding information about the individual initiatives which it lists.

, These two websites belong to professional organizations

dedicated to action research. The first belongs to the Society for Community Research and Action, a division of the

American Psychological Association. It promotes and publishes action research in many professions, one of which

is education. The second website belongs to the Action Research Special Interest Group of the American

Educational Research Association; as you might suspect from its name, it focuses exclusively on action research by

educators.

References

Ackerman, R. & MacKenzie, S. (Eds.). (2007). Uncovering teacher leadership: Voices from the field.

Thousand Oaks, CA: Corwin Press.

Barone, T. & Eisner, E. (2006). Arts-based research in education. In J. Green, g. Camilli, & P. Elmore (Eds.),

Handbook of complementary methods in education research. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Bissex, G. (1980). GNYS AT WRK. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Brydon-Miller, M., Greenwood, D., Maguire, D. (2003). Why action research? Action Research, 1(1), 3-28.

Clifford, P. & Friesen, S. (1993). A curious plan: Managing on the twelfth. Harvard Educational Review,

63(3), 339-358.

Fenstermacher, G. (1994). The knower and the known: The nature of knowledge in research on teaching. In

L. Darling-Hammond (Ed.), Review of research in education, Volume 20, pp. 3-56. Washington, D.C.:

American Educational Research Association.

Hayes, D. (2006). Telling stories: Sustaining improvement in schools operating under adverse conditions.

Improving Schools, 9(3), 203-213.

374

This book is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License

Hittleman, D. & Simon, A. (2005). Interpreting educational research, 4th edition. Englewood Cliffs, NJ:

Prentice-Hall.

Jimenez, R., Garcia, G., & Pearson, D. (1995). Three children, two languages and strategic reading: Case

studies in bilingual/monolingual reading. American Educational Research Journal, 32(1), 67-98.

Johnson, M. (2004). A philosophy of second language acquisition. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

Lortie, D. (1975). Schoolteacher. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Loughran, J., Hamilton, M., LaBoskey, V., & Russell, T. (Eds.). (2004). International handbook of self-study

of teaching and teacher education practices. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer.

McDermott, R., Goldman, S., & Varenne, H. (2006). The cultural work of learning disabilities. Educational

Researcher, 35(6), 12-17.

Mills, G. (2006). Action research: A guide for the teacher researcher, 3rd edition. New York: Prentice Hall.

Paley, V. (1981). Wally’s stories. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Paley, V. (1988). Mollie is three. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Paley, V. (1991). The boy who would be a helicopter. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Paley, V. (1998). Kwanzaa and me. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Paley, V. (2000). The kindness of children. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Paley, V. (2006). A child’s work: The importance of fantasy play. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Ragland, B. (2007). Positioning the practitioner-researcher: Five ways of looking at practice. Action

Research, 4(2), 165-182.

Richardson, V. (1994). Conducting research in practice. Educational Researcher, 23(5), 5-10.

Russell, T. & Loughran, J. (2005). Self-study as a context for productive learning. Studying Teacher

Education, 1(2), 103-106.

Samaras, A. & Freese, A. (Eds.). (2006). Self-study of teaching practices. New York: Peter Lang.

Schmuck, R. (2006). Practical action research for change. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.

Schön, D. (1983). The reflective practitioner. New York: Basic Books.

Schön, D. (1987, April). Educating the reflective practitioner. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the

American Educational Research Association, Washington, D.C.

Saltzstein, H., Millery, M., Eisenberg, Z., Dias, M., & O’Brien, D. (1997). Moral heteronomy in context:

Interviewer influence in New York City and Recife, Brazil. In H. Saltzstein (Ed.), New directions in child

development: Culture as a context for moral development, pp. 37-50. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Seifert, Kelvin. (1981). Have we oversold mainstreaming? Journal of the Canadian Association for Young

Children, 4(2), 6-9.

Educational Psychology 375 A Global Text

Appendix C: The reflective practitioner

St. Clair, R. (2005). Similarity and superunknowns: An essay on the challenges of educational research.

Harvard Educational Review, 75(4), 435-453.

Stenhouse, L. (1985). Research as a basis for teaching. London, UK: Heinemann.

Stringer, E. (2007). Action research, 3rd edition. Thousand Oaks, CA: Corwin Publications.

Tidwell, D. & Fitzgerald, L. (Eds.). (2006). Self-study and diversity. Rotterdam, The Netherlands: Sense

Publishers.

Ulichny, P. & Schoener, W. (1996). Teacher-researcher collaboration from two perspectives. Harvard

Educational Review, 66(3), 496-524.

Zeichner, K. (2007). Accumulating knowledge across self-studies in teacher education. Journal of Teacher

Education, 58(1), 36-46.

376

Our customer support team is here to answer your questions. Ask us anything!